Brightness induction: Unequal spatial integration with increments and decrements

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Brightness induction: Unequal spatial integration with increments and decrements"

Transcription

1 Visual Neuroscience (2004), 21, Printed in the USA. Copyright 2004 Cambridge University Press $16.00 DOI: S Brightness induction: Unequal spatial integration with increments and decrements SANG WOOK HONG and STEVEN K. SHEVELL Departments of Psychology and Ophthalmology and Visual Science, University of Chicago, Chicago (Received September 7, 2003; Accepted January 15, 2004) Abstract Modern theories of brightness induction include an influence from regions that do not share a border with the target. This study tested whether the spatial range of neural integration is the same with incremental versus decremental contrast edges in relatively remote parts of the background. Using an asymmetric matching task, observers set the brightness of a comparison ring, within its own uniform surround, to match the brightness of a test ring within a contiguous surround and a noncontiguous background. The measurements showed that the area of integration depended on the incremental versus decremental contrast polarity at the edge between the surround and background. This implies that brightness induction from an inhomogeneous background must consider the polarity of contrast edges within the whole scene. Keywords: Brightness induction, Brightness contrast, Increments, Decrements, Contrast polarity, Edge integration Introduction A gray patch on a dim background appears brighter than a physically identical patch on an intense background. This phenomenon is called brightness induction. A classical account of induction from a homogeneous background holds that the physical contrast between the target and its contiguous surround determines brightness (Wallach, 1948). Induction from nonuniform backgrounds, however, cannot be explained by only the contrast between the target and its contiguous surround. The brightness of a target within an inhomogeneous background depends on neural signals from throughout the area of the visual stimulus (Reid & Shapley, 1988; Shevell et al., 1992; Zaidi et al., 1992; Blakeslee & McCourt, 1997; Rudd & Arrington, 2001). Some theoretical models incorporate multiscale neural filters whose outputs are scaled by contrast normalization (e.g. Blakeslee & McCourt, 1997, 1999). Other models extend Wallach s theory by positing integration of neural signals from each luminance edge in the scene, with the strength of influence of an edge decreasing as the distance from the target increases. The present study investigated how the spatial range of neural integration depended on the polarity of the contrast edge between the contiguous surround and a noncontiguous background. The measurements showed that the range of spatial integration depended on the Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Steven K. Shevell, Visual Science Laboratories, The University of Chicago, 940 East 57th Street, Chicago, IL 60637, USA. shevell@uchicago.edu contrast polarity. While these results are most directly related to edge integration models (e.g. Rudd & Arrington, 2001), they also reveal a new property of brightness induction that should be implicit in any account of brightness perception. Materials and methods Apparatus Achromatic stimuli were generated using a Macintosh G4 computer, and were presented on a calibrated Sony color display (GDM-F520). The cathode ray tube (CRT) display had pixel resolution and a refresh rate of 75 Hz noninterlaced. The achromatic stimuli were approximately metameric to equal energy white with Judd (1951) chromaticity coordinates x 0.33, y The red, green, and blue guns of the color CRT were linearized using 10-bit lookup tables. Stimuli The CRT simultaneously displayed the (left) comparison field and the (right) test field side-by-side, separated horizontally by a 2.3-deg dark region (Fig. 1). The width of both the test ring and the comparison ring was 8.6 min of arc. The left comparison ring was surrounded by a uniform field at a fixed luminance of 24 cd0m 2 (inner0outer diameter of deg). The test ring, at 16 cd0m 2, was surrounded by contiguous concentric rings on either side, which in turn were surrounded by the background field (outer 353

2 354 S.W. Hong and S.K. Shevell Fig. 1. A schematic of the stimuli for the matching task (at left) and the luminance profile of the test field (at right). The right-side test-ring luminance was fixed at 16 cd0m 2. The test s contiguous rings were varied in luminance (18, 20, 26, and 30 cd0m 2 ) as indicated by the double-pointed arrows in the luminance profile, and in width ( min of arc). The left-side comparison-ring luminance was adjusted by the observer to match the appearance of the test ring. The comparison-ring surround luminance was fixed at 24 cd0m 2. diameter of 4.0 deg). The contiguous rings were varied in luminance (18 30 cd0m 2 ) and width (extending between 4.3 and 34.4 min of arc either side of the test ring). The noncontiguous background was fixed at the same luminance as the comparison background (24 cd0m 2 ) in most experiments, and was varied in size to fill the part of the test field not occupied by the test and its contiguous rings. In Experiment 2, an additional condition was tested with the contiguous rings at 26 cd0m 2 and the noncontiguous background at 30 cd0m 2. Procedure Observers participated in several practice sessions until they made stable matches. A chin rest was used to maintain a stable head position. In each session, between six and 12 conditions were run in a random order. When a condition began, the test field was presented on the right side of the CRT and observers adapted to it for 2 min. The comparison field then appeared and observers made one match followed by 1 min of adaptation to the whole stimulus display. Observers were instructed to look at the presented stimuli during both adaptation periods. This trial match was excluded from data analysis. The average of five following matches for each condition was taken as one measurement for further data analysis. Each condition was repeated three times on different days. The method of adjustment was used to set the luminance of the comparison ring to match the brightness of the test ring. Observers controlled the luminance of the comparison ring by pressing separate buttons on a joystick. Observers were free to view the stimuli during each trial (no steady fixation). The initial value of the comparison ring in each trial was randomized. Observers Three observers participated in the study. They had normal color vision as tested with a Neitz anomaloscope. Author S.W.H. was knowledgeable about the experimental design and had prior experience making brightness matches. Observers Y.I.O. and L.Y. were naïve. Consent forms were completed in accordance with the policy of the University of Chicago s Institutional Review Board. Results Experiment 1: Spatial integration with remote increments and decrements The brightness matches showed that the influence of the noncontiguous background decreased as the distance between the edge of the background and the test increased, which is consistent with published brightness models (Reid & Shapley, 1988; Rudd & Arrington, 2001). The measurements, however, also showed a difference in the range of spatial integration for incremental and decremental edges. The matches are plotted in Fig. 2, in which the abscissa is the width of each contiguous ring, and the ordinate is the observer s setting of the comparison-ring luminance to match the brightness of the test ring. Recall that the test-ring luminance was fixed at 16 cd0m 2, and both backgrounds were 24 cd0m 2. The luminance of the contiguous rings is indicated by symbol shape (circles, 18 cd0m 2 ; squares, 20 cd0m 2 ; diamonds, 26 cd0m 2 ; and triangles, 30 cd0m 2 ). Each gray horizontal band shows the two-standarderror range of the measurement for which the contiguous-surround luminance filled the entire area of the test background (taken from each rightmost plotted point). The light gray band, therefore, shows a statistical range for the match to the brightness of the test ring with a single uniform surround. The smallest contiguous-ring width for which the brightness match was within the gray band was taken as an estimate of the extent of spatial integration, because further increasing contiguous-ring width did not significantly change the brightness of the test. Open arrows show this ring width for conditions in which the luminance of the contiguous rings was lower than the luminance of the noncontiguous background (pure decremental condition, test contiguous surround background). Black arrows show this ring width for the conditions in which the luminance of the contiguous rings was higher than the luminance of the noncontiguous background (test contiguous surround background). When the luminance of the contiguous rings was lower than the luminance of the noncontiguous background (circles and squares, Fig. 2), the brightness of the test ring increased as the width of the contiguous ring increased until it reached min of arc for

3 Spatial integration with increments and decrements 355 brightness models that include neural integration from each luminance edge in a visual scene, with a decrease in the strength of the induction as the distance from the target to the edge increased (Reid & Shapley, 1988; Rudd & Arrington, 2001). When the luminance of the contiguous rings was higher than the luminance of the noncontiguous background (diamonds and triangles, Fig. 2), so that the contrast polarity of the noncontiguous edge (increment) was different from that of the contiguous edge (decrement), the brightness of the test ring reached asymptote more abruptly than for the pure decremental condition. The brightness of the test ring reached asymptote when the width of the contiguous ring was min of arc (compare to min of arc) for observer S.W.H., min of arc (compare to 26 min of arc) for Y.I.O., and 4.3 min of arc (compare to 34 min of arc) for observer L.Y. In sum, when the contrast polarity between the contiguous rings and background was different than the polarity between the test and its contiguous rings, the brightness of the test was affected by inducing light over a shorter distance (compare black to open arrows in Fig. 2). Fig. 2. Brightness matches as a function of the width of the contiguous rings (horizontal axis). The vertical axis is the observer s luminance setting for the comparison ring to match the brightness of the test ring (fixed at 16 cd0m 2 ). The symbol shape indicates the luminance of the contiguous rings (circles, 18 cd0m 2 ; squares, 20 cd0m 2 ; diamonds, 26 cd0m 2 ; and triangles, 30 cd0m 2 ). The luminance of the noncontiguous background was fixed at 24 cd0m 2. Error bars, from three different days measurements, were usually smaller than the symbol size. Each gray horizontal band represents the two-standard-error range of the measurement when the contiguous rings filled the entire area of the test background (rightmost points). Arrows at the bottom of each panel are the widths at which the brightness of the test ring reached asymptote (see text). Each panel shows results for a different observer. Experiment 2: Remote-edge contrast polarity, not magnitude of contiguous contrast A smaller range of spatial integration was found with edges of opposite polarity but perhaps this range reflected higher contiguousedge contrast, due to increasing the luminance of the contiguous surround, rather than the difference in contrast polarity between the contiguous and noncontiguous edges. Measurements with an additional luminance condition tested this possibility. In this experiment, the luminance of the contiguous rings was 26 cd0m 2 and the luminance of the background was 30 cd0m 2. The contrast at the contiguous edge was the same as in one of the previous conditions (diamonds, Fig. 2) but the contrast polarity of the noncontiguous edge was now a decrement. In this experiment, therefore, the contrast polarity between the contiguous rings and background was the same as between the test and the contiguous rings. If the magnitude of contrast at the contiguous edge determined the range of spatial integration, then the brightness of the test ring should asymptote abruptly, as was found for the 26 cd0m 2 surround in the first experiment (diamonds and leftmost black arrows, Fig. 2). Measurements for two of the observers showed instead long-range spatial integration: 26 min of arc for observer S.W.H. and 22 min of arc for observer L.Y. (open arrows, Fig. 3). These distances are comparable to the pure decremental case in Experiment 1 (circles and squares, Fig. 2), and substantially larger than the spatial integration found with the 26 cd0m 2 surround in Experiment 1 (26 vs. 4.3 min of arc for S.W.H., 22 vs. 4.3 min of arc for L.Y.). For observer Y.I.O., the range of spatial integration by our two-standard-error criterion was smaller than for the other observers (8.6 min of arc), but still larger than in Experiment 1 for this observer (4.3 min of arc with the 26 cd0m 2 surround). The short range for Y.I.O. in Experiment 2 resulted primarily from the large standard error with the uniform surround (note the large width of the gray band for this observer in Fig. 3). In this case, the estimate of the region of spatial integration was not reliable. observer S.W.H., 26 min of arc for observer Y.I.O., and 34 min of arc for observer L.Y. In each of these luminance conditions, therefore, the brightness of the test was affected by inducing light up to about 0.5 deg away. These results were consistent with Discussion This study confirmed that the brightness of a test was affected by inducing light spatially separated from the test area. The effective range of spatial integration, however, depended on the contrast

4 356 S.W. Hong and S.K. Shevell Fig. 3. Brightness matches as a function of the width of 26 cd0m 2 contiguous rings. The test-ring luminance was 16 cd0m 2. The luminance of the noncontiguous background was 30 cd0m 2. The ordinate, error bars, gray horizontal bands, and arrows are as in Fig. 2. Each panel shows results for a different observer. polarity at the noncontiguous edges. This dependence cannot be explained by brightness induction models that depend on a single center-surround receptive field, or by edge-integration models that do not take account of the polarity of contrast. A center-surround receptive field is often used to explain brightness induction because of its antagonistic spatial organization (Jameson & Hurvich, 1961; Kingdom & Moulden, 1989; Moulden & Kingdom, 1990). Receptive-field organization can account for the changes in brightness of a test that result from increasing the luminance of the contiguous surround, and for some brightness changes here as the size of the surround increases and thus replaces background light of a different luminance. The asymptotic brightness of the test as surround width increases would reflect a surround size that completely fills the receptive field. A center-surround receptive field, however, cannot explain the difference in the range of spatial integration between the pure decremental condition (test contiguous surround background) and the mixed polarity condition (test contiguous surround background). The contrast polarity at the noncontiguous edge should not change spatial integration. Edge-integration models posit spatial integration of neural signals representing the contiguous edge between test and surround, and the noncontiguous edge between surround and background. Rudd and Arrington (2001) propose that integration of neural signals depends on the strength of signals from both the noncontiguous and contiguous edges. They report that neural signals from a noncontiguous edge can be partially blocked by the contiguous edge, and that blocking is more complete with higher contrast at the contiguous edge. This might explain the shorter range of spatial integration in Experiment 1 with the luminance of the contiguous surround higher than the luminance of the noncontiguous background (Fig. 2), in which case greater contrast at the contiguous edge could more fully block the signal from the noncontiguous edge. Blockage, however, is posited to occur only when the polarity of the luminance edge between the noncontiguous background and the contiguous surround is a decrement (Zemach & Rudd, 2002). They report that the influence from an incremental noncontiguous edge is not blocked. Therefore, the short range of spatial integration in the mixed polarity condition cannot be explained by blocking. The results here showed that the magnitude of contrast at the contiguous edge could not explain the range of spatial integration: with contrast at the contiguous edge held constant (26 cd0m 2 contiguous surround, 16 cd0m 2 test), the extent of spatial integration depended on the polarity of contrast at the more distant edge between surround and background, with more widespread spatial integration in the pure decremental case [a recent abstract by Zemach & Rudd (2003) reports a similar observation]. Note also that a greater absolute magnitude of contrast at the noncontiguous edge did not increase the range over which it influenced test brightness. Noncontiguous edge contrast was higher in Experiment 1 (30 cd0m 2 contiguous surround, 24 cd0m 2 background) than in Experiment 2 (26 cd0m 2 contiguous surround, 30 cd0m 2 background), yet spatial integration was greater in Experiment 2. In sum, brightness induction from an inhomogeneous background depends on the polarity of luminance edges in the visual scene. When the polarity of the noncontiguous edge and the contiguous edge are both decremental, the influence of the noncontiguous background varies over a distance of about 0.5 deg. When the polarity of the noncontiguous edge is an increment and the contiguous edge a decrement, however, the extent of spatial integration is substantially less. Acknowledgments This research was supported by PHS grant EY Publication was supported in part by an unrestricted grant to the Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Science from Research to Prevent Blindness.

5 Spatial integration with increments and decrements 357 References Blakeslee, B. & McCourt, M.E. (1997). Similar mechanisms underlie simultaneous brightness contrast and grating induction. Vision Research 37, Blakeslee, B. & McCourt, M.E. (1999). A multiscale spatial filtering account of the White effect, simultaneous brightness contrast and grating induction. Vision Research 39, Jameson, D. & Hurvich, L.M. (1961). Complexities of perceived brightness. Science 133, Judd, D.B. (1951). Report of the U.S. Secretariat Committee on Colorimetry and Artificial Daylight. In Proceedings of the Twelfth Session of the CIE, Stockholm, 1.1L. Paris: Bureau Central de CIE. Kingdom, F. & Moulden, B. (1989). Corner effect in induced hue: Evidence for chromatic band-pass filter. Spatial Vision 4, Moulden, B. & Kingdom, F. (1990). Light-dark asymmetries in the Craik Cornsweet O Brien illusion and a new model of brightness coding. Spatial Vision 5, Reid, R.C. & Shapley, R. (1988). Brightness induction by local contrast and the spatial dependence of assimilation. Vision Research 28, Rudd, M.E. & Arrington, K.F. (2001). Darkness filling-in: A neural model of darkness induction. Vision Research 41, Shevell, S.K., Holliday, I. & Whittle, P. (1992). Two separate neural mechanisms of brightness induction. Vision Research 32, Wallach, H. (1948). Brightness constancy and the nature of achromatic colors. Journal of Experimental Psychology 38, Zaidi, Q., Yoshimi, B., Flanigan, N. & Canova, A. (1992). Lateral interactions within color mechanisms in simultaneous induced contrast. Vision Research 32, Zemach, I.K. & Rudd, M.E. (2002). Blocking of achromatic color induction signals by borders of different contrast polarities [Abstract]. Journal of Vision 2, 106a. Zemach, I.K. & Rudd, M.E. (2003). Spatial decay of achromatic color induction differs for lightness and darkness induction processes [Abstract]. Journal of Vision 3, 421a.

Surround effects on the shape of the temporal contrast-sensitivity function

Surround effects on the shape of the temporal contrast-sensitivity function B. Spehar and Q. Zaidi Vol. 14, No. 9/September 1997/J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 2517 Surround effects on the shape of the temporal contrast-sensitivity function Branka Spehar School of Psychology, University of

More information

Vision Research 69 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage:

Vision Research 69 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage: Vision Research 69 (2012) 49 63 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Vision Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Simultaneous contrast and gamut relativity in achromatic

More information

Simultaneous lightness contrast with double increments

Simultaneous lightness contrast with double increments Perception, 2001, volume 30, pages 889 ^ 897 DOI:10.1068/p3103 Simultaneous lightness contrast with double increments Paola Bressan, Rossana Actis-Grosso Dipartimento di Psicologia Generale, Universita

More information

A Model of Local Adaptation supplementary information

A Model of Local Adaptation supplementary information A Model of Local Adaptation supplementary information Peter Vangorp Bangor University, UK & MPI Informatik, Germany Karol Myszkowski MPI Informatik, Germany Erich W. Graf University of Southampton, United

More information

Contrast gain control: a bilinear model for chromatic selectivity

Contrast gain control: a bilinear model for chromatic selectivity B. Singer and M. D Zmura Vol. 12, No. 4/April 1995/J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 667 Contrast gain control: a bilinear model for chromatic selectivity Benjamin Singer and Michael D Zmura Department of Cognitive Sciences

More information

Photometric, geometric, and perceptual factors in illumination-independent lightness constancy

Photometric, geometric, and perceptual factors in illumination-independent lightness constancy Perception & Psychophysics 2006, 68 (1), 102-113 Photometric, geometric, and perceptual factors in illumination-independent lightness constancy ALESSANDRO SORANZO University of Teesside, Middlesbrough,

More information

Photometric, geometric and perceptual factors in illumination-independent lightness constancy

Photometric, geometric and perceptual factors in illumination-independent lightness constancy Photometric, geometric and perceptual factors in illumination-independent lightness constancy SORANZO, Alessandro and AGOSTINI, Tiziano Available from Sheffield Hallam University Research Archive (SHURA)

More information

Remodelling colour contrast: implications for visual processing and colour representation

Remodelling colour contrast: implications for visual processing and colour representation Vision Research 39 (1999) 1329 1345 Remodelling colour contrast: implications for visual processing and colour representation A.J. Shepherd * Department of Psychology, Birkbeck College, Uni ersity of London,

More information

COLOR SCIENCE. Concepts and Methods, Quantitative Data and Formulae, 2nd Edition. John Wiley & Sons New York Chichester Brisbane Toronto Singapore

COLOR SCIENCE. Concepts and Methods, Quantitative Data and Formulae, 2nd Edition. John Wiley & Sons New York Chichester Brisbane Toronto Singapore COLOR SCIENCE Concepts and Methods, Quantitative Data and Formulae, 2nd Edition GÜNTER WYSZECKI National Research Council, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada W. S. STILES Richmond, Surrey, England t^- n M 1982 A

More information

Supplemental Information. Noise and Correlations. in Parallel Perceptual Decision Making. Thomas U. Otto and Pascal Mamassian

Supplemental Information. Noise and Correlations. in Parallel Perceptual Decision Making. Thomas U. Otto and Pascal Mamassian Current Biology, Volume 22 Supplemental Information Noise and Correlations in Parallel Perceptual Decision Making Thomas U. Otto and Pascal Mamassian Supplemental Inventory Figure S1 (related to Figure

More information

Motion influences the Effects of Systematic Chromatic Changes

Motion influences the Effects of Systematic Chromatic Changes Motion influences the Effects of Systematic Chromatic Changes Peggy Gérardin 1, Philippe Roud 1, Sabine Süsstrunk 1 and Kenneth Knoblauch 2,3 1 Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne (EPFL), Lausanne,

More information

Lightness, equivalent backgrounds, and anchoring

Lightness, equivalent backgrounds, and anchoring Perception & Psychophysics 1997, 59 (5), 643-654 Lightness, equivalent backgrounds, and anchoring NICOLA BRUNO and PAOLO BERNARDIS University of Trieste, Trieste, Italy and JAMES SCHIRILLO Wake Forest

More information

Lightness, brightness, and brightness contrast: 2. Reflectance variation

Lightness, brightness, and brightness contrast: 2. Reflectance variation Perception & Psychophysics 1993, 54 (4), 457-468 Lightness, brightness, and brightness contrast: 2. Reflectance variation LAWRENCE E. AREND Eye Research Institute, Boston, Massachusetts and BRANKA SPEHAR

More information

Color vision and colorimetry

Color vision and colorimetry Color vision and colorimetry Photoreceptor types Rods Scotopic vision (low illumination) Do not mediate color perception High density in the periphery to capture many quanta Low spatial resolution Many-to-one

More information

The Visual Perception of Images

The Visual Perception of Images C. A. Bouman: Digital Image Processing - January 8, 2018 1 The Visual Perception of Images In order to understand images you must understand how humans perceive visual stimulus. Objectives: Understand

More information

Color perception SINA 08/09

Color perception SINA 08/09 Color perception Color adds another dimension to visual perception Enhances our visual experience Increase contrast between objects of similar lightness Helps recognizing objects However, it is clear that

More information

Adaptation to a 'spatial-frequency doubled' stimulus

Adaptation to a 'spatial-frequency doubled' stimulus Perception, 1980, volume 9, pages 523-528 Adaptation to a 'spatial-frequency doubled' stimulus Peter Thompson^!, Brian J Murphy Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania

More information

Contrast Sensitivity

Contrast Sensitivity Contrast Sensitivity Performance in a vision based task, such as reading text, recognizing faces, or operating a vehicle, is limited by the overall quality of the image. Image quality depends on many factors,

More information

Color luminance relationships and the McCollough effect

Color luminance relationships and the McCollough effect Perception & Psychophysics 2000, 62 (4), 659-672 Color luminance relationships and the McCollough effect MICHAEL A. WEBSTER and GOKHAN MALKOC University of Nevada, Reno, Nevada The McCollough effect is

More information

SIGNAL DETECTION BY HUMAN OBSERVERS" Prof. J. A. Swets P. D. Donahue Louise Iarussi

SIGNAL DETECTION BY HUMAN OBSERVERS Prof. J. A. Swets P. D. Donahue Louise Iarussi XIV. SIGNAL DETECTION BY HUMAN OBSERVERS" Prof. J. A. Swets P. D. Donahue Louise Iarussi Prof. D. M. Green Susan A. Sewall A. COLOR VISION The "law of additivity of luminances" has long been regarded as

More information

Brightness of Different Hues Is a Single Psychophysical Ratio Scale of Intensity

Brightness of Different Hues Is a Single Psychophysical Ratio Scale of Intensity Brightness of Different Hues Is a Single Psychophysical Ratio Scale of Intensity RAGNAR STEINGRIMSSON, R DUNCAN LUCE, and LOUIS NARENS University of California, Irvine Recent studies based on testable

More information

Hue discrimination in peripheral vision under conditions of dark and light adaptation*

Hue discrimination in peripheral vision under conditions of dark and light adaptation* Perception & Psychophysics 1974, Vol. 15, No.3, 586-590 Hue discrimination in peripheral vision under conditions of dark and light adaptation* BRUCE A. AMBLERt University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon 97403

More information

Vision & Perception. Simple model: simple reflectance/illumination model. image: x(n 1,n 2 )=i(n 1,n 2 )r(n 1,n 2 ) 0 < r(n 1,n 2 ) < 1

Vision & Perception. Simple model: simple reflectance/illumination model. image: x(n 1,n 2 )=i(n 1,n 2 )r(n 1,n 2 ) 0 < r(n 1,n 2 ) < 1 Simple model: simple reflectance/illumination model Eye illumination source i(n 1,n 2 ) image: x(n 1,n 2 )=i(n 1,n 2 )r(n 1,n 2 ) reflectance term r(n 1,n 2 ) where 0 < i(n 1,n 2 ) < 0 < r(n 1,n 2 )

More information

The response criterion, the stimulus configuration, and the relationship between brightness contrast and brightness constancy*

The response criterion, the stimulus configuration, and the relationship between brightness contrast and brightness constancy* Perception & Psychophysics 1974, Vol. 16 (2), 347-354 The response criterion, the stimulus configuration, and the relationship between brightness contrast and brightness constancy* BARRY R. HAIMSON Southeastern

More information

The influence of lightness, and the crispening effect on the perceived contrast of textured images

The influence of lightness, and the crispening effect on the perceived contrast of textured images The influence of lightness, and the crispening effect on the perceived contrast of textured images David Kane, Marcelo Bertalmío; Dept. of Information and Communication Technologies, Universitat Pompeu

More information

BASIC VISUAL SCIENCE CORE

BASIC VISUAL SCIENCE CORE BASIC VISUAL SCIENCE CORE Absolute and Increment Thresholds Ronald S. Harwerth Fall, 2016 1. Psychophysics of Vision 2. Light and Dark Adaptation Michael Kalloniatis and Charles Luu 1 The Neuron Doctrine

More information

Effect of luminance on suprathreshold contrast perception

Effect of luminance on suprathreshold contrast perception 1352 J. Opt. Soc. Am. A/Vol. 8, No. 8/August 1991 Peli et al. Effect of luminance on suprathreshold contrast perception Eli Peli Physiological Optics nit, Eye Research Institute, Boston, Massachusetts

More information

The relationship between the physical intensity of a light stimulus

The relationship between the physical intensity of a light stimulus A probabilistic explanation of brightness scaling Surajit Nundy* and Dale Purves* *Department of Neurobiology, Duke University Medical Center, Durham, NC 27710; and Washington University School of Medicine,

More information

Perception of brightness. Perception of Brightness. Physical measures 1. Light Ray. Physical measures 2. Light Source

Perception of brightness. Perception of Brightness. Physical measures 1. Light Ray. Physical measures 2. Light Source Perception of Brightness The physics and psychophysics Perception of brightness psychophysics: relate psychological measures to physical ones perception of brightness is one of the simplest aspects of

More information

CLINICAL VISUAL OPTICS (OPTO 223) Weeks XII & XIII Dr Salwa Alsaleh

CLINICAL VISUAL OPTICS (OPTO 223) Weeks XII & XIII Dr Salwa Alsaleh CLINICAL VISUAL OPTICS (OPTO 223) Weeks XII & XIII Dr Salwa Alsaleh OUTLINE OF WEEKS XII & XIII Temporal resolution Temporal Summation. Broca-Sulzer effect. Critical flicker frequency (CFF). Temporal Contrast

More information

V2 Thin Stripes Contain Spatially Organized Representations of Achromatic Luminance Change

V2 Thin Stripes Contain Spatially Organized Representations of Achromatic Luminance Change Cerebral Cortex January 2007;17:116-129 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhj131 Advance Access publication February 8, 2006 V2 Thin Stripes Contain Spatially Organized Representations of Achromatic Luminance Change

More information

Predicting the Effects of Discomfort Glare on Visual Performance of Pilots during Nighttime Flying

Predicting the Effects of Discomfort Glare on Visual Performance of Pilots during Nighttime Flying International Conference on Mechatronics, Control and Electronic Engineering (MCE 2014) Predicting the Effects of Discomfort Glare on Visual Performance of Pilots during Nighttime Flying Yandan Lin*, Mengdi

More information

Introduction to Colorimetry

Introduction to Colorimetry IES NY Issues in Color Seminar February 26, 2011 Introduction to Colorimetry Jean Paul Freyssinier Lighting Research Center, Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute Troy, New York, U.S.A. sponsored by www.lrc.rpi.edu/programs/solidstate/assist

More information

The CIE 1997 Interim Colour Appearance Model (Simple Version), CIECAM97s. CIE TC1-34 April, 1998

The CIE 1997 Interim Colour Appearance Model (Simple Version), CIECAM97s. CIE TC1-34 April, 1998 The CIE 1997 Interim Colour Appearance Model (Simple Version), CIECAM97s CIE TC1-34 April, 1998 i The following members of TC1-34, Testing Colour Appearance Models, took part in the preparation of this

More information

Motion Perception 1. PSY305 Lecture 12 JV Stone

Motion Perception 1. PSY305 Lecture 12 JV Stone Motion Perception 1 PSY305 Lecture 12 JV Stone 1 Structure Human visual system as a band-pass filter. Neuronal motion detection, the Reichardt detector. The aperture problem. 2 The visual system is a temporal

More information

Ratio model serves suprathreshold color luminance discrimination

Ratio model serves suprathreshold color luminance discrimination Sankeralli et al. Vol. 19, No. 3/March 2002/J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 425 Ratio model serves suprathreshold color luminance discrimination Marcel J. Sankeralli and Kathy T. Mullen McGill Vision Research, Department

More information

What is a receptive field? Why a sensory neuron has such particular RF How a RF was developed?

What is a receptive field? Why a sensory neuron has such particular RF How a RF was developed? What is a receptive field? Why a sensory neuron has such particular RF How a RF was developed? x 1 x 2 x 3 y f w 1 w 2 w 3 T x y = f (wx i i T ) i y The receptive field of a receptor is simply

More information

L ight color influence on obstacle recognition in road lighting. 1. Introduction

L ight color influence on obstacle recognition in road lighting. 1. Introduction Computer Applications in Electrical Engineering L ight color influence on obstacle recognition in road lighting Małgorzata Górczewska, Sandra Mroczkowska, Przemysław Skrzypczak Poznań University of Technology

More information

An Experimental Approach to a Definition of the Mesopic Adaptation Field

An Experimental Approach to a Definition of the Mesopic Adaptation Field May 29 June 1, 2012 NRC Ottawa, Ontario CORM 2012 Annual Conference and Business Meeting An Experimental Approach to a Definition of the Mesopic Adaptation Field Tatsukiyo Uchida*, Yoshi Ohno** *Panasonic

More information

Visual Imaging and the Electronic Age Color Science Metamers & Chromaticity Diagrams. Lecture #6 September 7, 2017 Prof. Donald P.

Visual Imaging and the Electronic Age Color Science Metamers & Chromaticity Diagrams. Lecture #6 September 7, 2017 Prof. Donald P. Visual Imaging and the Electronic Age Color Science Metamers & Chromaticity Diagrams Lecture #6 September 7, 2017 Prof. Donald P. Greenberg Matching the Power Spectrum of the Test Lamp with a Full Set

More information

The standard deviation of luminance as a metric for contrast in random-dot images

The standard deviation of luminance as a metric for contrast in random-dot images Perception, 1990, volume 19, pages 79-101 The standard deviation of luminance as a metric for contrast in random-dot images Bernard MouldenIT, Fred Kingdom, Linda F Gatley Department of Psychology, University

More information

Chapter 5 Forward-backward asymmetry: the effect of body position and pointer shapes

Chapter 5 Forward-backward asymmetry: the effect of body position and pointer shapes Chapter 5 Forward-backward asymmetry: the effect of body position and pointer shapes Abstract: In previous experiments with an exocentric pointing task we found that deviations from veridical settings

More information

Velocity Thresholds in Car-Following at Night

Velocity Thresholds in Car-Following at Night ., Velocity Thresholds in Car-Following at Night E. P. TODOSEV, Space Technology Laboratories, and R. E. FENTON, Ohio State University CR- FOLLOWNG is defined as that phenomenon in which a vehicle follows

More information

A COMPARISON BETWEEN DGI OBTAINED THROUGH DIFFERENT METHODS AND PERCEIVED EVALUATION INDEX DDG

A COMPARISON BETWEEN DGI OBTAINED THROUGH DIFFERENT METHODS AND PERCEIVED EVALUATION INDEX DDG , Volume 8, Number 1, p.28-36, 2011 A COMPARISON BETWEEN DGI OBTAINED THROUGH DIFFERENT METHODS AND PERCEIVED EVALUATION INDEX DDG L. Bellia, G.F. Iuliano and G. Spada DETEC Università degli Studi di Napoli

More information

CS Color. Aditi Majumder, CS 112 Slide 1

CS Color. Aditi Majumder, CS 112 Slide 1 CS 112 - Color Aditi Majumder, CS 112 Slide 1 Visible Light Spectrum Aditi Majumder, CS 112 Slide 2 Color is due to.. Selective emission/reflection of different wavelengths by surfaces in the world Different

More information

How big is a Gabor patch, and why should we care?

How big is a Gabor patch, and why should we care? Fredericksen et al. Vol. 14, No. 1/January 1997/J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 1 How big is a Gabor patch, and why should we care? R. E. Fredericksen* McGill Vision Research, 687 Pine Avenue West (H4-14), Montreal,

More information

Limulus. The Neural Code. Response of Visual Neurons 9/21/2011

Limulus. The Neural Code. Response of Visual Neurons 9/21/2011 Crab cam (Barlow et al., 2001) self inhibition recurrent inhibition lateral inhibition - L16. Neural processing in Linear Systems: Temporal and Spatial Filtering C. D. Hopkins Sept. 21, 2011 The Neural

More information

APPARENT CONTRAST OF SPATIALLY AND TEMPORALLY SAMPLED GRATINGS

APPARENT CONTRAST OF SPATIALLY AND TEMPORALLY SAMPLED GRATINGS ACTA NEUROBIOL. DXP. 1988, 48: 283-293 APPARENT CONTRAST OF SPATIALLY AND TEMPORALLY SAMPLED GRATINGS T. RADIL, G. NYMAN and P. LAURLNEN Institute of Physiology, Czechoslovak Academy of Sciences Videiiska

More information

Screen-space processing Further Graphics

Screen-space processing Further Graphics Screen-space processing Rafał Mantiuk Computer Laboratory, University of Cambridge Cornell Box and tone-mapping Rendering Photograph 2 Real-world scenes are more challenging } The match could not be achieved

More information

An evaluation of different measures of color saturation

An evaluation of different measures of color saturation An evaluation of different measures of color saturation Florian Schiller, Matteo Valsecchi, & Karl R. Gegenfurtner Department of Psychology, Justus Liebig University Giessen, Giessen, Germany. Corresponding

More information

Measuring surface achromatic color: Toward a common measure for increments and decrements

Measuring surface achromatic color: Toward a common measure for increments and decrements Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers 2003, 35 (1), 70-81 Measuring surface achromatic color: Toward a common measure for increments and decrements GÁBOR JANDÓ University of Pécs, Pécs, Hungary

More information

Theory of colour measurement Contemporary wool dyeing and finishing

Theory of colour measurement Contemporary wool dyeing and finishing Theory of colour measurement Contemporary wool dyeing and finishing Dr Rex Brady Deakin University Colour measurement theory Topics 1. How we see colour 2. Generation of colours 3. Measurement of colour

More information

Spatial Vision: Primary Visual Cortex (Chapter 3, part 1)

Spatial Vision: Primary Visual Cortex (Chapter 3, part 1) Spatial Vision: Primary Visual Cortex (Chapter 3, part 1) Lecture 6 Jonathan Pillow Sensation & Perception (PSY 345 / NEU 325) Princeton University, Spring 2015 1 Chapter 2 remnants 2 Receptive field:

More information

Colour. The visible spectrum of light corresponds wavelengths roughly from 400 to 700 nm.

Colour. The visible spectrum of light corresponds wavelengths roughly from 400 to 700 nm. Colour The visible spectrum of light corresponds wavelengths roughly from 4 to 7 nm. The image above is not colour calibrated. But it offers a rough idea of the correspondence between colours and wavelengths.

More information

Is the Human Visual System Invariant to Translation and Scale?

Is the Human Visual System Invariant to Translation and Scale? The AAAI 207 Spring Symposium on Science of Intelligence: Computational Principles of Natural and Artificial Intelligence Technical Report SS-7-07 Is the Human Visual System Invariant to Translation and

More information

DEA 3500: HUMAN FACTORS: THE AMBIENT ENVIRONMENT (Fall 2017)

DEA 3500: HUMAN FACTORS: THE AMBIENT ENVIRONMENT (Fall 2017) DEA 3500: HUMAN FACTORS: THE AMBIENT ENVIRONMENT (Fall 2017) NAME: HOMEWORK 3 Due Date: November 21st in class (NOTE: You will need to consult the readings as well as your class notes to complete the homework)

More information

Colour Part One. Energy Density CPSC 553 P Wavelength 700 nm

Colour Part One. Energy Density CPSC 553 P Wavelength 700 nm Colour Part One Energy Density 400 Wavelength 700 nm CPSC 553 P 1 Human Perception An Active Organising Process Many illusions experiments from psychology Colour not just a matter of measuring wavelength

More information

Color Image Correction

Color Image Correction FLDE. p. 1/48 Color Image Correction Benasque, September 2007 Vicent Caselles Departament de Tecnologia Universitat Pompeu Fabra joint work with M.Bertalmío, R. Palma, E. Provenzi FLDE. p. 2/48 Overview

More information

Unmasking the mechanisms for Vernier acuity: evidence for a template model for Vernier acuity

Unmasking the mechanisms for Vernier acuity: evidence for a template model for Vernier acuity Vision Research 40 (2000) 951 972 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Unmasking the mechanisms for Vernier acuity: evidence for a template model for Vernier acuity Dennis M. Levi a, *, Stanley A. Klein b, Thom

More information

Color vision and colorimetry

Color vision and colorimetry Color vision and colorimetry Photoreceptor types Rods Scotopic vision (low illumination) Do not mediate color perception High density in the periphery to capture many quanta Low spatial resolution Many-to-one

More information

15 Grossberg Network 1

15 Grossberg Network 1 Grossberg Network Biological Motivation: Vision Bipolar Cell Amacrine Cell Ganglion Cell Optic Nerve Cone Light Lens Rod Horizontal Cell Retina Optic Nerve Fiber Eyeball and Retina Layers of Retina The

More information

Linear Systems Theory Handout

Linear Systems Theory Handout Linear Systems Theory Handout David Heeger, Stanford University Matteo Carandini, ETH/University of Zurich Characterizing the complete input-output properties of a system by exhaustive measurement is usually

More information

Brightness of Different Hues is a Single Psychophysical Ratio Scale of Intensity

Brightness of Different Hues is a Single Psychophysical Ratio Scale of Intensity Brightness of Different Hues is a Single Psychophysical Ratio Scale of Intensity Ragnar Steingrimsson, R. Duncan Luce, and Louis Narens Institute for Mathematical Behavioral Science University of California,

More information

Modelling the Rayleigh match

Modelling the Rayleigh match Visual Neuroscience (2004), 21, 477 482. Printed in the USA. Copyright 2004 Cambridge University Press 0952-5238004 $16.00 DOI: 10.10170S095252380421344X Modelling the Rayleigh match P.B.M. THOMAS and

More information

Opponent Color Spaces

Opponent Color Spaces C. A. Bouman: Digital Image Processing - January 8, 2018 1 Opponent Color Spaces Perception of color is usually not best represented in RGB. A better model of HVS is the so-call opponent color model Opponent

More information

Lateral organization & computation

Lateral organization & computation Lateral organization & computation review Population encoding & decoding lateral organization Efficient representations that reduce or exploit redundancy Fixation task 1rst order Retinotopic maps Log-polar

More information

A neuromorphic model for achromatic and chromatic surface representation of natural images

A neuromorphic model for achromatic and chromatic surface representation of natural images Neural Networks 17 (2004) 787 808 2004 Special Issue A neuromorphic model for achromatic and chromatic surface representation of natural images Simon Hong 1, Stephen Grossberg* Department of Cognitive

More information

Absence of a chromatic linear motion mechanism in human vision

Absence of a chromatic linear motion mechanism in human vision Vision Research 40 (2000) 1993 2010 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Absence of a chromatic linear motion mechanism in human vision Tatsuya Yoshizawa *, Kathy T. Mullen, Curtis L. Baker Jr. Department of

More information

Local luminance factors that determine the maximum disparity for seeing cyclopean surface shape

Local luminance factors that determine the maximum disparity for seeing cyclopean surface shape Vision Research 40 (2000) 1157 1165 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Local luminance factors that determine the maximum disparity for seeing cyclopean surface shape Lynn R. Ziegler *, Frederick A.A. Kingdom,

More information

Adaptation in the Neural Code of the Retina

Adaptation in the Neural Code of the Retina Adaptation in the Neural Code of the Retina Lens Retina Fovea Optic Nerve Optic Nerve Bottleneck Neurons Information Receptors: 108 95% Optic Nerve 106 5% After Polyak 1941 Visual Cortex ~1010 Mean Intensity

More information

matrix resulted. All values that were not one-zero were converted to standard normal

matrix resulted. All values that were not one-zero were converted to standard normal Appendix A A. General Statistics A.1 Morrisey s Incomplete Matrix Solution for Case V Because there was unanimous agreement for some pairs, a zero-one proportion matrix resulted. All values that were not

More information

Exploring the map reading process with eye movement analysis

Exploring the map reading process with eye movement analysis Exploring the map reading process with eye movement analysis Vassilios Krassanakis School of Rural & Surveying Engineering, National Technical University of Athens 9, Heroon Polytechniou Str., Zographos,

More information

AMP DISPLAY INC. SPECIFICATIONS. 4.3-in COLOR TFT MODULE AM480272QTZQW-00H

AMP DISPLAY INC. SPECIFICATIONS. 4.3-in COLOR TFT MODULE AM480272QTZQW-00H AMP DISPLAY INC. SPECIFICATIONS CUSTOMER: CUSTOMER PART NO. AMP DISPLAY PART NO. APPROVED BY: 4.3-in COLOR TFT MODULE AM480272QTZQW-00H DATE: APPROVED FOR SPECIFICATIONS APPROVED FOR SPECIFICATION AND

More information

--- . \ \ Visibility of Targets. ~~Lr ~ I\ ~ 4 negative Contrast ... WERNER ADRIAN. positive Contrast. Eye. -2 logo.

--- . \ \ Visibility of Targets. ~~Lr ~ I\ ~ 4 negative Contrast ... WERNER ADRIAN. positive Contrast. Eye. -2 logo. TRANSPORTATON RESEARCH RECORD 247 39 Visibility of Targets WERNER ADRAN Traffic safety is highly correlated to the amount of visual information that can be obtained from the road and its immediate environment.

More information

A Neuromorphic Model for Achromatic and Chromatic Surface Representation of Natural Images

A Neuromorphic Model for Achromatic and Chromatic Surface Representation of Natural Images Neuromorphic Model for chromatic and Chromatic Surface Representation of Natural Images Simon Hong + and Stephen Grossberg * Department of Cognitive and Neural Systems nd Center for daptive Systems Boston

More information

WEEK 5: BRIGHTNESS* PERCEPTION

WEEK 5: BRIGHTNESS* PERCEPTION CN530 S-2004 5-1 CN530 S-2004 5-2 WEEK 5: BRIGHTNESS* PERCEPTION 1) Brightness perception: Quantifying percepts 2) Isomorphistic and nonisomorphistic theories 3) Craik-O Brien-Cornsweet (COCE) effect 4)

More information

Temporal context calibrates interval timing

Temporal context calibrates interval timing Temporal context calibrates interval timing, Mehrdad Jazayeri & Michael N. Shadlen Helen Hay Whitney Foundation HHMI, NPRC, Department of Physiology and Biophysics, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington

More information

Shengfu LU Professor Brain Informatics Laboratory International WIC Institute Beijing University of Technology Beijing, China

Shengfu LU Professor Brain Informatics Laboratory International WIC Institute Beijing University of Technology Beijing, China NEW DEFINITIONS OF KINETIC VISUAL ACUITY AND KINETIC VISUAL FIELD AND THEIR AGING EFFECTS J. WU, S. LU, S. MIYAMOTO, Y. HAYASHI NEW DEFINITIONS OF KINETIC VISUAL ACUITY AND KINETIC VISUAL FIELD AND THEIR

More information

Illustrating Opponent Process, Color Evolution, and Color Blindness by the Decoding Model of Color Vision

Illustrating Opponent Process, Color Evolution, and Color Blindness by the Decoding Model of Color Vision Illustrating Opponent Process, Color Evolution, and Color Blindness by the Decoding Model of Color Vision LU, CHENGUANG 4643 North Park Drive, Port Alberni, BC, CANADA, V9Y 3H7 A symmetrical model of color

More information

DESIGNING CNN GENES. Received January 23, 2003; Revised April 2, 2003

DESIGNING CNN GENES. Received January 23, 2003; Revised April 2, 2003 Tutorials and Reviews International Journal of Bifurcation and Chaos, Vol. 13, No. 10 (2003 2739 2824 c World Scientific Publishing Company DESIGNING CNN GENES MAKOTO ITOH Department of Information and

More information

Misperceptions of Angular Velocities Influence the Perception of Rigidity in the Kinetic Depth Effect

Misperceptions of Angular Velocities Influence the Perception of Rigidity in the Kinetic Depth Effect Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 1997, Vol. 23, No. 4, 1111-1129 Copyright 1997 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/97/S3.00 Misperceptions of

More information

Tick Talk. Introduction

Tick Talk. Introduction Tick Talk Douglas J. Davidson (dvdsn@casper.beckman.uiuc.edu) Kathryn Bock (kbock@s.psych.uiuc.edu) David E. Irwin (dirwin@s.psych.uiuc.edu) Beckman Institute University of Illinois 405 N. Mathews Urbana,

More information

DEA 350: HUMAN FACTORS: THE AMBIENT ENVIRONMENT (Spring 2008)

DEA 350: HUMAN FACTORS: THE AMBIENT ENVIRONMENT (Spring 2008) DEA 350: HUMAN FACTORS: THE AMBIENT ENVIRONMENT (Spring 2008) NAME: HOMEWORK II Due Date: 24 th April in class (NOTE: You will need to consult the readings as well as your class notes to complete the homework.

More information

Spatial pattern summation is phase-insensitive in the fovea but not in the periphery

Spatial pattern summation is phase-insensitive in the fovea but not in the periphery Spatial pattern summation is phase-insensitive in the fovea but not in the periphery CHIEN-CHUNG CHEN * and CHRISTOPHER W. TYLER Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute, 2318 Fillmore Street, San Francisco,

More information

Comparison of receptive fields to polar and Cartesian stimuli computed with two kinds of models

Comparison of receptive fields to polar and Cartesian stimuli computed with two kinds of models Supplemental Material Comparison of receptive fields to polar and Cartesian stimuli computed with two kinds of models Motivation The purpose of this analysis is to verify that context dependent changes

More information

SEASONAL AND ANNUAL TRENDS OF AUSTRALIAN MINIMUM/MAXIMUM DAILY TEMPERATURES DURING

SEASONAL AND ANNUAL TRENDS OF AUSTRALIAN MINIMUM/MAXIMUM DAILY TEMPERATURES DURING SEASONAL AND ANNUAL TRENDS OF AUSTRALIAN MINIMUM/MAXIMUM DAILY TEMPERATURES DURING 1856-2014 W. A. van Wijngaarden* and A. Mouraviev Physics Department, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada 1. INTRODUCTION

More information

Digital Image Processing

Digital Image Processing Digital Image Processing 16 November 2006 Dr. ir. Aleksandra Pizurica Prof. Dr. Ir. Wilfried Philips Aleksandra.Pizurica @telin.ugent.be Tel: 09/264.3415 UNIVERSITEIT GENT Telecommunicatie en Informatieverwerking

More information

Detectability measures the difference between the means of the noisy ``signal'' distribution and the ``noise-only'' distribution.

Detectability measures the difference between the means of the noisy ``signal'' distribution and the ``noise-only'' distribution. VISUAL PERCEPTION Here is the Visual Perception Video Outline. Visual Perception is another subject where a conceptual understanding of the big picture is critical. When there are equations, pay particular

More information

Brightness matching by peripheral vision of motion

Brightness matching by peripheral vision of motion Brightness matching by peripheral vision of motion Gerald Klaus Kranewitter Mathematisches Institut der Universität Innsbruck, Technikerstrasse 25, A-6020 Innsbruck, Austria Roman Rudolf Liedl Mathematisches

More information

Color and compositing

Color and compositing Color and compositing 2D Computer Graphics: Diego Nehab Summer 208. Radiometry Measurement of radiant energy in terms of absolute power Wave vs. particle Wavelength (λ), frequency (ν = c λ ), and amplitude

More information

Stimulus Features that Determine the Visual Location of a Bright Bar

Stimulus Features that Determine the Visual Location of a Bright Bar Stimulus Features that Determine the Visual Location of a Bright Bar R. J. Watr,* M. J. Morgan,* and R. M. Wordf A modification to the standard vernier target that has a detrimental effect on acuity is

More information

DEM C TTH-PW-N

DEM C TTH-PW-N Display Elektronik GmbH LCD MODULE DEM 320240C TTH-PW-N 3,5 TFT (Low Reflective) Product Specification Ver.: 0 03.12.2010 Version: 0 PAGE: 1 Revise Records Rev. Date Contents Written Approved 0 03.12.2010

More information

12.2. The Earth Moon System KNOW? The Phases of the Moon. Did You

12.2. The Earth Moon System KNOW? The Phases of the Moon. Did You 12.2 The Earth Moon System Did You KNOW? The Moon is Earth s closest neighbour. It is highly influential in our lives because it causes the oceans tides. The Moon is also responsible for eclipses. waxing

More information

UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA. Santa Barbara. "The Role of Illumination Perception in Color Constancy" Doctorate of Philosophy.

UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA. Santa Barbara. The Role of Illumination Perception in Color Constancy Doctorate of Philosophy. UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA Santa Barbara "The Role of Illumination Perception in Color Constancy" This Dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctorate of Philosophy

More information

The idiosyncratic nature of confidence

The idiosyncratic nature of confidence SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION Articles DOI: 10.1038/s41562-017-0215-1 In the format provided by the authors and unedited. The idiosyncratic nature of confidence 1,2 Joaquin Navajas *, Chandni Hindocha 1,3,

More information

Color images C1 C2 C3

Color images C1 C2 C3 Color imaging Color images C1 C2 C3 Each colored pixel corresponds to a vector of three values {C1,C2,C3} The characteristics of the components depend on the chosen colorspace (RGB, YUV, CIELab,..) Digital

More information

EXTENDING THE CIE 2006 MODEL

EXTENDING THE CIE 2006 MODEL MARK D. FAIRCHILD & YUTA ASANO CUSTOM COLOR MATCHING FUNCTIONS: EXTENDING THE CIE 2006 MODEL TWO BIG QUESTIONS WHAT DO YOU SEE? DO YOU SEE WHAT I SEE? CIE COLORIMETRY 1931 & 1964, 2- & 10-deg. 2 1.5 Tristimulus

More information

INTRODUCTION TO THE TELESCOPE

INTRODUCTION TO THE TELESCOPE AST 113/114 Fall 2014 / Spring 2016 NAME: INTRODUCTION TO THE TELESCOPE What will you learn in this Lab? For a few of the labs this semester, you will be using an 8-inch Celestron telescope to take observations.

More information

Appearance of colored patterns: separability. Allen B. Poirson* and Brian A. Wandell

Appearance of colored patterns: separability. Allen B. Poirson* and Brian A. Wandell 2458 J. Opt. Soc. Am. A/Vol. 1, No. 12/December 1993 Appearance of colored patterns: separability pattern-color Allen B. Poirson* and Brian A. Wandell Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford,

More information

The influence function for visual interpolation

The influence function for visual interpolation The influence function for visual interpolation Alex K. Hon', Laurence T. Ma1oney, Michael S. Landy adepartment of Psychology, NYU bdepartment of Neural Science, NYU Washington Square, New York, NY 10003,

More information