Evaluating the Cognitive Adequacy of the DLine-Region Calculus 1

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Evaluating the Cognitive Adequacy of the DLine-Region Calculus 1"

Transcription

1 Klippel, A., Yang, J., Li, R., & Wallgrün, J. O. (in print, 2012). Evaluating the cognitive adequacy of the DLine- Region Calculus. In SDH Evaluating the Cognitive Adequacy of the DLine-Region Calculus 1 Alexander Klippel, Jinlong Yang, Rui Li, Jan Oliver Wallgrün Department of Geography, GeoVISTA Center, Pennsylvania State University [klippel,jinlong,rui.li,wallgrun]@psu.edu Abstract. Qualitative spatio-temporal calculi play a crucial role in modeling, representing, and reasoning about geospatial dynamics such as the movement of agents or geographic entities. They are ubiquitous in ontological modeling, information retrieval, they play a central part at the human-machine interface, and are critical to process data collected from geosensor networks. What is common to all these application areas is the search for a mechanism to transform data into knowledge borrowing heavily from strategies of (human) cognitive information processing. Astonishingly, there is paucity in actual behavioral evaluations on whether the suggested calculi are indeed cognitively adequate. While the assumption seems to be made that qualitative equals cognitive, a more differentiated view is needed. This paper is filling the void by the first (to the best of our knowledge) behavioral assessment of the DLine-Region calculus using actual dynamic stimuli. These assessments are crucial as the few experiments that exist have clearly demonstrated that topological relations form conceptual groups (clusters), a fact that seems to be highly likely for the 26 DLine-Region relations as well. Our results show which topological relations form (cognitive) conceptual clusters. 1 Introduction The meaningful processing of spatio-temporal data is a challenging and recent research topic. The tremendous amount of data that is becoming increasingly available has spurred multidisciplinary efforts to process and analyze data telling various stories of the dynamic earth (e.g., Adrienko, Adrienko, Dykes, Fabrikant, & Wachowicz, 2008; Yuan & Hornsby, 2008). One important development are qualitative spatiotemporal representation and reasoning (short: QSTR) approaches (e.g., Kurata & Egenhofer, 2009; Muller, 2002; Sridhar, Cohn, & Hogg, 2011; van de Weghe, Billen, Kuijpers, & Bogaert, 2008). The reason for the popularity of qualitative formalisms can be summarized by a quote from Galton s (2000) seminal book on qualitative change: The divisions of qualitative space correspond to salient discontinuities in our apprehension of quantitative space. 1 This research is funded through the National Science Foundations (# ). adfa, p. 1, Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2011

2 The importance of this statement is difficult to overestimate. If this statement is true, then we have found by using QSTR a way to bridge the gap between cognitive (semantic) information processing and requirements of formal systems fundamental to modern information technologies. From our usage of the word if the reader may derive that it is, unfortunately, not that easy. There are numerous suggestions for QSTR that all identify sometimes contradictory divisions of quantitative space. Additionally, while often a claim is made that qualitative approaches bear some inherent cognitive/commonsense aspects of how spatio-temporal information is processed, there is a paucity of actual behavioral evaluations that would back up the claim of capturing cognitive aspects of spatio-temporal information processing using QSTR. Hence, we do need an experimental framework that allows for effectively and efficiently assessing QSTR approaches cognitively. This article details an experiment assessing the DLine-Region calculus (Kurata, 2008) while at the same time showing general ways of assessing the cognitive adequacy of QSTR approaches that are built on jointly exhaustive and mutually exclusive relations. The remainder of this article is structured as follows: First, we provide some background information on existing behavioral studies as well as the DLine-Region calculus. Second, we report on the conducted experiment evaluating the divisions (topological equivalence classes) that are inherent to the DLine-Region calculus. The results show that we need to superimpose a hierarchy onto the 26 primitive relations to reflect human conceptualizations of movement patterns captured by this calculus. We discuss in the outlook some strategies how behavioral results can be transformed into weights for conceptual neighborhood graphs (CNGs) that will cognitively adjust their use in areas such as information retrieval, ontology engineering, or the geo-spatial semantic web. 2 Background Given the space limitations we will focus on some essential behavioral studies and a brief introduction to the DLine-Region calculus. Of particular importance to the topic of this article are the experiments by Mark and Egenhofer (e.g., Mark & Egenhofer, 1995; Mark & Egenhofer, 1994a; Shariff, Egenhofer, & Mark, 1998) on topologically characterized relations between a line and a region. In their experiments, the line had no direction and they used static images (rather than animations, see Section 3). They focused on both cognitive conceptualization processes as well as the spatial semantics of linguistic expressions. In some of their experiments they employed a grouping paradigm (Mark & Egenhofer, 1994b), similar to the approach taken in the experiment we will present later on. Their findings crystalized in the famous statement that topology matters and metric refines (Egenhofer and Mark, 1995). Not as widely discussed but equally important is their finding that the 19 relations between a line and a region are cognitively not primitive relations. In other words, the 19 relations form conceptual groups (clusters) with larger within group similarity as well as larger between group dissimilarity. This aspect has also been addressed in formal research papers and we will come back to this aspect throughout the paper.

3 Behavioral research on topology and qualitative calculi addressing actual movement patterns is rare. There are only a few studies that use dynamic stimuli in their experiments. Noteworthy is the research by Lu and collaborators (2009) in which the authors evaluate Allen s (1983) temporal interval calculus. The results, in a nutshell, indicate that certain relations can be considered forming clusters, mimicking the findings of Mark and Egenhofer (1994b). Specifically, before and meet form one cluster and all other relations of Allen s calculus form a second one. Our own research (see Klippel et al. in press, for an overview) so far has addressed topological movement patterns that can be modeled as changing relations between two spatially extended entities. Evaluating RCC-8 (Randell, Cui, & Cohn, 1992) and Egenhofer s intersection models (Egenhofer & Franzosa, 1991) that can be used to characterize these movement patterns, we found that topological relations form conceptual groups (clusters) and that domain semantics is an important contextual factor (Klippel, in press). To the best of our knowledge, there are no other published behavioral evaluations of dynamic movement patterns characterized by qualitative calculi 2. Back to the DLine-Region calculus; Kurata and Egenhofer (2007) extended the original 9-intersection models proposed by Egenhofer and collaborators (Egenhofer & Franzosa, 1991) such that the direction of a line (in relation to a region) can be captured, too. The original 9-intersection model details the relation between two spatial objects A and B by creating a 3 x 3 matrix that details, from a point-set topological perspective, the relation (intersection) between three topological parts of A and B: interior, boundary, and exterior. For the case of a directed line, Kurata and Egenhofer introduced a finer distinction of the boundary of the line separately representing the start and the end of a line. This finer distinction results in an extra row in the 9- intersection matrix which then, consequently, is referred to as the 9+-Intersection Model (Kurata, 2008). In 2D, 19 relations are possible between a line and a region but there are 26 topologically equivalent relations possible in case the line is directed. Visually these relations can be organized into a conceptual neighborhood graph (see Figure 1, see also Egenhofer & Al-Taha, 1992; Freksa, 1992; Randell & Cohn, 1989). Two relations, and, are conceptual neighbors if it is possible for to hold over a tuple of objects at a certain point in time, and for to hold over the tuple at a later time, with no other (third) mutually exclusive relation holding in between (Cohn, 2008; Freksa, 1992). A neighborhood graph has one node for each relation, and an edge between two nodes if the corresponding relations are neighbors. The important aspect to keep in mind, which will tremendously add to the transformative nature of this paper, is that virtually every calculus with jointly exhaustive and pairwise disjoint (JEPD) relations (such as RCC and the intersection models) has a conceptual neighborhood graph (Cohn & Renz, 2008), and that the methods applied here will be universally applicable amongst these calculi to improve their cognitive adequacy. 2 There are numerous proposals on general cognitive aspects of spatial dynamics (e.g., Mennis, Peuquet, & Qian, 2000), cognitive studies on events and movement (Shipley & Zacks, 2008) and there are also some unpublished studies.

4 Fig. 1. Conceptual neighborhood graph for the 26 DLine-Region relations (see Kurata & Egenhofer, 2009) 3 Experiment We have extended an experimental framework (Knauff, Rauh, & Renz, 1997; Mark & Egenhofer, 1994b) that is tailored to requirements in the spatial sciencess in that it allows for evaluating qualitative spatial calculi built on JEPD relations. We are using a grouping paradigm, which is classically employed to reveal cognitive conceptualiza- The tions, and combining it with animations based on topological equivalence classes. equivalence classes for the purpose of this article are the ones specified in the DLine- critical Region calculus, which we briefly introduced in the previous section. The questionss that this experiment is answering are: Are the equivalencee classes identified by the DLine-Region calculus salient discon- tinuities in our apprehension of (qualitative) space?

5 On the basis of previous research it is fair to assume that not all 26 relations are equally salient but that they will form groups. If so, which relations are considered more similar to each other than others? Participants. 26 Penn State undergraduate students participated for course credit. Average age was 22 (9 female). Design and materials. 78 randomized animated icons depicting 26 DLine-Region relations (three per equivalence class) were constructed in Adobe Flash CS4. Each icon was 120 by 120 pixels in size. Given that participants seee actual animations, we restricted the movement in that it, generally speaking, only went from left to right. Figure 2 shows an example of one DLine-Regionn relation (d, see Figure 1). A random start point was selected in the exterior of the startt region. The movement of the black dot starts from the left exterior, fully crosses the interior, and then ends at a randomly selected point in the right exterior (the end region). To give another example, for rela- region, tion (m 1 ) the dot startss from a randomized location in the interior of the start moves in the interior, and ends somewhere on the boundary in the end region. Fig. 2. Icon shows the case of DLine-Region relation (d), where the dot starts from a randomrandomized ized location in the exterior (left), fully crosses the interior, and then ends at a location in the exterior (right). In the construction of all icons, particular attention was paid to two specific aspects: 1) that the starting and ending relations were perceptually clear; and 2) that the speed of the dot was constant in and among all icons. The speed of the dot movement was kept constant by maintaining the same ratio between the path length and the number of frames. At the end of each dot movement, the dot paused to represent the ending relation beforee the movement was repeated. The construction of icons in- ambigu- volved manual inspection by experts to remove and replace icons that weree ous due to the randomized locations of start- and end-points. The final 78 icons did not convey DLine-Region relations that are ambiguous. Procedure. The experiment took place in a GIS lab and was set up as a group ex- (Optiplex 755, 24 widescreen LCD monitors). View blocks ensured that participants performed this task individually. Participants performed a free classification (category periment. The lab seats up to 16 participants at the same time at Dell workstations construction) task as well as a linguistic labeling task. Our custom-made grouping software, CatScan, allows for presenting dynamic stimuli and administers the complete experiment (see Figure 3). All 78 animations

6 showing the 26 DLine-Region relations were initially displayed on the left side of the screen in random order. The right side was empty and participants were required to create alll groups. Animated icons can be placed into groups by simple drag-and-drop operations; they can be placed into groups, out of groups, or moved between groups. In case a group is deleted, all icons are placed back on the left side. The main group- par- ing task was preceded by a warm up grouping task (sorting animals) to acquaint ticipants with the interface, the grouping environment, and general idea of a free clas- were sification task. After finishing the task (no time limit was given), participants presented with the groups they created and provided linguistic labels for these groups: a short label of no more than 5 words (e.g., inside out in) and a longer description (e.g., found the dots that started inside the circle then went out then back in). Fig. 3. The screenshots of the experiment interface. The top screenshot shows the initial screen before participants started to group. The bottom screenshot shows an ongoing experiment with groups created by the participant.

7 4 Results On average, participants created 11.3 groups (min: 3; max 39, statistically an outlier) and it took them 22 min on average to finish the grouping task. The average number of groups is, as expected, below the 26 formally defined topological equivalence classes. This result is in accordance with several findings (Klippel et al. in press; Mark & Egenhofer, 1994b) that topological equivalence classes defined by a number of calculi are forming conceptual clusters, whose formally defined granularity is often not the granularity identified by human participants. The grouping behavior of participants itself is captured in individual similarity matrices which encode two icons being placed into the same group as 1 and two icons not being placed into the same group as 0. By summing over all individual similarity matrices, an overall similarity matrix (OSM) can be created. Thereby maximum similarity is assigned to those pairs of animations that are always placed together into the same group and minimum similarity is assigned to those pairs of animations that are never placed into the same group. The maximum similarity corresponds to the number of participants (here: N = 26), the minimum similarity is 0. This data can be analyzed using a number of techniques such as cluster analysis, multidimensional scaling, or simple heat maps that allow for visualizing raw similarities. Figure 4 shows a combination of a cluster analysis (Ward s method) and a heat map. Additional annotations in Figure 4 (dashed boxes) show a synthesis of comparing different cluster methods as a means to validate clustering results (Kos & Psenicka, 2000). The two dendrograms (top and left part) are identical and show the result of the cluster analysis; icon names are placed on the bottom and right part of the heat map and consist of the relation name plus a number from 1-3. The heat map visualizes raw similarities such that higher similarities are displayed in darker gray and lower similarities are displayed in lighter gray. The combination of heat map and cluster analysis allows for a better interpretation of the grouping behavior as the clustering structure revealed by the dendrograms can be directly related to actual grouping behavior. A first observation is that topological equivalence is a strong predictor for the similarity ratings; this is indicated by the fact that all three instances of all topological relations are in neighboring columns/rows, that is, they are grouped together most frequently compared to all other grouping possibilities (the dark gray cells along the diagonal, top-left to bottom-right). To this end, these results are largely consistent with data from other experiments (Klippel et al. in press; Mark & Egenhofer, 1994b) that topology at the base level is a strong grouping criterion. Equally interesting is, however, the overall grouping structure, and the combination of heat map and dendrograms allows for a deeper interpretation. Figure 4 reveals on a coarse level a three cluster solution using Ward s method that has, however, to be somewhat modified if three clustering methods (Ward s, average linkage, complete linkage) are compared. The result of this synthesis is indicated by the dashed-line-boxes. To additionally visualize the solution indicated by the combined analysis (i.e. the dashed-line-boxes) we have also depicted these results in

8 Figure 5 using a merged CNG (compare Figure 1) and using dashed lines reflecting the cluster solution shown in Figure 4. The synthesis of all three clustering methods allows for identifying two large clusters that are identical across all three clustering methods and one cluster that requires refinement. The two large clusters identical across methods are: cluster 1 with DLine- Region relations a, b, m 1, m 2, n 1, n 2, g, h, and l; cluster 2 with DLine-Region relations c, o 1, o 2, i, j, and k; and cluster 3, which has to be analyzed in more detail, contains on the coarsest granularity the following relations: p 1, p 2, q 1, q 2, r 1, r 2, s 1, s 2, d, e, f. Besides the fact that topological equivalence is at the core of the similarity ratings, it is worthwhile to note that the clusters in general also reflect topologically induced similarities. In other words, the general clustering structure does not violate topological similarity in the sense that the groups formed by participants are connected subgraphs in the DLine-Region CNG (see Figures 1 and 5). However, the granularity required by a formal topological characterization does not seem to be reflected in the grouping behavior of the participants. Participants focused on more abstract characteristics of the movement patterns. In the following we summarize these characteristics: Cluster 1: No part of the movement is taking place outside the region, meaning that the intersection of any component of the DLine with the exterior of the region is empty. The movement starts either on the border of the region or inside the region. The movement ends either inside or on the border. Parts of the movement can take place along the border. Cluster 2: Part of the movement has to take place outside the region but it is neither the beginning nor the ending. Start and end of the movement can be on the border or in the interior of the region. Cluster 3: Part of the movement has to take place outside the region. The movement can end in all three possible locations: outside, inside, and on the border. Part of the movement can take place in the interior of the region or not. As indicated above, clusters 1 and 2 are stable across different clustering methods while cluster 3 seems to require a finer level of analysis, that is, it does not show up consistently across different methods. The clustering structure that is indicated in Figures 4 (dashed boxed) and Figure 5 (dashed lines) reflects this finer level of granularity at which all three clustering methods (Ward s, average linkage, complete linkage) agree. Topologically this finer level of distinction makes sense in that it reveals connected subgraphs of the CNG that are singled out. This finer level of granularity in cluster 3 can be summarized as follows: 3a contains the relations p 1, q 1, and d; the main characteristic here is that the movement ends outside the region while the starting location can be outside, inside, or on the boundary. Cluster 3b contains relations p 2 and q 2 ; the main characteristic is that the movement starts outside and ends either inside or on the boundary (after having been inside). Cluster 3c contains relations s 2 and r 2 ; the movement starts outside and ends on the border without intersection the

9 interior. Cluster 3d (r 1 and s 1 ) contains relations whose movement starts on the border and ends outside the region without intersecting the interior. Cluster 3e singles out relation f which is the only movement pattern that takes place completely outside the region. Cluster 3f singles out relation e which almost takes place completely outside the region except for the interior of the line thatt intersects with the boundary of the region. Finally, we would like to point out the somewhat special role of relation l. While it does seem to be integrated into cluster 1, the analysis of the raw similarities clearly indicates its different role in this cluster. This is not surprising as movement pattern l takes place completely on the boundary of the region. Fig. 4. The figure showss a combinationn of a heat map and cluster analysis (Ward s method). Additionally, rectangles indicate a synthesis of analyzing different clustering methods. The numbers identify clusterss discussed in the text. A color and high resolution version of this fig- ure is available at min.us/ /msdh2012_figure4.

10 The cluster analysiss shows obvious that we need a coarser granularity of movement primitives than offered by the DLine-Region calculus. As revealed by the general characteristics of the three clusters, important distinctions are made based on the movement in relation to the region focusing particularly on distinction of inside ver- linguis- sus outside movements or combinations thereof. We therefore looked into the tic descriptions (short labels) that participants provided. As the groupingg behavior differs from participant to participant we looked into the descriptions here from a general level using a word cloud to reveal frequencies of individual terms (additional- that ly we performed an actual quantitative word count). Figure 6 shows the results seem to reinforce our interpretation that the threee basic distinctions made in the interare distin- section models (including the 9+ model) are crucial, that is, movements guished on whether they are going in or out, and whether they take place inside or outside. The most frequently used movement related terms are (in this order): in, out, to, on, outside, straight, inside, through, and arch. Fig. 5. Merged DLine-Region CNG reflecting the analysis discussed in Figure 4, Section 4. The color coding corresponds to the clusters identifiedd by comparing three clustering methods. A color and high resolution version of this figure is available at min.us/msdh2012_figure5. Interestingly, most of these terms can be directly related to some topological char- and acteristics. However, there are also several non-topological terms such as straight arch. We will discuss the latter in the next section. 5 Discussion In our discussion of the results, we will draw comparisons to the previously mentioned research by Mark and Egenhofer (1994b, referred to henceforth as E&M94).

11 Althoughh their research setup focused on line-region relations and methodologically used a different set-up, several of their findings are worth looking into in the light of our results. One of E&M94 s central findings indicated that the 19 most frequently grouped pairs correspond to the 19 that were within the 9-intersection classes. We do find similar results in our data visualized in Figure 4 indicated by the fact that all three instances of all topological relations are in neighboring columns/rows, that is, they are grouped together most frequently compared to all other grouping possibilities. This shows that topology is indeed a major grouping criterion at the fine granularity analy- sis level. Fig. 6. Wordle showing the frequencies (larger meaning more often used) of words used by participants to characterize their groups (short labels). Equally important, E&M94 found a comparable pattern with respect to creating clusters of line-regionn relations, that is, cluster analysis reveals that the 26 original DLine-Region relations are conceptually not the primitives. Additionally, we found similarities in that larger clusters are formed by relations on the left side of the CNG (compare Figure 10 in E&M94 and Figure 5). While there are an additional 7 rela- tions in the DLine-Region calculus and several differences in the research methodolo- gy, this is an interesting finding. In both CNGs the left side has movement patterns that do not take place outside the region while the right side shows more variety in- addi- cluding the exterior of the region as a place where movement happens. Taking tionally into consideration the linguistic analysiss (see Figure 6), we find that in/out and inside/outside distinctions are both linguistically and conceptually a dominating criterion. E&M94 s analysis also singled out several relations as most likely forming their own clusters. Specifically, relations a, l, and f (for English speaking participants). We have discussed already the somewhat prominent situations of relations l and f, that is, that in our experiments they form more individualistic clusters as well. That relation a is not singled out may have to do with the different experimental setup: According to current event conceptualization theories, the ending relations of movement patterns play an important role in their conceptualization (Regier & Zheng, 2007). From this

12 perspective relation l is not that different from, for example, relation m 2. However, E&M94 s results are slightly different for different language groups (English versus German). Our research focused on English speaking participants but we will discuss the opportunity to use our research framework in cross-linguistic studies in the outlook. As the linguistic analysis reveals, participants did not group solely on the basis of topological information although it may appear this way. The question of the primacy of topology as a way to think spatially has received more attention recently (Klippel et al. in press; Schwering, 2011) calling into question the seminal statement that topology matters and metric refines (Egenhofer & Mark, 1995). Our experiment was not explicitly designed to address this question. However, there are certain constraints that inevitably lead to the introduction of factors other than topology to realize the 26 DLine-Region relations. For example, we used a circle as our region and thereby making it impossible for a straight line to start and end in the circle while having been outside it in between. Most prominently this aspect is featured in linguistic descriptions straight and arch, which made it into the top ten of the most frequent terms. There is current research in event conceptualization that stresses the importance of path characteristics on the conceptualization of movement patterns (Maguire, Brumberg, Ennis, & Shipley, 2011; Shipley & Maguire, 2008). 6 Conclusions To conclude, our research adds to the body of knowledge that asserts that distinctions in form of equivalence classes made by qualitative formal calculi are not necessarily the ones that are foundational to the human cognitive system. Several research approaches have shown that the granularity of formal calculi is inadequate for modeling human conceptualizations of both static and dynamic spatial relations. To address this issue the majority of approaches (e.g., Clementini, Di Felice, & van Oosterom, 1993; Schneider & Behr, 2006) define formal criteria on how to cluster topological equivalence classes such that the overall number of topological predicates is reduced. Clementini and collaborators (1993) suggest five basic relations that are derived on the basis of the emptiness and non-emptiness of component intersection, inclusion and non-inclusion of one object in another object, and the dimension of the component intersection. Schneider and Behr (2006) developed a method based exclusively on the emptiness and non-emptiness of component intersection. Coming from a database user perspective, Schneider and Behr developed the concepts of topological cluster predicates and topological predicate groups to reduce the number of predicates in a user-defined or application-specific manner. Our research adds to this body of knowledge by having behaviorally evaluated the DLine-Region calculus that requires, formally, the distinction of 26 relations between a directed line and a region based on an extended version of Egenhofer s intersection models. Kurata and Egenhofer (2009) discuss several approaches on reducing the 26 relations as their primary goal is to model human concepts of motion. While we find some commonalities in the approaches they discuss and the results of our experi-

13 ments, there is no complete agreement between any of the discussed approaches and our results. This demonstrates once more the importance of behavioral evaluations of qualitative calculi that is often called for (e.g., Clementini et al., 1993) but rarely delivered. Given the ubiquity of qualitative spatio-temporal calculi as tools and means to bridge the gap between a formal systems and human conceptions of space and time, our research has the potential to provide insights into necessary adaptations/modifications of qualitative calculi to deserve the label cognitively adequate. Our research methodology, in general, is tailored to calculi based on jointly exhaustive and pairwise disjoint relations that all form conceptual neighborhood graphs (Cohn & Renz, 2008). The use of JEPD and CNGs in areas such as linguistics (Ross, Hois, & Kelleher, 2010), robotics, database query languages and information retrieval, and ontological modeling (e.g. for the semantic web) can be greatly enhanced by behavioral research that provides the necessary bridge between cognitive and formal spatio-temporal semantics. Future research directions are manifold given both the ubiquity of QSTR in research and application and the paucity of behavioral evaluations. The following ones strike us as important: We have recently demonstrated that domain semantics has a meaningful influence on the grouping behavior of participants, that is, which original topological relations form cognitive conceptual clusters (Klippel in press). To this end, a theory is needed that would allow for specifying meta-domain characteristics and how they influence cognitive conceptualizations of movement patterns. Mark and Egenhofer (1994b) raised already the question of the influence of language on the conceptualization of line-region relations. Linguistic (and potentially cultural) influences surface in the spatial science sporadically (see Mark, Turk, & Stea, 2007 for a more substantial treatment) but are still not integrated into core theories. A transdisciplinary research agenda is needed to deliver results that could influence spatial theories more fundamentally. We have developed an approach to characterize movement patterns based on the notion of conceptual primitives (Klippel 2011) that is built on the basic distinctions used to define topological relations between a line and a region: interior, exterior, and boundary (the latter distinguished as movement on a spot and extended movement on the boundary, see also Kurata & Egenhofer, 2009). This approach is comparable to work on movement patterns by Stewart Hornsby and Cole (2007) in that basic topological distinctions constitute primitive distinctions that could be additionally annotated by using, for example, direction information. We consider it important to deepen this line of research as it potentially allows for linking linguistic expressions with formal spatial characterizations more flexibly and has the capability of modeling and interpreting continuous movement behavior. 7 References Adrienko, G., Adrienko, N., Dykes, J., Fabrikant, S. I., & Wachowicz, M. (2008). Geovisualization of dynamics, movement and change: Key issues and developing approaches in visualization research. Information Visualization, 7(3/4),

14 Allen, J. F. (1983). Maintaining knowledge about temporal intervals. Communications of the ACM, 26(11), Clementini, E., Di Felice, P., & van Oosterom, P. (1993). A small set of formal topological relationships suitable for end-user interaction. In D. Abel & B. C. Ooi (Eds.), Advances in Spatial Databases. Third International Symposium, SSD '93 Singapore, June 23 25, 1993 Proceedings (pp ). Berlin: Springer. Cohn, A. G. (2008). Conceptual neighborhood. In S. Shekhar & H. Xiong (Eds.), Encyclopedia of GIS (p. 123). Boston, MA: Springer. Cohn, A. G., & Renz, J. (2008). Qualitative spatial representation and reasoning. In F. van Harmelen, V. Lifschitz, & B. Porter (Eds.), Foundations of artificial intelligence. Handbook of knowledge representation (1st ed., pp ). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Egenhofer, M. J., & Al-Taha, K. K. (1992). Reasoning about gradual changes of topological relationships. In A. U. Frank, I. Campari, & U. Formentini (Eds.), Theories and methods of spatio-temporal reasoning in geographic space (pp ). Berlin: Springer. Egenhofer, M. J., & Franzosa, R. D. (1991). Point-set topological spatial relations. International Journal of Geographical Information Systems, 5(2), Egenhofer, M. J., & Mark, D. M. (1995). Naive geography. In A. U. Frank & W. Kuhn (Eds.), Spatial Information Theory. A Theoretical Basis for GIS. International Conference, COSIT 95, Semmering, Austria, September 21-23, 1995, Proceedings (pp. 1 15). Berlin: Springer. Freksa, C. (1992). Temporal reasoning based on semi-intervals. Artificial Intelligence, 54(1), Galton, A. (2000). Qualitative spatial change. Spatial information systems. Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press. Klippel, A. (in press). Spatial information theory meets spatial thinking - Is topology the Rosetta Stone of spatio-temporal cognition? Annals of the Association of American Geographers. Klippel, A. (2011). Movement choremes: Bridging cognitive understanding and formal characterization of movement patterns. Topics in Cognitive Science, 3(4), Klippel, A., Li, R., Yang, J., Hardisty, F., & Xu, S. (in press). The Egenhofer-Cohn hypothesis: Or, topological relativity? In M. Raubal, A. U. Frank, & D. M. Mark (Eds.), Cognitive and Linguistic Aspects of Geographic Space - New Perspectives on Geographic Information Research. Berlin: Springer. Knauff, M., Rauh, R., & Renz, J. (1997). A cognitive assessment of topological spatial relations: Results from an empirical investigation. In S. C. Hirtle & A. U. Frank (Eds.), Spatial information theory: A theoretical basis for GIS (pp ). Berlin: Springer. Kos, A. J., & Psenicka, C. (2000). Measuring cluster similarity across methods. Psychological Reports, 86, Kurata, Y. (2008). The 9+-intersection: A universal framework for modeling topological relations. In T. J. Cova, H. J. Miller, K. Beard, A. U. Frank, & M. F. Goodchild (Eds.), Geographic information science. 5th international conference, GIScience 2008, Park City, UT, USA, September 23-26, 2008 ; proceedings (pp ). Berlin: Springer. Kurata, Y., & Egenhofer, M. J. (2007). The 9+-Intersection for Topological Relations between a Directed Line Segment and a Region. In B. Gottfried (Ed.): Vol. 42. TZI Technical Report, 1st Workshop on Behaviour Monitoring and Interpretation (BMI'07), in conjunction with 30th German Conference on Artificial Intelligence (pp ). Universität Bremen. Kurata, Y., & Egenhofer, M. J. (2009). Interpretation of behaviors from a viewpoint of topology. In B. Gottfried & H. Aghajan (Eds.), Behaviour monitoring and interpretation. Ambient intelligence and smart environments (pp ). Amsterdam: IOS Press. Lu, S., Harter, D., & Graesser, A. C. (2009). An empirical and computational investigation of perceiving and remembering event temporal relations. Cognitive Science, 33, Maguire, M. J., Brumberg, J., Ennis, M., & Shipley, T. F. (2011). Similarities in object and event segmentation: A geometric approach to event path segmentation. Spatial Cognition and Computation, (3),

15 Mark, D. M., & Egenhofer, M. J. (1994a). Calibrating the meanings of spatial predicates from natural language: Line-region relations. In T. C. Waugh & R. G. Healey (Eds.), Advances in GIS Research, 6th International Symposium on Spatial Data Handling (pp ). Mark, D. M., & Egenhofer, M. J. (1994b). Modeling spatial relations between lines and regions: Combining formal mathematical models and human subject testing. Cartography and Geographic Information Systems, 21(3), Mark, D. M., & Egenhofer, M. J. (1995). Topology of prototypical spatial relations between lines and regions in English and Spanish. In Proceedings, Auto Carto 12, Charlotte, North Carolina, March 1995, (pp ). Mark, D. M., Turk, A. G., & Stea, D. (2007). Progress on Yindjibarndi ethnophysiography. In S. Winter, B. Kuipers, M. Duckham, & L. Kulik (Eds.), Spatial Information Theory. 9th International Conference, COSIT 2007, Melbourne, Australia, September 19-23, 2007 Proceedings (pp. 1 19). Berlin: Springer. Mennis, J., Peuquet, D. J., & Qian, L. (2000). A conceptual framework for incorporating cognitive principles into geographical database representation. International Journal of Geographical Information Science, 14(6), Muller, P. (2002). Topological spatio-temporal reasoning and representation. Computational Intelligence, 18(3), Randell, D. A., Cui, Z., & Cohn, A. G. (1992). A spatial logic based on regions and connections. In B. Nebel, C. Rich, & W. R. Swartout (Eds.), Proceedings of the 3rd International Conference on Knowledge Representation and Reasoning (pp ). Morgan Kaufmann. Randell, D., & Cohn, A. (1989). Modelling Topological and Metrical Properties in Physical Processes. In R. Brachman, H. Levesque, & R. Reiter (Eds.), Proceedings 1st International Conference on the Principles of Knowledge Representation and Reasoning (pp ). Los Altos: Morgan Kaufmann. Regier, T., & Zheng, M. (2007). Attention to endpoints: A cross-linguistic constraint on spatial meaning. Cognitive Science, 31(4), Ross, R. J., Hois, J., & Kelleher, J. (Eds.) (2010). Computational Models of Spatial Language Interpretation (CoSLI) Workshop at Spatial Cognition CEUR Workshop Proceedings. Schneider, M., & Behr, T. (2006). Topological relationships between complex spatial objects. ACM Transactions on Database Systems, 31(1), Schwering, A. (2011). Does metric really define topology (personal communication). May 20th, Shariff, A. R., Egenhofer, M. J., & Mark, D. M. (1998). Natural-language spatial relations between linear and areal objects: The topology and metric of English-language terms. International Journal of Geographical Information Science, 12(3), Shipley, T. F., & Maguire, M. J. (2008). Geometric information for event segmentation. In T. F. Shipley & J. M. Zacks (Eds.), Understanding events: How humans see, represent, and act on events (pp ). New York: Oxford University Press. Shipley, T. F., & Zacks, J. M. (Eds.) (2008). Understanding events: How humans see, represent, and act on events. New York: Oxford University Press. Sridhar, M., Cohn, A., & Hogg, D. (2011). From Video to RCC8: Exploiting a Distance Based Semantics to Stabilise the Interpretation of Mereotopological Relations: Spatial Information Theory. In M. Egenhofer, N. Giudice, R. Moratz, & M. Worboys (Eds.), Lecture Notes in Computer Science. Spatial Information Theory. 10th International Conference, COSIT 2011, Belfast, ME, USA, September 12-16, Proceedings (pp ). Springer. Stewart Hornsby, K., & Cole, S. (2007). Modeling moving geospatial objects from an eventbased perspective. Transactions in GIS, 11(4), van de Weghe, N., Billen, R., Kuijpers, B., & Bogaert, P. (Eds.) Moving Objects: From Natural to Formal Language: Workshop held in conjunction with GIScience 2008, Utah. Yuan, M., & Hornsby, K. S. (2008). Computation and visualization for the understanding dynamics in geographic domains: A research agenda. Boca Raton, Fla.: CRC Press.

GeoVISTA Center, Department of Geography, The Pennsylvania State University, PA, USA

GeoVISTA Center, Department of Geography, The Pennsylvania State University, PA, USA Formally grounding spatio-temporal thinking Klippel, A., Wallgrün, J. O., Yang, J., Li, R., & Dylla, F. (in print, 2012). Formally grounding spatio temporal thinking. Cognitive Processing. Alexander Klippel

More information

Alexander Klippel and Chris Weaver. GeoVISTA Center, Department of Geography The Pennsylvania State University, PA, USA

Alexander Klippel and Chris Weaver. GeoVISTA Center, Department of Geography The Pennsylvania State University, PA, USA Analyzing Behavioral Similarity Measures in Linguistic and Non-linguistic Conceptualization of Spatial Information and the Question of Individual Differences Alexander Klippel and Chris Weaver GeoVISTA

More information

Geographic Event Conceptualization: Where Spatial and Cognitive Sciences Meet

Geographic Event Conceptualization: Where Spatial and Cognitive Sciences Meet Geographic Event Conceptualization: Where Spatial and Cognitive Sciences Meet Rui Li, Alexander Klippel, Jinlong Yang {rui.li, klippel, jinlong}@psu.edu GeoVISTA Center, Department of Geography 302 Walker

More information

Convex Hull-Based Metric Refinements for Topological Spatial Relations

Convex Hull-Based Metric Refinements for Topological Spatial Relations ABSTRACT Convex Hull-Based Metric Refinements for Topological Spatial Relations Fuyu (Frank) Xu School of Computing and Information Science University of Maine Orono, ME 04469-5711, USA fuyu.xu@maine.edu

More information

Accepted by: Annals of the Association of American Geographers. Spatial Information Theory Meets Spatial Thinking - Is Topology the Rosetta Stone

Accepted by: Annals of the Association of American Geographers. Spatial Information Theory Meets Spatial Thinking - Is Topology the Rosetta Stone Accepted by: Annals of the Association of American Geographers Spatial Information Theory Meets Spatial Thinking - Is Topology the Rosetta Stone of Spatial Cognition? Alexander Klippel Abstract. Topology

More information

Towards Usable Topological Operators at GIS User Interfaces

Towards Usable Topological Operators at GIS User Interfaces Towards Usable Topological Operators at GIS User Interfaces Catharina Riedemann Institute for Geoinformatics, University of Münster Münster, Germany riedemann@ifgi.uni-muenster.de SUMMARY The topological

More information

THE PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIVERSITY SCHREYER HONORS COLLEGE DEPARTMENT OF GEOGRAPHY

THE PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIVERSITY SCHREYER HONORS COLLEGE DEPARTMENT OF GEOGRAPHY THE PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIVERSITY SCHREYER HONORS COLLEGE DEPARTMENT OF GEOGRAPHY ASSESSING THE COGNITVE ADEQUACY OF TOPOLOGICAL CALCULI: TRANSLATION VS. SCALING JINLONG YANG Spring 2011 A thesis submitted

More information

The Egenhofer Cohn Hypothesis or, Topological Relativity?

The Egenhofer Cohn Hypothesis or, Topological Relativity? The Egenhofer Cohn Hypothesis or, Topological Relativity? Alexander Klippel, Rui Li, Jinlong Yang, Frank Hardisty and Sen Xu Abstract In this chapter, we provide an overview of research on cognitively

More information

How to Handle Incomplete Knowledge Concerning Moving Objects

How to Handle Incomplete Knowledge Concerning Moving Objects B B is How to Handle Incomplete Knowledge Concerning Moving Objects Nico Van de Weghe 1, Peter Bogaert 1, Anthony G. Cohn 2, Matthias Delafontaine 1, Leen De Temmerman 1, Tijs Neutens 1, Philippe De Maeyer

More information

The endpoint hypothesis: A topological-cognitive assessment of geographic scale movement patterns

The endpoint hypothesis: A topological-cognitive assessment of geographic scale movement patterns The endpoint hypothesis: A topological-cognitive assessment of geographic scale movement patterns Alexander Klippel, Rui Li Department of Geography, GeoVISTA Center The Pennsylvania State University, University

More information

Intelligent GIS: Automatic generation of qualitative spatial information

Intelligent GIS: Automatic generation of qualitative spatial information Intelligent GIS: Automatic generation of qualitative spatial information Jimmy A. Lee 1 and Jane Brennan 1 1 University of Technology, Sydney, FIT, P.O. Box 123, Broadway NSW 2007, Australia janeb@it.uts.edu.au

More information

Investigations into the Cognitive Conceptualization and Similarity Assessment of Spatial Scenes 1

Investigations into the Cognitive Conceptualization and Similarity Assessment of Spatial Scenes 1 Wallgrün, J. O.; Yang, J.; Klippel, A.; Dylla, F. (in print, 01). Investigations into the cognitive conceptualization and similarity assessment of spatial scenes. In GIScience 01. Investigations into the

More information

Identifying Factors of Geographic Event Conceptualization

Identifying Factors of Geographic Event Conceptualization International Journal of Geographical Information Science Vol., No.,, 1 28 Identifying Factors of Geographic Event Conceptualization Alexander Klippel 1, Michael Worboys 2, Matt Duckham 3 1. GeoVISTA Center,

More information

Mappings For Cognitive Semantic Interoperability

Mappings For Cognitive Semantic Interoperability Mappings For Cognitive Semantic Interoperability Martin Raubal Institute for Geoinformatics University of Münster, Germany raubal@uni-muenster.de SUMMARY Semantic interoperability for geographic information

More information

Geographic Analysis of Linguistically Encoded Movement Patterns A Contextualized Perspective

Geographic Analysis of Linguistically Encoded Movement Patterns A Contextualized Perspective Geographic Analysis of Linguistically Encoded Movement Patterns A Contextualized Perspective Alexander Klippel 1, Alan MacEachren 1, Prasenjit Mitra 2, Ian Turton 1, Xiao Zhang 2, Anuj Jaiswal 2, Kean

More information

Cognitive Evaluation of Spatial Formalisms: Intuitive Granularities of Overlap Relations *

Cognitive Evaluation of Spatial Formalisms: Intuitive Granularities of Overlap Relations * Wallgrün, J. O., Yang, J., & Klippel, A. (2014). Cognitive evaluation of spatial formalisms: Intuitive granularities of overlap relations. International Journal of Cognitive Informatics and Natural Intelligence,

More information

Qualitative Spatial-Relation Reasoning for Design

Qualitative Spatial-Relation Reasoning for Design Qualitative Spatial-Relation Reasoning for Design Max J. Egenhofer National Center for Geographic Information and Analysis Department of Spatial Information Science and Engineering Department of Computer

More information

Location Based Concierge Service with Spatially Extended Topology for Moving Objects

Location Based Concierge Service with Spatially Extended Topology for Moving Objects The Journal of GIS Association of Korea, Vol. 17, No. 4, pp. 445-454, December 2009 Location Based Concierge Service with Spatially Extended Topology for Moving Objects Byoungjae Lee* ABSTRACT Beyond simple

More information

A General Framework for Conflation

A General Framework for Conflation A General Framework for Conflation Benjamin Adams, Linna Li, Martin Raubal, Michael F. Goodchild University of California, Santa Barbara, CA, USA Email: badams@cs.ucsb.edu, linna@geog.ucsb.edu, raubal@geog.ucsb.edu,

More information

Deriving place graphs from spatial databases

Deriving place graphs from spatial databases 25 Deriving place graphs from spatial databases Ehsan Hamzei ehamzei@student.unimelb.edu.au Hua Hua Research School of Computer Science Australian National University hua.hua@anu.edu.au Martin Tomko tomkom@unimelb.edu.au

More information

An Ontology-based Framework for Modeling Movement on a Smart Campus

An Ontology-based Framework for Modeling Movement on a Smart Campus An Ontology-based Framework for Modeling Movement on a Smart Campus Junchuan Fan 1, Kathleen Stewart 1 1 Department of Geographical and Sustainability Sciences, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA, 52242,

More information

Spatial Reasoning With A Hole

Spatial Reasoning With A Hole Spatial Reasoning With A Hole Max J. Egenhofer and Maria Vasardani National Center for Geographic Information and Analysis and Department of Spatial Information Science and Engineering University of Maine

More information

Relative adjacencies in spatial pseudo-partitions

Relative adjacencies in spatial pseudo-partitions Relative adjacencies in spatial pseudo-partitions Roderic Béra 1, Christophe Claramunt 1 1 Naval Academy Research Institute, Lanvéoc-Poulmic, BP 600, 29240 Brest Naval, France {bera, claramunt}@ecole-navale.fr

More information

Exploring Spatial Relationships for Knowledge Discovery in Spatial Data

Exploring Spatial Relationships for Knowledge Discovery in Spatial Data 2009 International Conference on Computer Engineering and Applications IPCSIT vol.2 (2011) (2011) IACSIT Press, Singapore Exploring Spatial Relationships for Knowledge Discovery in Spatial Norazwin Buang

More information

Granular Description of Qualitative Change

Granular Description of Qualitative Change Proceedings of the Twenty-Third International Joint Conference on Artificial Intelligence Granular Description of Qualitative Change John G. Stell School of Computing University of Leeds, U.K. Abstract

More information

On minimal models of the Region Connection Calculus

On minimal models of the Region Connection Calculus Fundamenta Informaticae 69 (2006) 1 20 1 IOS Press On minimal models of the Region Connection Calculus Lirong Xia State Key Laboratory of Intelligent Technology and Systems Department of Computer Science

More information

How context influences the segmentation of movement trajectories an experimental approach for environmental and behavioral context

How context influences the segmentation of movement trajectories an experimental approach for environmental and behavioral context How context influences the segmentation of movement trajectories an experimental approach for environmental and behavioral context Anna-Katharina Lautenschütz 1 Department of Geography, University of Zürich,

More information

Spatio-temporal configurations of dynamics points in a 1D space

Spatio-temporal configurations of dynamics points in a 1D space Spatio-temporal configurations of dynamics points in a 1D space Pierre Hallot 1, Roland Billen 1 1 Geomatics Unit, University of Liège, Allée du 6 Août, 17 B-4000 Liège Belgium {P.Hallot@ulg.ac.be, rbillen@ulg.ac.be}

More information

Maintaining Relational Consistency in a Graph-Based Place Database

Maintaining Relational Consistency in a Graph-Based Place Database Maintaining Relational Consistency in a Graph-Based Place Database Hao Chen Department of Infrastructure Engineering, Geomatics The University of Melbourne Email: hchen@student.unimelb.edu.au Introduction

More information

Naming Topological Operators at GIS User Interfaces

Naming Topological Operators at GIS User Interfaces Naming Topological Operators at GIS User Interfaces Catharina Riedemann Institute for Geoinformatics, University of Münster Münster, Germany riedemann@uni-muenster.de SUMMARY User interfaces of geospatial

More information

Metrics and topologies for geographic space

Metrics and topologies for geographic space Published in Proc. 7 th Intl. Symp. Spatial Data Handling, Delft, Netherlands. Metrics and topologies for geographic space Michael F Worboys Department of Computer Science, Keele University, Staffs ST5

More information

Towards Process-Based Ontology for Representing Dynamic Geospatial Phenomena

Towards Process-Based Ontology for Representing Dynamic Geospatial Phenomena Towards Process-Based Ontology for Representing Dynamic Geospatial Phenomena Anusuriya Devaraju Institute for Geoinformatics, University of Muenster, Germany anusuriya.devaraju@uni-muenster.de 1 Introduction

More information

Qualitative Spatio-Temporal Reasoning & Spatial Database Design

Qualitative Spatio-Temporal Reasoning & Spatial Database Design Qualitative Spatio-Temporal Reasoning Focus on 2 research topics at the of the University of Liège J-P. Donnay P. Hallot F. Laplanche Curriculum in «Surveying & Geomatics» in the Faculty of Sciences of

More information

gathered by Geosensor Networks

gathered by Geosensor Networks Towards a generalization of data gathered by Geosensor Networks Christoph Stasch Bilateral Event 2009 NationalInstitute for Space Research (INPE) Sao Paulo, Brazil Institute for Geoinformatics Institute

More information

VISUAL ANALYTICS APPROACH FOR CONSIDERING UNCERTAINTY INFORMATION IN CHANGE ANALYSIS PROCESSES

VISUAL ANALYTICS APPROACH FOR CONSIDERING UNCERTAINTY INFORMATION IN CHANGE ANALYSIS PROCESSES VISUAL ANALYTICS APPROACH FOR CONSIDERING UNCERTAINTY INFORMATION IN CHANGE ANALYSIS PROCESSES J. Schiewe HafenCity University Hamburg, Lab for Geoinformatics and Geovisualization, Hebebrandstr. 1, 22297

More information

Three-Dimensional Visualization of Activity-Travel Patterns

Three-Dimensional Visualization of Activity-Travel Patterns C. Rinner 231 Three-Dimensional Visualization of Activity-Travel Patterns Claus Rinner Department of Geography University of Toronto, Canada rinner@geog.utoronto.ca ABSTRACT Geographers have long been

More information

Lecture 1: Geospatial Data Models

Lecture 1: Geospatial Data Models Lecture 1: GEOG413/613 Dr. Anthony Jjumba Introduction Course Outline Journal Article Review Projects (and short presentations) Final Exam (April 3) Participation in class discussions Geog413/Geog613 A

More information

Machine Learning for Interpretation of Spatial Natural Language in terms of QSR

Machine Learning for Interpretation of Spatial Natural Language in terms of QSR Machine Learning for Interpretation of Spatial Natural Language in terms of QSR Parisa Kordjamshidi 1, Joana Hois 2, Martijn van Otterlo 1, and Marie-Francine Moens 1 1 Katholieke Universiteit Leuven,

More information

reviewed paper Supporting Spatial Planning with Qualitative Configuration Analysis Paolo Fogliaroni, Gerhard Navratil

reviewed paper Supporting Spatial Planning with Qualitative Configuration Analysis Paolo Fogliaroni, Gerhard Navratil reviewed paper Supporting Spatial Planning with Qualitative Configuration Analysis Paolo Fogliaroni, Gerhard Navratil (Dr. Paolo Fogliaroni, Vienna University or Technology, Department for Geodesy and

More information

Spatial Relations for Semantic Similarity Measurement

Spatial Relations for Semantic Similarity Measurement Spatial Relations for Semantic Similarity Measurement Angela Schwering 1,2 and Martin Raubal 2 1 Ordnance Survey of Great Britain, United Kingdom 2 Institute for Geoinformatics, University of Muenster,

More information

Cognitive Engineering for Geographic Information Science

Cognitive Engineering for Geographic Information Science Cognitive Engineering for Geographic Information Science Martin Raubal Department of Geography, UCSB raubal@geog.ucsb.edu 21 Jan 2009 ThinkSpatial, UCSB 1 GIScience Motivation systematic study of all aspects

More information

A Class of Star-Algebras for Point-Based Qualitative Reasoning in Two- Dimensional Space

A Class of Star-Algebras for Point-Based Qualitative Reasoning in Two- Dimensional Space From: FLAIRS- Proceedings. Copyright AAAI (www.aaai.org). All rights reserved. A Class of Star-Algebras for Point-Based Qualitative Reasoning in Two- Dimensional Space Debasis Mitra Department of Computer

More information

Relations Between Spatial Calculi About Directions and Orientations (Extended Abstract 1 )

Relations Between Spatial Calculi About Directions and Orientations (Extended Abstract 1 ) Relations Between Spatial Calculi About Directions and Orientations (Extended Abstract 1 ) Till Mossakowski 1, Reinhard Moratz 2 1 Otto-von-Guericke-University of Magdeburg, Germany 2 University of Maine,

More information

Qualitative Spatial Calculi with Assymetric Granularity

Qualitative Spatial Calculi with Assymetric Granularity Qualitative Spatial Calculi with Assymetric Granularity Sree Harsha R November 13, 2007 Abstract Qualitative Spatial/Direction Calculi gives the orientation of any two points to eachother in a 2-D space

More information

Spatio-Temporal Relationships in a Primitive Space: an attempt to simplify spatio-temporal analysis

Spatio-Temporal Relationships in a Primitive Space: an attempt to simplify spatio-temporal analysis Spatio-Temporal Relationships in a Primitive Space: an attempt to simplify spatio-temporal analysis Pierre Hallot 1 1 Geomatics Unit / University of Liège (Belgium) P.Hallot@ulg.ac.be INTRODUCTION Nowadays,

More information

A qualitative trajectory calculus as a basis for representing moving objects in Geographical Information Systems

A qualitative trajectory calculus as a basis for representing moving objects in Geographical Information Systems Control and Cybernetics vol. 35 (2006) No. 1 A qualitative trajectory calculus as a basis for representing moving objects in Geographical Information Systems by Nico Van de Weghe 1, Anthony G. Cohn 2,

More information

Cognitive modeling with conceptual spaces

Cognitive modeling with conceptual spaces Cognitive modeling with conceptual spaces Martin Raubal Department of Geography, University of California at Santa Barbara 5713 Ellison Hall, Santa Barbara, CA 93106, U.S.A. raubal@geog.ucsb.edu Abstract.

More information

From Research Objects to Research Networks: Combining Spatial and Semantic Search

From Research Objects to Research Networks: Combining Spatial and Semantic Search From Research Objects to Research Networks: Combining Spatial and Semantic Search Sara Lafia 1 and Lisa Staehli 2 1 Department of Geography, UCSB, Santa Barbara, CA, USA 2 Institute of Cartography and

More information

Modeling Temporal Uncertainty of Events: a. Descriptive Perspective

Modeling Temporal Uncertainty of Events: a. Descriptive Perspective Modeling Temporal Uncertainty of Events: a escriptive Perspective Yihong Yuan, Yu Liu 2 epartment of Geography, University of California, Santa Barbara, CA, USA yuan@geog.ucsb.edu 2 Institute of Remote

More information

Formalizing natural-language spatial relations between linear objects with topological and metric properties

Formalizing natural-language spatial relations between linear objects with topological and metric properties Formalizing natural-language spatial relations between linear objects with topological and metric properties JUN XU* Institute of Geographic Science and Natural Resources Research, Chinese Academy of Science,

More information

Detailed and Integrated Representation of Spatial Relations

Detailed and Integrated Representation of Spatial Relations Detailed and Integrated Representation of Spatial Relations WU Changbin, Chen Xia College of Geographical science Nanjing Normal University, Nanjing 210023, China Abstract. Spatial relation is one of the

More information

GEO-INFORMATION (LAKE DATA) SERVICE BASED ON ONTOLOGY

GEO-INFORMATION (LAKE DATA) SERVICE BASED ON ONTOLOGY GEO-INFORMATION (LAKE DATA) SERVICE BASED ON ONTOLOGY Long-hua He* and Junjie Li Nanjing Institute of Geography & Limnology, Chinese Academy of Science, Nanjing 210008, China * Email: lhhe@niglas.ac.cn

More information

Spatial Computing. or how to design a right-brain hemisphere. Christian Freksa University of Bremen

Spatial Computing. or how to design a right-brain hemisphere. Christian Freksa University of Bremen Spatial Computing or how to design a right-brain hemisphere Christian Freksa University of Bremen 1 Acknowledgments 2 Some Examples of Spatial Problems (How) can I get the piano into my living room? How

More information

Environmental Cognition and Perception I

Environmental Cognition and Perception I Environmental Cognition and Perception I Review: Spatial Interaction and Spatial Behavior II - Individual travel behavior - Activity space - Mental maps How we perceive the environment Maps in the head

More information

Kai-Florian Richter Stephan Winter. Landmarks. GIScience for Intelligent Services

Kai-Florian Richter Stephan Winter. Landmarks. GIScience for Intelligent Services Landmarks Kai-Florian Richter Stephan Winter Landmarks GIScience for Intelligent Services 123 Kai-Florian Richter Department of Geography University of Zurich Zurich, Switzerland Stephan Winter Department

More information

Taxonomies of Building Objects towards Topographic and Thematic Geo-Ontologies

Taxonomies of Building Objects towards Topographic and Thematic Geo-Ontologies Taxonomies of Building Objects towards Topographic and Thematic Geo-Ontologies Melih Basaraner Division of Cartography, Department of Geomatic Engineering, Yildiz Technical University (YTU), Istanbul Turkey

More information

pursues interdisciplinary long-term research in Spatial Cognition. Particular emphasis is given to:

pursues interdisciplinary long-term research in Spatial Cognition. Particular emphasis is given to: The Transregional Collaborative Research Center SFB/TR 8 Spatial Cognition: Reasoning, Action, Interaction at the Universities of Bremen and Freiburg, Germany pursues interdisciplinary long-term research

More information

Preferred Mental Models in Qualitative Spatial Reasoning: A Cognitive Assessment of Allen s Calculus

Preferred Mental Models in Qualitative Spatial Reasoning: A Cognitive Assessment of Allen s Calculus Knauff, M., Rauh, R., & Schlieder, C. (1995). Preferred mental models in qualitative spatial reasoning: A cognitive assessment of Allen's calculus. In Proceedings of the Seventeenth Annual Conference of

More information

Clock-Modeled Ternary Spatial Relations for Visual Scene Analysis

Clock-Modeled Ternary Spatial Relations for Visual Scene Analysis Clock-Modeled Ternary Spatial Relations for Visual Scene Analysis Joanna Isabelle Olszewska School of Computing and Engineering, University of Huddersfield Queensgate, Huddersfield, HD1 3DH, United Kingdom

More information

The Concept of Geographic Relevance

The Concept of Geographic Relevance The Concept of Geographic Relevance Tumasch Reichenbacher, Stefano De Sabbata, Paul Crease University of Zurich Winterthurerstr. 190 8057 Zurich, Switzerland Keywords Geographic relevance, context INTRODUCTION

More information

Why Is Cartographic Generalization So Hard?

Why Is Cartographic Generalization So Hard? 1 Why Is Cartographic Generalization So Hard? Andrew U. Frank Department for Geoinformation and Cartography Gusshausstrasse 27-29/E-127-1 A-1040 Vienna, Austria frank@geoinfo.tuwien.ac.at 1 Introduction

More information

Curriculum Vitae. Dr. Danqing (Dana) Xiao. Ph.D. in Department of Geography, University of California Santa Barbara.

Curriculum Vitae. Dr. Danqing (Dana) Xiao. Ph.D. in Department of Geography, University of California Santa Barbara. Curriculum Vitae Dr. Danqing (Dana) Xiao Personal Information Assistant Professor Department of Environmental, Geographical, and Geological Sciences (EGGS), Bloomsburg University of Pennsylvania Research

More information

NR402 GIS Applications in Natural Resources

NR402 GIS Applications in Natural Resources NR402 GIS Applications in Natural Resources Lesson 1 Introduction to GIS Eva Strand, University of Idaho Map of the Pacific Northwest from http://www.or.blm.gov/gis/ Welcome to NR402 GIS Applications in

More information

Adding ternary complex roles to ALCRP(D)

Adding ternary complex roles to ALCRP(D) Adding ternary complex roles to ALCRP(D) A.Kaplunova, V. Haarslev, R.Möller University of Hamburg, Computer Science Department Vogt-Kölln-Str. 30, 22527 Hamburg, Germany Abstract The goal of this paper

More information

A reference landform ontology for automated delineation of depression landforms from DEMs

A reference landform ontology for automated delineation of depression landforms from DEMs A reference landform ontology for automated delineation of depression landforms from DEMs Gaurav Sinha 1, Samantha Arundel 2, Kathleen Stewart 3, David Mark 4, Torsten Hahmann 5, Boleslo Romero 6, Alexandre

More information

Web Visualization of Geo-Spatial Data using SVG and VRML/X3D

Web Visualization of Geo-Spatial Data using SVG and VRML/X3D Web Visualization of Geo-Spatial Data using SVG and VRML/X3D Jianghui Ying Falls Church, VA 22043, USA jying@vt.edu Denis Gračanin Blacksburg, VA 24061, USA gracanin@vt.edu Chang-Tien Lu Falls Church,

More information

SPATIAL DATA MINING. Ms. S. Malathi, Lecturer in Computer Applications, KGiSL - IIM

SPATIAL DATA MINING. Ms. S. Malathi, Lecturer in Computer Applications, KGiSL - IIM SPATIAL DATA MINING Ms. S. Malathi, Lecturer in Computer Applications, KGiSL - IIM INTRODUCTION The main difference between data mining in relational DBS and in spatial DBS is that attributes of the neighbors

More information

A set theoretic view of the ISA hierarchy

A set theoretic view of the ISA hierarchy Loughborough University Institutional Repository A set theoretic view of the ISA hierarchy This item was submitted to Loughborough University's Institutional Repository by the/an author. Citation: CHEUNG,

More information

Key Words: geospatial ontologies, formal concept analysis, semantic integration, multi-scale, multi-context.

Key Words: geospatial ontologies, formal concept analysis, semantic integration, multi-scale, multi-context. Marinos Kavouras & Margarita Kokla Department of Rural and Surveying Engineering National Technical University of Athens 9, H. Polytechniou Str., 157 80 Zografos Campus, Athens - Greece Tel: 30+1+772-2731/2637,

More information

Conceptual Modeling in the Environmental Domain

Conceptual Modeling in the Environmental Domain Appeared in: 15 th IMACS World Congress on Scientific Computation, Modelling and Applied Mathematics, Berlin, 1997 Conceptual Modeling in the Environmental Domain Ulrich Heller, Peter Struss Dept. of Computer

More information

Representation of Geographic Data

Representation of Geographic Data GIS 5210 Week 2 The Nature of Spatial Variation Three principles of the nature of spatial variation: proximity effects are key to understanding spatial variation issues of geographic scale and level of

More information

Spatial Cognition: From Rat-Research to Multifunctional Spatial Assistance Systems

Spatial Cognition: From Rat-Research to Multifunctional Spatial Assistance Systems Spatial Cognition: From Rat-Research to Multifunctional Spatial Assistance Systems Markus Knauff, Christoph Schlieder, Christian Freksa Spatial cognition research is making rapid progress in understanding

More information

Towards a Structured Analysis of Approximate Problem Solving: ACaseStudy in Classification

Towards a Structured Analysis of Approximate Problem Solving: ACaseStudy in Classification Towards a Structured Analysis of Approximate Problem Solving: ACaseStudy in Classification Perry Groot and Annette ten Teije and Frank van Harmelen Division of Mathematics and Computer Science, Faculty

More information

Modeling Discrete Processes Over Multiple Levels Of Detail Using Partial Function Application

Modeling Discrete Processes Over Multiple Levels Of Detail Using Partial Function Application Modeling Discrete Processes Over Multiple Levels Of Detail Using Partial Function Application Paul WEISER a and Andrew U. FRANK a a Technical University of Vienna Institute for Geoinformation and Cartography,

More information

Affordances in Representing the Behaviour of Event-Based Systems

Affordances in Representing the Behaviour of Event-Based Systems Affordances in Representing the Behaviour of Event-Based Systems Fahim T. IMAM a,1, Thomas R. DEAN b a School of Computing, Queen s University, Canada b Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering,

More information

Spatial Intelligence. Angela Schwering

Spatial Intelligence. Angela Schwering Spatial Intelligence Angela Schwering What I do What I do intelligent representation and processing of spatial information From the Cognitive Perspective How do humans perceive spatial information? How

More information

Structural Salience as a Landmark

Structural Salience as a Landmark Structural Salience as a Landmark Introduction Alexander Klippel 1, Kai-Florian Richter 2, Stefan Hansen 2 1 Cooperative Research Centre for Spatial Information The University of Melbourne, Australia aklippel@unimelb.edu.au

More information

A Computable Language of Architecture

A Computable Language of Architecture A Computable Language of Architecture Description of Descriptor Language in Supporting Compound Definitions Introduction Sora Key Carnegie Mellon University, Computational Design Laboratory, USA http://www.code.arc.cmu.edu

More information

Discriminative Direction for Kernel Classifiers

Discriminative Direction for Kernel Classifiers Discriminative Direction for Kernel Classifiers Polina Golland Artificial Intelligence Lab Massachusetts Institute of Technology Cambridge, MA 02139 polina@ai.mit.edu Abstract In many scientific and engineering

More information

MULTIDIMENSIONAL REPRESENTATION OF GEOGRAPHIC FEATURES. E. Lynn Usery U.S. Geological Survey United States of America

MULTIDIMENSIONAL REPRESENTATION OF GEOGRAPHIC FEATURES. E. Lynn Usery U.S. Geological Survey United States of America MULTIDIMENSIONAL REPRESENTATION OF GEOGRAPHIC FEATURES E. Lynn Usery U.S. Geological Survey United States of America KEY WORDS: knowledge representation, object-oriented, multi-scale database, data models,

More information

Implementing Visual Analytics Methods for Massive Collections of Movement Data

Implementing Visual Analytics Methods for Massive Collections of Movement Data Implementing Visual Analytics Methods for Massive Collections of Movement Data G. Andrienko, N. Andrienko Fraunhofer Institute Intelligent Analysis and Information Systems Schloss Birlinghoven, D-53754

More information

Multi-cultural Aspects of Spatial Knowledge

Multi-cultural Aspects of Spatial Knowledge Multi-cultural Aspects of Spatial Knowledge Andrew U. Frank Geoinformation TU Vienna frank@geoinfo.tuwien.ac.at www.geoinfo.tuwien.ac.at Andrew Frank 1 Overview 1. What is culture? 2. Cultural influences

More information

Knowledge Discovery Based Query Answering in Hierarchical Information Systems

Knowledge Discovery Based Query Answering in Hierarchical Information Systems Knowledge Discovery Based Query Answering in Hierarchical Information Systems Zbigniew W. Raś 1,2, Agnieszka Dardzińska 3, and Osman Gürdal 4 1 Univ. of North Carolina, Dept. of Comp. Sci., Charlotte,

More information

INVESTIGATING GEOSPARQL REQUIREMENTS FOR PARTICIPATORY URBAN PLANNING

INVESTIGATING GEOSPARQL REQUIREMENTS FOR PARTICIPATORY URBAN PLANNING INVESTIGATING GEOSPARQL REQUIREMENTS FOR PARTICIPATORY URBAN PLANNING E. Mohammadi a, *, A. J.S. Hunter a a Geomatics Department, Schulich School of Engineering, University of Calgary, Calgary, T2N 1N4,

More information

Cultural and Language Influences on the Interpretation of Spatial Prepositions

Cultural and Language Influences on the Interpretation of Spatial Prepositions Cultural and Language Influences on the Interpretation of Spatial Prepositions Mark M. HALL and Christopher B. JONES Abstract Culture and language can influence the generation and interpretation of spatial

More information

CONCEPTUAL DEVELOPMENT OF AN ASSISTANT FOR CHANGE DETECTION AND ANALYSIS BASED ON REMOTELY SENSED SCENES

CONCEPTUAL DEVELOPMENT OF AN ASSISTANT FOR CHANGE DETECTION AND ANALYSIS BASED ON REMOTELY SENSED SCENES CONCEPTUAL DEVELOPMENT OF AN ASSISTANT FOR CHANGE DETECTION AND ANALYSIS BASED ON REMOTELY SENSED SCENES J. Schiewe University of Osnabrück, Institute for Geoinformatics and Remote Sensing, Seminarstr.

More information

DIMENSIONS OF QUALITATIVE SPATIAL REASONING

DIMENSIONS OF QUALITATIVE SPATIAL REASONING In Qualitative Reasoning in Decision Technologies, Proc. QUARDET 93, N. Piera Carreté & M.G. Singh, eds., CIMNE Barcelona 1993, pp. 483-492. DIMENSIONS OF QUALITATIVE SPATIAL REASONING Christian Freksa

More information

Combining cardinal direction relations and other orientation relations in QSR

Combining cardinal direction relations and other orientation relations in QSR Combining cardinal direction relations and other orientation relations in QSR mar Isli Fachbereich Informatik, Universität Hamburg, Vogt-Kölln-Strasse 30, D-22527 Hamburg, Germany isli@informatik.uni-hamburg.de

More information

Can Vector Space Bases Model Context?

Can Vector Space Bases Model Context? Can Vector Space Bases Model Context? Massimo Melucci University of Padua Department of Information Engineering Via Gradenigo, 6/a 35031 Padova Italy melo@dei.unipd.it Abstract Current Information Retrieval

More information

Similarities and differences between outdoor and indoor space from the perspective of navigation

Similarities and differences between outdoor and indoor space from the perspective of navigation Similarities and differences between outdoor and indoor space from the perspective of navigation (Extended Abstract) Liping Yang, Michael Worboys Department of Spatial Information Science and Engineering,

More information

arxiv: v1 [cs.ai] 1 Jun 2016

arxiv: v1 [cs.ai] 1 Jun 2016 A A Survey of Qualitative Spatial and Temporal Calculi Algebraic and Computational Properties FRANK DYLLA, University of Bremen JAE HEE LEE, University of Bremen TILL MOSSAKOWSKI, University of Magdeburg

More information

Reproducible AGILE

Reproducible AGILE Reproducible Research @ AGILE The term Reproducible Research refers to the idea that the ultimate product of academic research is a scientific paper published along with the data, methods and full computational

More information

Comparing Color and Leader Line Approaches for Highlighting in Geovisualization

Comparing Color and Leader Line Approaches for Highlighting in Geovisualization Comparing Color and Leader Line Approaches for Highlighting in Geovisualization A. L. Griffin 1, A. C. Robinson 2 1 School of Physical, Environmental, and Mathematical Sciences, University of New South

More information

4CitySemantics. GIS-Semantic Tool for Urban Intervention Areas

4CitySemantics. GIS-Semantic Tool for Urban Intervention Areas 4CitySemantics GIS-Semantic Tool for Urban Intervention Areas Nuno MONTENEGRO 1 ; Jorge GOMES 2 ; Paulo URBANO 2 José P. DUARTE 1 1 Faculdade de Arquitectura da Universidade Técnica de Lisboa, Rua Sá Nogueira,

More information

A CARTOGRAPHIC DATA MODEL FOR BETTER GEOGRAPHICAL VISUALIZATION BASED ON KNOWLEDGE

A CARTOGRAPHIC DATA MODEL FOR BETTER GEOGRAPHICAL VISUALIZATION BASED ON KNOWLEDGE A CARTOGRAPHIC DATA MODEL FOR BETTER GEOGRAPHICAL VISUALIZATION BASED ON KNOWLEDGE Yang MEI a, *, Lin LI a a School Of Resource And Environmental Science, Wuhan University,129 Luoyu Road, Wuhan 430079,

More information

Research Note: A more powerful test statistic for reasoning about interference between units

Research Note: A more powerful test statistic for reasoning about interference between units Research Note: A more powerful test statistic for reasoning about interference between units Jake Bowers Mark Fredrickson Peter M. Aronow August 26, 2015 Abstract Bowers, Fredrickson and Panagopoulos (2012)

More information

Geospatial Semantics. Yingjie Hu. Geospatial Semantics

Geospatial Semantics. Yingjie Hu. Geospatial Semantics Outline What is geospatial? Why do we need it? Existing researches. Conclusions. What is geospatial? Semantics The meaning of expressions Syntax How you express the meaning E.g. I love GIS What is geospatial?

More information

Zero Hours of System Training

Zero Hours of System Training Zero Hours of System Training A challenge for the next 25 years Werner Kuhn Center for Spatial Studies Department of Geography University of California at Santa Barbara 1854 http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/file:snow-cholera-map-1.jpg

More information

Fuzzy Systems. Introduction

Fuzzy Systems. Introduction Fuzzy Systems Introduction Prof. Dr. Rudolf Kruse Christian Moewes {kruse,cmoewes}@iws.cs.uni-magdeburg.de Otto-von-Guericke University of Magdeburg Faculty of Computer Science Department of Knowledge

More information

Fuzzy Systems. Introduction

Fuzzy Systems. Introduction Fuzzy Systems Introduction Prof. Dr. Rudolf Kruse Christoph Doell {kruse,doell}@iws.cs.uni-magdeburg.de Otto-von-Guericke University of Magdeburg Faculty of Computer Science Department of Knowledge Processing

More information