BEHAVIORAL MECHANISMS OF COEXISTENCE IN SYMPATRIC SPECIES OF DESERT RODENTS, DIPODOMYS ORDII AND D. MERRIAMI

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "BEHAVIORAL MECHANISMS OF COEXISTENCE IN SYMPATRIC SPECIES OF DESERT RODENTS, DIPODOMYS ORDII AND D. MERRIAMI"

Transcription

1 BEHAVIORAL MECHANISMS OF COEXISTENCE IN SYMPATRIC SPECIES OF DESERT RODENTS, DIPODOMYS ORDII AND D. MERRIAMI LEONORA M. PERRI AND JAN A. RANDALL Department of Biology, San Francisco State University, San Francisco, CA We investigated intraspecific and interspecific behaviors that function as mechanisms of social spacing and tolerance to allow coexistence of sympatric species of desert rodents. Radiotracking data showed overlap in home ranges of two species of kangaroo rats, Dipodomys ordii and D. merriami. Home ranges of males of both species overlapped females; females exhibited exclusive home ranges. Although D. ordii dominated D. merriami in paired encounters in the laboratory, D. merriami avoided D. ordii, and aggression was infrequent Tests for recognition by olfactory cues revealed that kangaroo rats did not respond to heterospecific scent and behaviors of intraspecific recognition of D. ordii were similar to those described previously for D. merriami. Females responded to the scent of con specific females and preferred the scent of familiar, con specific males; males showed no preferences. These two species of kangaroo rats probably coexist through interspecific avoidance and intraspecific spacing mediated by familiarity and neighbor recognition in females. Key words: Dipodomys ordii, Dipodomys merriami, home range, sympatry, behavior, coexistence, olfaction, dominance, avoidance Our goal was to investigate intraspecific and interspecific behaviors that might function as mechanisms of social spacing and tolerance to allow biologically similar, sympatric species to coexist. Coexistence of ecologically similar rodents in desert communities has long been of interest to community ecologists (see reviews by Brown and Harney, 1993; Randall, 1993; Reichman and Price, 1993). Small rodents, especially members of the Heteromyidae (kangaroo rats, Dipodomys; kangaroo mice, Microdipodops; pocket mice, Perognathus and Chaetodipus), have been of special interest because of the use of seeds as the same ephemeral food resources by coexisting species (Heske et ai., 1994; Reichman and Price, 1993; Rosenzweig and Winakur, 1969). Up to six species of heteromyids can coexist in desert communities (Brown, 1973). Because similar species often overlap in use of resources, the potential for compe- tition may be high. Roles of resource partitioning and competition in structuring heteromyid communities, therefore, have received much attention in the quest to answer how closely related species in North American deserts coexist (Brown and Harney, 1993; Heske et al., 1994). It is well accepted that the diversity of sympatric granivores is associated with complexity of habitat (Reichman and Price, 1993). Rodents might partition resources by habitat selection (M'Closkey, 1976, 1978; Schroder and Rosenzweig, 1975), exploitative competition for seeds in different microhabitats (Bowers, 1982; Price, 1978), optimizing foraging choices (Price and Heinz, 1984; Price and Reichman, 1987; Reichman and Oberstein, 1977), and differences in hoarding behavior (Jenkins and Breck, 1998) and seed husking (Dayan and Simberloff, 1994; Randall, 1993). Risk of predation might promote coexistence based on occupation of micohabitats where each spe- Journal of Mammalogy. 80(4): ,

2 1298 JOURNAL OF MAMMALOGY Vol. 80, No.4 cies is best adapted to compete for resources while avoiding predation (Kotler et ai., 1994). Use of habitat could vary temporally (Haim and Rozenfeld, 1993), and some sympatric heteromyids are active during distinctly different times of the night or season (Kenagy, 1973; O'Farrell, 1974) and use similar habitats at different times of the year (M'Closkey, 1976; O'Farrell, 1980). Coexisting heteromyids also vary in body size, which could influence habitat use and help to space species. Typical body sizes of granivores in desert communities differ by ratios of > 1.5 (Bowers and Brown, 1982; Brown, 1973). Brown and Lieberman (1973) predicted that heteromyids reduce competition by selecting different-sized seeds based upon these differences in body size. However, the seed-sizeselection hypothesis has not been supported (Lemen, 1978; Price, 1983), and resource partitioning of seeds by size is not an important mechanism of coexistence in heteromyids (Randall, 1993). Social interactions are neglected in most of the above investigations. Even when behavior is considered, aggressive interactions are assumed to be instrumental in interference competition as a means of shaping community structure (Brown and Munger, 1985; Frye, 1983; Reichman, 1983). Larger species of heteromyids are believed to dominate smaller species by aggressive interference (Basset, 1995; Blaustein and Risser, 1976; Frye, 1983). Social behaviors that minimize aggression, such as recognition of neighbors, also could function in structuring of heteromyid communities (Randall, 1993). Closely related species often exhibit distinctly different behavior patterns (Banks et al., 1979; Cranford and Derting, 1983; Dempster et ai., 1992; Gouat et al., 1996; Livoreil et ai., 1993; Wolff et ai., 1983), and some species are naturally tolerant, rather than aggressive (Gouat et ai., 1996; Livoreil et ai., 1993). We chose to study behavioral mechanisms of coexistence of Dipodomys ordii and D. merriami for several reasons. First, these solitary, nocturnal rodents are sympatric over a large part of their geographic ranges in North America (Hall, 1981). They overlap in about one-third of the geographic range of D. ordii and about one-half of the geographic range of D. merriami in a variety of habitats (Brown, 1973; M'Closkey, 1978; Rosenzweig and Winakur, 1969). Second, population fluctuations of D. ordii and D. merriami are correlated positively (Brown and Heske, 1990a, 1990b; Brown and Zeng, 1989; Heske et ai., 1994). Because both species may experience high population densities at the same time, competition should increase in regions of sympatry. Both species also are similar in body size with D. ordii slightly larger than D. merriami. This ratio of body sizes is < 1.5 (ca. 1.15), which is the ratio of body sizes of desert granivores that is considered necessary for coexistence (Bowers and Brown, 1982; Brown and Harney, 1993). Finally, in addition to similar morphology and use of habitat, D. ordii and D. merriami exhibit extensive intraspecific and interspecific overlap in home ranges (Schroder, 1987). Schroder (1987) proposed that the ecological similarity between D. ordii and D. merriami may result in neither species having a clear competitive advantage. Intraspecific densities would be kept below saturation to permit the two species to coexist in habitats different from the habitat occupied by either species alone. Because aggressive interference would be ineffective and costly, other behavioral mechanisms of coexistence should evolve. To understand behaviors that mediate coexistence of D. merriami and D. ordii, we conducted studies in the laboratory and field. We measured home-range overlap with radiotelemetry, made behavioral observations in the field, and conducted olfactory preference tests and behavioral encounters in the laboratory. We investigated interspecific and intraspecific olfactory communication because chemical cues have been found to maintain separation between sympatric species of desert rodents (Haim and Rozenfeld, 1993;

3 November 1999 PERRI AND RANDALL-BEHAVIOR OF KANGAROO RATS 1299 Johnston and Robinson, 1993). We tested species recognition through a comparison of responses to heterospecific odors from sebaceous glands of D. ordii and D. merriami. We conducted intraspecific test of gender recognition and familiarity in D. ordii to compare with a previous study of D. merriami (Randall, 1981). Finally, we compared behavioral interactions of D. ordii and D. merriami in paired encounters in an arena sufficiently large to allow avoidance behavior (Ostfeld,1985). MATERIALS AND METHODS Radiotracking and observations.-we monitored movements and observed behavior of D. ordii and D. merriami on a 200-by-240-m study site 7 kin NE of Portal, Cochise Co., Arizona, 1 July-12 August 1988 and 2-26 July 1989 (Randall, 1984). D. merriami and D. ordii occupied part of the site (ca. 200 by 140 m), while a more open area was occupied by D. spectabilis. Numbered stakes positioned at 15-m intervals served as trapping stations and for radiotracking coordinates. Resident kangaroo rats were trapped with Sherman live traps and marked with numbered eartags covered with color-coded reflectant tape for individual recognition at night. We trapped animals regularly to census and weigh them, attach ear tags, and check reproductive condition. In 1988, we trapped and marked 24 D. merriami (15 males, 9 females) and 38 D. ordii (13 males, 25 females). D. merriami had an average weight of 44.3 ± 1.10 g (males 48.9 ± 0.98, females 41.9± 1.29 g) and D. ordii 50.1 ± 1.4 g (males 51.4 ± 1.03, females 49.4 ± 1.68 g) for a body-size ratio of We implanted transmitters in 17 D. merriami (10 males, 7 females) and 11 D. ordii (5 males, 6 females). A sharp decline occurred in the population in 1989, and we trapped 7 D. merriami (5 males, 2 females) and 10 D. ordii (2 males, 8 females). Transmitters were implanted into three males and one female D. merriami and two males and four females of D. ordii. We implanted mouse-style transmitters with an internal antenna and individually tuned frequencies (AVM Instrument Inc., Livermore, CA) in animals captured on the study site and transported to the laboratory at the Southwestern Research Station. We anesthetized kangaroo rats with a light dosage of sodium pentobarbital (0.02 cm3 /40 g body weight) and implanted the 3-g transmitter-battery unit, encapsulated in beeswax, subcutaneously and lateral to the dorsal midline. Animals were allowed to recover and released at the point of capture the following night. We located kangaroo rats by approaching them on foot with a 12-channel receiver and a hand-held Yagi antenna. We located individuals at I-h intervals for 6-8 h/night, with day burrows located before sunset, and recorded coordinates of locations from the trapping grid by speaking into a hand-held tape recorder. In 1988, we tracked 12 D. merriami a minimum of 10 and a maximum of 40 nights and 11 D. ordii from 7 to 28 nights. Insufficient data were collected from other rats with transmitters because of failure of transmitters or disappearance or death of the animal. In 1989, we tracked four D. merriami (3 males, 1 female) and six D. ordii (2 males, 4 females) an average of 14.2 nights. We also made 62 h of focal-animal observations in We located a kangaroo rat via the radio signal and then followed it until it went into a burrow or disappeared. We recorded interactions with other kangaroo rats into a handheld tape recorder, including chases, distance foraging apart, and nonaggressive contact between any part of the animals' bodies. We estimated home ranges with the minimum-convex-polygon (MCP) and adaptive-kernel (AK) methods in the CALHOME program (Kie et al., 1996; see Gehrt and Fritzell, 1997, for a discussion of the advantages and disadvantages of MCP and AK methods). We considered the MCP a better representation of home ranges of kangaroo rats than AK and used it in our calculations and illustrations of overlap of home ranges. We calculated home ranges with a 95% distribution of data. We analyzed the following number of radiotracking locations for individuals of each species and sex in 1988: ex = 217) for male D. merriami (n = 6), (X = 119.5) for female D. merriami (n = 6), (X = 95) for male D. ordii (n = 5), (X = 180.4) for female D. ordii (n = 6). In 1989, we analyzed locations (X = 87) for four D. merriami and (X = 100.8) for six D. ordii. We measured overlap between home ranges by graphing the CALHOME output using Lotus, Freelance Graphics 96. From data points in Lo-

4 1300 JOURNAL OF MAMMALOGY Vol. 80, No.4 tus, we generated overlapping polygons for each individual we wanted to measure and used Scion ImagePC (NIH Image for Macintosh by Scion Corporation available at to create additional polygons where home ranges overlapped. We calculated from the polygons of each individual kangaroo rat the percentage of non-overlapping area (the area occupied exclusively by that individual) and the percentage of area that overlapped with other individuals. Olfactory tests.-we collected 12 D. ordii and 21 D. merriami for our laboratory studies during 4 weeks of trapping in 1991 and added five and seven more animals, respectively, during 3 weeks of trapping in July We limited our trapping to the area southeast of Portal, Cochise Co., Arizona, where both D. ordii and D. merriami lived sympatrically and which was near the area where our radiotracking data were obtained. We transported animals by automobile to the laboratory at San Francisco State University where they were housed on 4-5 cm of sand in individual plastic cages (26 by 16 by 13 cm) with wire lids and a tin can burrow. They were kept in a windowless room under a 14L: lod cycle and provided with wild-birdseed mix and leaves of lettuce on alternate days. All animals were reproductively mature; males had descended testes and females exhibited recurrent swelling of the vaginal area as an indication of estrus. In the interspecific olfactory tests, we tested seven males and seven females of each species in October 1992 for their responses to body oils, presented on cotton swabs, of same-sex conspecifics and heterospecifics in a counterbalanced order. Body oils were a mixture of secretions from the non-specialized sebaceous glands associated with hair and a specialized sebaceous gland located on the dorsum. Sebum from this gland forms a gradient down the sides of the animal (Westerhaus, 1983). We determined a dorsal gland to be functional by presence of waxy sebaceous secretions (Quay, 1953). Both male and female D. ordii had functional dorsal glands, but because the dorsal gland in D. merriami is sexually dimorphic (Quay, 1953), only males had functional glands (Lepri and Randall, 1983). We collected body oils from donors by slowly and gently wiping a cotton swab, three times, along each side of the animal. Because sandbathing removes excess body oils, we deprived animals of sand for h in empty plastic cages (15 by 8 by 7 cm) with wire lids to ensure sufficient amounts of oil for all olfactory tests. We tested animals in a circular aluminum arena (90 cm in diameter and 60 cm high) on ca. 3 cm of sand. The arena had removable sides to facilitate easy removal of sand and cleaning of the bottom of the arena with 95% alcohol between tests to remove odors. We imprinted two 17 -cm diameter circles in the test arena by pressing a circular plastic container into the sand on opposite sides of the arena 7 cm from the arena's edge. Into the center of one circle we placed a 2.5-cm long cotton swab with the body oil stimulus upright in the sand and in the other circle we placed an identical swab with no scent as a control. We transported animals between their cages and the test arena within their tin-can burrows and placed the can within the arena. Immediately after the animal vacated the can, we removed it and began to record the amount of time sniffing and approaching the swabs (tabulated as time investigating) within the imprinted circle for a 5-min test (Randall, 1991). We also tabulated number of times kangaroo rats entered each circle (approach) and sandbathing frequencies. Testing began ca. 2 h into the animal's dark cycle under a red light placed directly above the arena. All tests were conducted blind, so we were unaware of the gender and species of the test animal, stimulus presented and position of the stimulus. We controlled for position effects by changing position of stimuli within the arena (north, south, east, or west) and position of the observer relative to the stimuli. Because of asynchronous estrous cycles between the two species, we were unable to control totally for estrous stages. One female of each species was in an estrous condition during 50% of testing, while all remaining females were in a non-estrous condition throughout the testing period. Of the 28 animals tested, 9 D. ordii and 12 D. merriami had been tested previously in the arena in a sandbathing study. We habituated seven kangaroo rats not previously used in a test to the arena for 5 min. Number of animals tested differed each day because we used some test animals as scent donors. We tested 6-8 animals (at least one female and one male of each species) each day. Because of the testing schedule, ca. 75% of animals were tested prior to donating scent and 25% were tested after donating. Over a 4-day period, 20 animals (five of each species and of each sex) were

5 November 1999 PERRI AND RANDALL-BEHAVIOR OF KANGAROO RATS 1301 tested first and became donors the following day (24-h after testing), while eight animals (two of each species and of each sex) donated first and were tested h after donating. The above sequence was repeated over the following 4-day period to complete the second test for each animal. Each animal was tested twice and used twice as a scent donor. We conducted intraspecific olfactory tests 1-15 January We tested gender recognition in D. ordii to compare with D. merriami (Randall, 1981) by testing responses of D. ordii to body oils of opposite-sex conspecifics. Testing procedures and measured responses were similar to the interspecific test above, except we chose to control for estrous condition, instead of controlling the schedule of testing, because stimuli were opposite-sex individuals. Also, each individual was tested only once with one stimulus. Testing schedule depended upon the time at which females entered a non-estrous condition. Forty-eight hours after a female entered a nonestrous condition (indicated by a closed non-perforate vagina), we either tested her or deprived her of sand to become a scent donor. To test for familiarity, we tested seven nonestrous females and seven sexually mature males for their choice of scent of familiar and unfamiliar opposite-sex conspecifics. Test procedures and measured responses were similar to those in the previous swab investigations, except that both cotton swabs contained body oils. To establish familiarity, male and female pairs cohabited in cages (26 by 16 by 13 cm with wire lids) with a Plexiglas barrier that had 15 5-mm diameter holes drilled to allow exchange of odor and soil containing body oils. The seven pairs lived together for 6 weeks before testing during which time each female completed one to two complete estrous cycles. No females, however, exhibited estrus at the time of testing 5-11 March All animals had prior experience in the test arena. During the test, animals had a choice between cotton swabs with the body oils of their familiar cage-mate and body oils of an unfamiliar conspecific. Each animal donated scent once as a familiar cage-mate and once as an unfamiliar cage-mate. We separated scent donors from their cage-mates and deprived them of sand for 48 h before donating scent. After the 1st testing day, we housed scent donors and test animals together for 24 h before separating cage-mates again to deprive the second set of scent donors of sand for 48 h. Paired encounters.-we conducted paired encounters between same-sex pairs in the laboratory to detect differences in aggression and ability of one species to displace the other. We paired 12 D. ordii (six males, six females) with each other and with 10 D. merriami that were used as stimulus animals. Two D. merriami were paired twice. Because data for conspecifics paired together were not independent, we met assumptions for independence by averaging (total score of each behavior in the encounter divided by two) scores of behavior tabulated in the six conspecific encounters and the two scores of the same two animals in interactions with heterospecifics. We combined data for males and females and analyzed the averages of the six pairs with a Wilcoxin-sign rank test. We video-taped behavior (Table 1) during 15- min encounters with a camera (8 mm Sony EVe-X7 video camcorder) mounted directly above the same arena used in olfactory investigations. We placed one individual of a designated pair on each side of a removable cardboard barrier in the arena, removed the barrier, and began the test. At the end of the 15-min test, we separated individuals by placing the barrier between them. We distinguished individuals in paired encounters from a mark with a water-soluble marker on the mid-dorsal surface just posterior to the head. The same body area of the other individual also was marked with a moist cotton swab, ca. 4 h prior to testing. We paired animals that differed by <5 g and tested each animal on two separate days, 48 h apart. We tested one to six female D. ordii over 5 testing days and one to five male D. ordii males over 7 days. The schedule for males differed from the schedule for females because we used two male D. merriami twice as heterospecific stimuli, and we spaced encounters of these males 48 h apart. For reasons similar to those given for the previous investigations, we were unable to control completely estrous condition in females. One female of each species was in estrus, while all remaining females were in a non-estrous condition throughout testing March Because all but one D. merriami had no experience in the arena and 1 year had lapsed since all D. ordii had experience in the arena, we gave each animal 5 min alone in the arena on sand,

6 1302 JOURNAL OF MAMMALOGY Vol. 80, No.4 TABLE i.-descriptions of behaviors tabulated for Dipodomys ordii in paired encounters with D. ordii and D. merriami. Behavior Approach other Approached by other Approached each other Displaced other Displaced by other Not displaced Initiates nasal contact Allows nasal contact Initiates physical contact Sandbathing behavior 24 h prior to testing, to habituate them to the arena and video camera. We analyzed the video tapes frame by frame. We copied the 8 mm video tapes to VHS with running time burned into a window dub (accurate to 1130 s) and viewed tapes using a Sony i9-inch television monitor and a ProScan PSVR8i VHS player with jog-and-shuttle control. For time measurements, we recorded frame times at the beginning and end of measured behaviors (Table i). We then converted these frames into real time. We used SYSTAT (Wil- 250, , 250 Description The test animal decreases distance between itself and the other animal in any noncontinuous decrease in distance (pause of 2/30 s counts as a separate approach). The other animal decreases distance between itself and the test animal. Distance between both animals is decreased simultaneously. The test animal decreases distance between itself and the other animal as the other animal increases distance between itself and the test animal. The other animal decreases distance between itself and the test animal as the test animal increases distance between itself and the other animal. The other animal decreases distance between itself and the test animal, but the test animal does not increase distance. The test animal moves its rostrum to within I cm of the other animal. The test animal does not increase distance while the other animal initiates nasal contact. Same as nasal contact except uses other parts of body. As described by Randall (1981). 200 ' B 50- i;..;.., '.:':> '....' U FIG. i.-distribution of home ranges of A) Dipodomys merriami and B) D. ordii and individual symbols of day burrows on the same isoby-2s0-m area. Males are represented by dashed lines and closed symbols; females are solid lines with open symbols. kinson, i990) for all statistical analyses as specified. RESULTS Spacing.-We found considerable overlap in home ranges between the two species in 1988 (Fig. 1). Home ranges of D. ordii overlapped those of D. merriami by 36.4%, and D. merriami overlapped D. ordii by 49.5%. The true overlap probably was more extensive than our data show because only a portion of kangaroo rats on the study site had radiotransmitters. Both species typically used individual burrows during the day, and many individuals had more than one day burrow in the home area. Entrances of day burrows of the two species could be in proximity, and we found kangaroo rats using day burrows ca. 1 m apart on opposite ends of the same clump of bushes. Kangaroo rats, however, seemed to place their day burrows in areas of their home ranges that overlapped least with other kangaroo rats (Fig. 1). Both species showed gender-specific patterns of home-range overlap in intraspecific spacing. Conspecific females tended to have exclusive home ranges, while males overlapped females and other males (Fig. 1; Table 2). Home ranges of females over-

7 November /999 PERRI AND RANDALL-BEHAVIOR OF KANGAROO RATS 1303 TABLE 2.-Percentage of overlap in home ranges of Dipodomys ordii and D. merriami with conspecifics in high (1988) and low (1989) population densities. Interaction Year Species Female-female 1988 D.ordii 3.14 D. merriami D.ordii 0 D. merriami... c: (1) () en C> c: cts C> en (1) > c: (1) E I Male Female Male Female Conspecific Heterospecific FIG. 2.-Time investigating ex ::!:: SEs) cotton swabs with conspecific and heterospecific scent from same-sex animals and blank swabs with no scent as a control by male and female A) Dipodomys ordii and B) D. merriami during 5-min tests (n = 7 for each sex and species). Male-male Male on female Female on male lapped <10%; males overlapped 10-20%. Conspecifics of the opposite sex overlapped most with 50% overlap of male D. merriami on home ranges of females of D. mer- A riami (Table 2). We found no significant difference in size of home ranges. Male D. merriami, however, tended to have larger home ranges than either female D. merriami or D. ordii in 1988 (two-way ANOVA, species and sex interaction: F = 3.77; d.f. = 1,19; P = 0.067). Home ranges of male D. merriami averaged 1,671.7 ± m 2 compared to ± m 2 for female D. merriami, ± m 2 for male D. ordii, and ± m 2 for female D. ordii. Population densities in 1989 were lower than in 1988, and there was less overlap among kangaroo rats (Table 2). Despite the lower population density, neither species exhibited expanded home ranges. Sizes of home ranges of male and female D. ordii were similar, and the home ranges of six animals averaged ± 58.6 m 2 Home ranges of D. merriami averaged 1,302.0 ± m 2 The female D. merriami had a home range of m 2, and the three males averaged 1,638.7 ± 1,030.5 m 2 Olfactory tests.-d. ordii and D. merriami spent about the same amount of time investigating the cotton swab with body oils of heterospecifics as they spent investigating the control swab with no scent (Fig. 2A and 2B). There was no significant difference in time spent investigating or frequency of approaches toward scent of heterospecifics and the control by males and females of either species.

8 1304 JOURNAL OF MAMMALOGY Vol. 80, No.4 -c: Q).. 0 (/) 0).. c: CtI 0) (/) Q) > c: Q) E i= Male Female Scent D Blank Fig. 3.-Time investigating ex ::!:: SEs) opposite-sex body oils and a no scent control (blank) by female (n = 7) and male (n = 7) Dipodomys ordii during 5-min tests. Females of both species, however, responded to scent of same-sex conspecifics (Fig. 2). Female D. ordii spent more time investigating the swab with body oils of conspecific females than the control with no scent (paired t = 2.44, d.! = 6, P = 0.051; Fig. 2A), and they approached the scent of female D. ordii at higher frequencies (8.86 ± 2.1) than they approached the control (5.14 ± 1.03; t = 2.52, dj = 6, P = 0.045). Similarly, female D. merriami spent more time investigating the scent of female D. merriami compared with the control (t = 2.67, dj = 6, P = 0.037; Fig. 2B), but they did not approach the scent of female D. merriami at higher frequencies than they approached the control. In contrast to females, males exhibited no preference for scent of conspecific males. Male D. ordii and D. merriami spent equal time investigating and approaching scented and unscented cotton swabs (Fig. 2A and 2B). Female D. ordii exhibited a preference for scent of male D. ordii and spent significantly more time investigating body oils from males than the control (t = 3.37, dj = 5, P = 0.02; Fig. 3). Males, however, spent equal time investigating scent of fe- 1: 10 VJ C) 8 c:: ; 6.5 Q) E i= VJ o 10 0 co e c.. 8 c.. co -6 0 c:: Q) 4 :l Q" 2 u.. Familiar scent DUnfamiliar scent 12r o Male Female FIG. 4.-A) Time investigating ex ::!:: SEs) and B) number of approaches to scent of familiar and unfamiliar opposite-sex donors by male (n = 7) and female (n = 7) Dipodomys ordii during 5-min choice tests. males compared to the control (Fig. 3). Neither females nor males approached opposite-sex scent (8.86 ± 1.72 times and 6.71 ± 1.97 times, respectively) significantly more than the control (6.47 ± 2.01 times and 5.71 ± 1.13 times, respectively). Female D. ordii preferred scent of familiar males to unfamiliar males and spent more time investigating scent of their familiar, male cage-mates compared with unfamiliar males (t = 4.12, dj = 6, P = 0.006; Fig. 4A). Females approached scent of familiar males at higher frequencies than the scent of unfamiliar males (4.43 ± 1.13, t = 4.56, d.! = 6, P = 0.004; Fig. 4B). A B

9 November 1999 PERRI AND RANDALL-BEHAVIOR OF KANGAROO RATS 1305 TABLE 3.-Frequency of behavior of D. ordii in 5-min paired encounter with Dipodomys ordii and with D. merriami (n = 6 pairs; see Table 1 for definitions of behavior). Encounters Behavior Frequency of approach To other 62.4 By other To each other 8.01 Frequency to displace Other By other Frequency not displaced "P < X D.ordii Male D. ordii, however, showed no preference for scent of familiar females. There was no significant difference in time spent investigating or in frequency of approaches toward familiar and unfamiliar scent of females by males (Fig. 4A and 4B). Behavioral interactions.-in general, D. ordii interacted more with conspecifics than with D. merriami and was approached and displaced by conspecifics more than by D. merriami. (Table 3). Conspecifics approached D. ordii significantly more than they were approached by D. merriami (Wilcoxin sign-rank test, Z = 0.046), and D. ordii was displaced by conspecifics significantly more times than displaced by D. merriami (z = 0.031). D. ordii displaced D. merriami twice as often as D. merriami displaced D. ordii, but that comparison was not significant (z = 0.37, Table 3). No other behavioral comparisons differed significantly. Quick and non-injurious aggressive contact between individuals occurred in 25% of encounters. Two male D. ordii initiated physical contact six times with D. merriami. Two female D. ordii initiated physical contact with conspecifics 9 times and heterospecifics 31 times. Nasal contact and sandbathing occurred infrequently. We observed 11 interspecific interactions D. merriami SE X SE " " during focal-animal observation of D. ordii and D. merriami in the field D. ordii chased D. merriami in two interactions. However, D. merriami immediately returned after the brief chase to about the same location and began to forage. D. merriami ran away and avoided D. ordii without a chase in a third interaction. There was mutual tolerance in the remaining eight observations in which the two species foraged < 10m apart. In three cases, kangaroo rats were only ca. 1 m apart. Two interactions were observed between D. ordii. In one case, there was a chase, and in the other rats tolerated each other while foraging 1- m apart. In interactions between two D. merriami, two resulted in a chase, one in mutual tolerance and two in amicable contact. DISCUSSION Interference competition.-our radiotracking data and field observations revealed that D. ordii and D. merriami coexisted at high population densities in home ranges with considerable overlap. Our behavioral data suggest that avoidance, rather than interference, probably mediates coexistence of D. ordii and D. merriami. Although D. ordii dominated D. merriami in paired encounters, D. merriami displayed

10 1306 JOURNAL OF MAMMALOGY Vol. 80. No.4 an overall avoidance of D. ordii, and D. ordii was displaced by conspecifics significantly more than it was displaced by D. merriami. Agonistic behaviors always were initiated by D. ordii, but these occurred infrequently in both laboratory and field. This tolerance is in striking contrast to the agonistic behavior of D. ordii toward D. merriami observed by Blaustein and Risser (1976). We believe that our study, conducted in a larger arena than that used by Blaustein and Risser and coupled with observations in the field, reflects more accurately interspecific behavior between these species. If provided a large enough space, D. merriami usually retreats before any physical contract can occur between it and a dominant species (Congdon, 1974). Amount of agonistic behavior directed toward a competitor should reflect degree of true competition (MacArthur, 1972). Because individuals of a species have a greater impact on reproductive success of members of their own species than on members of another species, exploitation of resources should be reflected more in intraspecific interactions than in interspecific interactions. Aggression can be costly in time and energy, and it only should be employed if a resource is defensible and the benefit gained is greater than the cost. D. ordii, therefore, should not direct aggression toward D. merriami, which avoids them, but they should direct aggression toward conspecifics that do not avoid them and with which they are probably in more direct competition. Also, the slight dominance of D. ordii toward D. merriami in the laboratory may not result in competitive dominance under natural conditions in which individuals within their established home ranges exhibit site-specific dominance. For example, Peromyscus maniculatus dominates P. leucopus in the laboratory; but, in an area of sympatry where home ranges of species overlap, individuals dominated in core areas of their individual home ranges (Wolff et al., 1983). Distribution of day burrows on our study site suggests that kangaroo rats also may attempt to avoid the core area of other species. Even if interference interactions between the two species occurs, costly aggression and fighting are unnecessary to maintain separation. If individuals of both species coincide in time and space to use a similar resource, behavioral dominance of D. ordii may cause D. merriami to alter its pattern of resource use to avoid D. ordii, such as adopting a different pattern of seed hoarding (Jenkins and Breck, 1998). Encounters in the field often consisted of a brief chase and the immediate return of both animals to their respective foraging sites. In times of extreme limitation of resources, however, an increase in interspecific interactions as resources become scarce might occur (Kaufmann, 1983). Avoidance behavior could be a common component of species coexistence in deserts where animals increase home ranges and foraging efficiency rather than to increase aggressive interference (Dempster et al., 1992; Gouat et al., 1996). Olfaction and spacing.-we found no evidence that olfaction mediates interactions between D. ordii and D. merriami. Neither species responded to scent of the other species in our tests. Females of both species, however, responded to scent of female conspecifics, which they typically exclude from their home ranges, and they did not respond to scent of either heterospecific females or conspecific males, with which their home ranges overlap. Males also did not respond to scent of either conspecific or heterospecific males. In D. merriami (Behrends et al., 1986a, 1986b; 0' Farrell, 1980; Randall, 1989) and D. ordii (this study), spatial arrangement of conspecific neighbors exhibits a distinct gender-related pattern in which females occupy exclusive home ranges. Females may space themselves through olfactory information about the location of conspeific female neighbors. Males that do not respond to scent of other kangaroo rats share home ranges with them. Familiarity also may be an important el-

11 November 1999 PERRI AND RANDALL-BEHAVIOR OF KANGAROO RATS 1307 ement in social structure of D. ordii. The preference of female D. ordii for scent of familiar males suggests that (as in D. merriami-randall, 1991) olfactory cues may function in neighbor recognition. In both species, familiarity may play an important role, not only to space neighbors, but in mating interactions. In D. ordii (this investigation) and D. merriami (Randall, 1991), females preferred scent of familiar to unfamiliar males, whereas males did not discriminate the scent of either familiar or unfamiliar females. Female D. merriami allowed familiar, but not unfamiliar, males to contact them, while males attempted to contact both familiar and unfamiliar females equally (Randall, 1989). This suggests that familiarity is important to females, especially in allowing contact by males. Because mating occurs primarily between familiar individuals in D. merriami (Randall, 1989) and familiarity is established by repeated exposure between close neighbors, males may compete for preferred locations within proximity to conspecific females (Daly, 1977 ; DeVries et al., 1997). Community structure.-the community composition of D. ordii and D. merriami seems shaped by intraspecific spacing behavior and interspecific interactions in which the species avoid each other. Intraspecific interactions are probably more important in shaping heteromyid communities of D. ordii and D. merriami where they coexist than interspecific interactions. Intraspecific behaviors of recognition in D. ordii (this study) and D. merriami (Randall, 1991) may act to space conspecifics in similar ways. Spacing behavior has an important effect on ecological processes, such as regulation of population densities and use of resources (Brown and Orians, 1970; Gliwicz, 1988; Ims, 1988). Perhaps, intraspecific spacing behavior regulates populations of D. ordii and D. merriami and allows them to coexist ecologically. Perturbation experiments of sympatric D. ordii and D. merriami revealed a strong intraspecific response from contiguous populations of the two species of heteromyids (Schroder and Rosenzweig, 1975), which suggests that individuals of a species are in greater competition with conspecifics than closely related heterospecifics. D. ordii may be unable to exclude D. merriami from a sympatric area, even when densities appear high, as evidenced by coexistence of both species at high densities in the Portal area (Heske et al., 1994; Valone et al., 1995). The Portal area may represent a transitional habitat (Brown and Heske, 1990) that differs from habitats occupied by either species alone to cause the species to be more strongly influenced by intraspecific rather than interspecific interactions (Ayala, 1970). We suggest that D. ordii and D. merriami coexist in the Portal area because populations of each species are kept below saturation for shared resources. The successful increase of D. ordii on the study site without extermination of existing species implies that the original community was unsaturated (Pianka, 1988). D. ordii began to appear in low numbers on the Portal site in 1982, which had been occupied by only D. merriami and D. spectabilis since Densities on this site and a nearby area fluctuated in synchrony, peaked in 1988, and declined substantially in the following years (Brown and Heske, 1990a; Heske et ai., 1994). The two species continued to coexist, however, for 10 years without excluding each other on our study site and on another study site nearby (Heske et al., 1994). Because spacing behavior in female rodents may limit number of possible breeding territories and density of mature females in the population (Bondrup-Nielsen, 1986; Saitoh, 1981), space available for home ranges of females also may limit Dipodomys to cause the populations of each species to exist below the density that the habitat is capable of supporting and below the density where competitive interactions between species normally operate. Schroder (1987) suggested that the ecological similarity between D. ordii and D. merriami may result in neither species having a clear competitive advan-

12 1308 JOURNAL OF MAMMALOGY Vol. 80. No.4 tage. Fluctuations of immigration from and emigration to allopatric populations, predation, and disease may strongly influence interactions to keep intraspecific densities below saturation and permit coexistence in a habitat that differs from the habitat occupied by either species alone, even if species use similar resources. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We are grateful to all the students who helped with radiotracking and trapping, especially J. Shore and M. B. Stone, and for support of the staff at the Southwestern Research Station. We especially thank B. Robbins and R. Larson for their support and assistance and to them, S. Jenkins, M. Price and an anonymous reviewer for a critical review of the manuscript. J. A. Randall appreciates the continued support of her research on desert rodents by the National Science Foundation and the National Geographic Society. LITERATURE CITED AYALA, F Competition, coexistence and evolution. Pp , in Essays in evolution and genetics (M. K. Hecht and W. D. Steere, eds.). Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York. BANKS E. M., U. W. HUCK, AND N. J. MANKOVICH Interspecific aggression in captive male lemmings. Animal Behaviour, 27: BASSET, A Body size-related coexistence: an approach through allometric constraints on homerange use. Ecology, 76: BEHRENDS, P., M. DALY, AND M. I. WILSON. 1986a. Range use patterns and spatial relationships of Merriam's kangaroo rats (Dipodomys merriami). Behaviour, 96: b. Above-ground activity of Merriam's kangaroo rats (Dipodomys merriami) in relation to sex and reproduction. Behaviour, 96: BLAUSTEIN, A R., AND A C. RISSER Interspecific interactions between three sympatric species of kangaroo rats (Dipodomys). Animal Behaviour, 24: BONDRUP-NIELSEN, S Investigation of spacing behaviour of Clethrionomys gapperi by experimentation. The Journal of Animal Ecology, 55: BOWERS, M.A Foraging behavior of heteromyid rodents: field evidence of resource partitioning. Journal of Mammalogy, 63: BOWERS, M. A, AND J. H. BROWN Body size and coexistence in desert rodents: chance or community structure? Ecology, 63: BROWN, J. H Species diversity of seed-eating desert rodents in sand dune habitats. Ecology, 54: BROWN, J. H., AND B. A. HARNEY Population and community ecology of heteromyid rodents in temperate habitats. pp , in Biology of the Heteromydae (H. H. Genoways and J. H. Brown, eds.). Special Publication, The American Society of Marnmalogists, 10: BROWN, J. H., AND E. J. HESKE. 1990a. Temporal changes in a Chihuahuan Desert rodent community. Oikos, 59: b. Control of a desert-grassland transition by a keystone rodent guild. Science, 250: BROWN, J. H., AND G. A. LIEBERMAN Resource utilization and coexistence of seed-eating desert rodents in sand dune habitats. Ecology, 54: BROWN, J. H., AND J. C. MUNGER Experimental manipulation of a desert rodent community: food addition and species removal. Ecology, 66: BROWN, J. H., AND Z. ZENG Comparative population ecology of eleven species of rodents in the Chihuahuan Desert. Ecology, 70: BROWN, J. L., AND G. H. ORIANS Spacing patterns in mobile animals. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 1 : CONGDON, J Effect of habitat quality on distributions of three sympatric species of desert rodents. Journal of Marnmalogy, 55: CRANFORD, J. A, AND T. L. DERTING Intra- and interspecific behavior of Microtus pennsylvanicus and Microtus pinetorum. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 13:7-11. DALY, M Some experimental tests of the functional significance of scent-marking by gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus). Journal of Comparative Physiological Psychology, 91: DAYAN, T., AND D. SIMBERLOFF Morphological relationships among coexisting heteromyids: an incisive dental character. The American Naturalist, 143: DEMPSTER, E. R., R. DEMPSTER, AND M. R. PERRIN A comparative study of the behaviour of six taxa of male and female gerbils (Rodentia) in intra- and interspecific encounters. Ethology, 91: DEVRIES, A C., C. L. JOHNSON, AND S. C. CARTER Familiarity and gender influence social preferences in prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Canadian Journal of Zoology, 79: FRYE, R. J Experimental field evidence of interspecific aggression between two species of kangaroo rats (Dipodomys). Oecologia, 59: GEHRT, S. D., AND E. K. FRITZELL Sexual differences in home ranges of raccoons. Journal of Mammalogy, 78: GLIWICZ, J The role of dispersal in models of small rodent population dynamics. Oikos, 52: GOUAT, P., I. E. YAHYAOUI, V. MANDIER, B. LIVOREIL, AND C. BAUDOIN Social behaviour and the use of space in two sympatric ground squirrels, Spermophilus spp., from the Chihuahua desert. Pp. 9-20, in Fifth international conference rodens & spatium, biodiversity and adaptation (A Zaime, ed.). Acetes Editions, Rabat, Morroco. HAIM, A., AND F. M. ROZENFELD Temporal

13 November 1999 PERRI AND RANDALL-BEHAVIOR OF KANGAROO RATS 1309 segregation in coexisting Acomys species: the role of odour. Physiology and Behavior, 54: HALL, E. R The mammals of North America. Second ed. John Wiley & Sons, New York, 1: HESKE, E. J., J. H. BROWN, AND S. MISTRY Longterm experimental study of a Chihuahuan desert rodent community: 13 years of competition. Ecology, 75: IMS, R. A Spatial clumping of sexually receptive females induces space sharing among male voles. Nature, 335: JENKINS, S. H., AND S. W. BECK Differences in food hoarding among six species of heteromyid rodents. Journal of Mammalogy, 79: JOHNSTON, R. E., AND T. A. ROBINSON Crossspecies discrimination of individual odors by hamsters (Muridae: Mesocricetus auratus, Phodopus campbelli). Ethology, 94: KAUFMANN, J. H On the definitions and functions of dominance and territoriality. Biological Review, 58:1-20. KENAGY, G. J Daily and seasonal patterns of activity and energetics in a heteromyid rodent community. Ecology, 54: KIE, J. G., J. A. BALDWIN, AND C. J. EVANS CALHOME: a program for estimating animal home ranges. Wildlife Society Bulletin, 24: KOTLER, B. P., J. S. BROWN, AND W. A. MITCHELL The role of predation in shaping the behaviour, morphology and community organisation of desert rodents. Australian Journal of Zoology, 42: LEMEN, C. A Seed size selection in heteromyids: a second look. Oecologia, 35: LEPRI, J. J., AND J. A. RANDALL Hormonal regulation of sandbathing in male kangaroo rats (Dipodomys merriami). Behavioral Neural Biology, 37: LIVOREIL, B., P. GOUAT, AND C. BAUDOIN A comparative study of social behaviour of two sympatric ground squirrels (Spermophilus spilosoma, S. mexicanus). Ethology, 93: MACARTHUR, R. H Geographical ecology: patterns in the distribution of species. Harper & Row, New York. M'CLOSKEY, R. T Community structure in sympatric rodents. Ecology, 57: Niche separation and assembly in four species of Sonoran desert rodents. The American Naturalist, 112: O'FARRELL, M. J Seasonal activity patterns of rodents in a sagebrush community. Journal of Mammalogy, 55: Spatial relationships of rodents in a sagebrush community. Journal of Mammalogy, 61: OSTFELD, R. S Limiting resources and territoriality in microtine rodents. The American Naturalist. 126:1-15. PIANKA, E. R Evolutionary ecology. Harper and Row, New York. PRICE, M. V The role of microhabitat in structuring desert rodent communities. Ecology, 59: Laboratory studies of seed size and seed species selection by heteromyid rodents. Oecologia, 60: PRICE, M. V., AND K. M. HEINZ Effects of body size, seed density, and soil characteristics on rates of seed harvest by heteromyid rodents. Oecologia, 61 : PRICE, M. v., AND O. J. REICHMAN Distribution of seeds in Sonoran desert soils: implications for heteromyid rodent foraging. Ecology, 68: QUAY, W. B Seasonal and sexual differences in the dorsal skin gland of the kangaroo rat (Dipodomys). Journal of Mammalogy, 34:1-14. RANDALL, J. A Olfactory communication at sandbathing loci by sympatric species of kangaroo rats. Journal of Mammalogy, 62: Territorial defense and advertisement by footdrumming in bannertail kangaroo rats (Dipodomys spectabilis) at high and low population densities. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 16: Neighbor recognition in a solitary desert rodent Dipodomys merriami. Ethology, 81: Sandbathing to establish familiarity in the Merriam's kangaroo rat, Dipodomys merriami. Animal Behaviour, 41: Behavioural adaptations of desert rodents (Heteromyidae). Animal Behaviour, 45: REICHMAN, O. J Behavior of desert heteromyids. Great Basin Naturalist Memoirs, 7: REICHMAN, O. J., AND D. OBERSTEIN Selection of seed distribution types by Dipodomys merriami and Perognathus amplus. Ecology, 58: REICHMAN, O. J., AND M. V. PRICE Ecological aspects of heteromyid foraging, pp , in Biology of the Heteromydae (H. H. Genoways and J. H. Brown, eds.). Special Publication, The American Society of Mammalogists, 10: ROSENZWEIG, M. L., AND J. WINAKUR Population ecology of desert rodent communities: habitats and environmental complexity. Ecology, 50: SAITOH, T Control of female maturation in high density populations of the red-backed vole Clethrionomys rufocanus bedfordiae. The Journal of Animal Ecology, 50: SCHRODER, G. D Mechanisms for coexistence among three species of Dipodomys: habitat selection and an alternative. Ecology, 68: SCHRODER, G. D., AND M. L. ROSENZWEIG Perturbation analysis of competition and overlap in habitat utilization between Dipodomys ordii and Dipodomys merriami. Oecologia, 19:9-28. VALONE, T. J., J. H. BROWN, AND C. L. JACOBI Catastrophic decline of a desert rodent, Dipodomys spectabilis: insights from a long-term study. Journal of Mammalogy, 76: WESTERHAUS, M. D A histological comparison of the dorsal and generalized holocrine skin glands

REVISION: POPULATION ECOLOGY 18 SEPTEMBER 2013

REVISION: POPULATION ECOLOGY 18 SEPTEMBER 2013 REVISION: POPULATION ECOLOGY 18 SEPTEMBER 2013 Lesson Description In this lesson we: Revise population ecology by working through some exam questions. Key Concepts Definition of Population A population

More information

Population Ecology. Study of populations in relation to the environment. Increase population size= endangered species

Population Ecology. Study of populations in relation to the environment. Increase population size= endangered species Population Basics Population Ecology Study of populations in relation to the environment Purpose: Increase population size= endangered species Decrease population size = pests, invasive species Maintain

More information

Effect of Species 2 on Species 1 Competition - - Predator-Prey + - Parasite-Host + -

Effect of Species 2 on Species 1 Competition - - Predator-Prey + - Parasite-Host + - Community Ecology Community - a group of organisms, of different species, living in the same area Community ecology is the study of the interactions between species The presence of one species may affect

More information

Community Ecology. Classification of types of interspecific interactions: Effect of Species 1 on Species 2

Community Ecology. Classification of types of interspecific interactions: Effect of Species 1 on Species 2 Community Ecology Community - a group of organisms, of different species, living in the same area Community ecology is the study of the interactions between species The presence of one species may affect

More information

Size and overlap of home range in a high density population of the Japanese field vole Microtus montebelli

Size and overlap of home range in a high density population of the Japanese field vole Microtus montebelli Acta Theriologica 40 (3): 249-256, 1995. PL ISSN 0001-7051 Size and overlap of home range in a high density population of the Japanese field vole Microtus montebelli Kohtaro URAYAMA Urayama K. 1995. Size

More information

Imprinting and kin recognition

Imprinting and kin recognition Imprinting and kin recognition Imprinting Konrad Lorenz Filial imprinting Critical period Sensitive period Experimental approaches Hours after hatching precocial altricial Filial imprinting Multiple cues

More information

Assessment Schedule 2016 Biology: Demonstrate understanding of the responses of plants and animals to their external environment (91603)

Assessment Schedule 2016 Biology: Demonstrate understanding of the responses of plants and animals to their external environment (91603) NCEA Level 3 Biology (91603) 2016 page 1 of 6 Assessment Schedule 2016 Biology: Demonstrate understanding of the responses of plants and animals to their external environment (91603) Evidence Statement

More information

The Problem of Where to Live

The Problem of Where to Live April 5: Habitat Selection: Intro The Problem of Where to Live Physical and biotic environment critically affects fitness An animal's needs may be met only in certain habitats, which should select for

More information

Additional Case Study: Calculating the Size of a Small Mammal Population

Additional Case Study: Calculating the Size of a Small Mammal Population Student Worksheet LSM 14.1-2 Additional Case Study: Calculating the Size of a Small Mammal Population Objective To use field study data on shrew populations to examine the characteristics of a natural

More information

EFFECTS OF PRIOR POPULATION DENSITY ON USE OF SPACE BY MEADOW VOLES, MICROTUS PENNSYLVANICUS

EFFECTS OF PRIOR POPULATION DENSITY ON USE OF SPACE BY MEADOW VOLES, MICROTUS PENNSYLVANICUS EFFECTS OF PRIOR POPULATION DENSITY ON USE OF SPACE BY MEADOW VOLES, MICROTUS PENNSYLVANICUS STEPHEN R. PUGH AND RICHARD S. OSTFELD University of New Hampshire at Manchester, 220 Hackett Hill Road, Manchester,

More information

PATH ANALYSIS: A CRITICAL EVALUATION USING LONG-TERM EXPERIMENTAL DATA

PATH ANALYSIS: A CRITICAL EVALUATION USING LONG-TERM EXPERIMENTAL DATA Vol. 149, No. 1 The American Naturalist January 1997 PATH ANALYSIS: A CRITICAL EVALUATION USING LONG-TERM EXPERIMENTAL DATA Department of Biology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, New Mexico 87131

More information

Unit 8: Ecology Guided Reading Questions (60 pts total)

Unit 8: Ecology Guided Reading Questions (60 pts total) AP Biology Biology, Campbell and Reece, 10th Edition Adapted from chapter reading guides originally created by Lynn Miriello Name: Unit 8: Ecology Guided Reading Questions (60 pts total) Chapter 51 Animal

More information

PECKIANA Volume 8 (2012) pp

PECKIANA Volume 8 (2012) pp PECKIANA Volume 8 (2012) pp. 203 207 ISSN 1618-1735 The reactions of the common dormouse (Muscardinus avellanarius) and the yellow-necked mouse (Apodemus flavicollis) to the odour of nest Abstract Arboreal

More information

What standard are we focusing on today?

What standard are we focusing on today? What standard are we focusing on today? Standard H.B.6 The student will demonstrate an understanding that ecosystems are complex, interactive systems that include both biological communities and physical

More information

Lesson Overview 4.2 Niches and Community Interactions

Lesson Overview 4.2 Niches and Community Interactions THINK ABOUT IT If you ask someone where an organism lives, that person might answer on a coral reef or in the desert. Lesson Overview 4.2 Niches and Community Interactions These answers give the environment

More information

Chapter Niches and Community Interactions

Chapter Niches and Community Interactions Chapter 4 4.2 Niches and Community Interactions Key Questions: 1) What is a niche? 2) How does competition shape communities? 3) How do predation and herbivory shape communites? 4) What are three primary

More information

Chapter 4 Ecosystems and Living Organisms

Chapter 4 Ecosystems and Living Organisms Chapter 4 Ecosystems and Living Organisms I. Evolution A. The cumulative genetic changes that occur in a population of organisms over time 1. Current theories proposed by Charles Darwin, a 19 th century

More information

Community Structure. Community An assemblage of all the populations interacting in an area

Community Structure. Community An assemblage of all the populations interacting in an area Community Structure Community An assemblage of all the populations interacting in an area Community Ecology The ecological community is the set of plant and animal species that occupy an area Questions

More information

FW662 Lecture 9 Immigration and Emigration 1. Lecture 9. Role of immigration and emigration in populations.

FW662 Lecture 9 Immigration and Emigration 1. Lecture 9. Role of immigration and emigration in populations. FW662 Lecture 9 Immigration and Emigration 1 Lecture 9. Role of immigration and emigration in populations. Reading: Sinclair, A. R. E. 1992. Do large mammals disperse like small mammals? Pages 229-242

More information

Community Interactions. Community An assemblage of all the populations interacting in an area

Community Interactions. Community An assemblage of all the populations interacting in an area Community Interactions Community An assemblage of all the populations interacting in an area Populations are affected by: Available living space habitat Resource Availability niche Species interactions

More information

Aggregations on larger scales. Metapopulation. Definition: A group of interconnected subpopulations Sources and Sinks

Aggregations on larger scales. Metapopulation. Definition: A group of interconnected subpopulations Sources and Sinks Aggregations on larger scales. Metapopulation Definition: A group of interconnected subpopulations Sources and Sinks Metapopulation - interconnected group of subpopulations sink source McKillup and McKillup

More information

Speciation and Patterns of Evolution

Speciation and Patterns of Evolution Speciation and Patterns of Evolution What is a species? Biologically, a species is defined as members of a population that can interbreed under natural conditions Different species are considered reproductively

More information

Exam begins on next page SCORE (to be filled in by grader) Multiple choice: 17 questions (34 points total); 2 points each x ==>

Exam begins on next page SCORE (to be filled in by grader) Multiple choice: 17 questions (34 points total); 2 points each x ==> Page 1 Biology 217: Ecology First Midterm Exam, Spring 2007 There should be 8 pages to this exam - take a moment and count them now. Put your name on the first page of the exam, and on each of the last

More information

IV. Natural Selection

IV. Natural Selection IV. Natural Selection A. Important points (1) Natural selection does not cause genetic changes in individuals (2) Change in allele frequency occurs in populations (3) Fitness!" Reproductive Success = survival

More information

14.1. Every organism has a habitat and a niche. A habitat differs from a niche. Interactions in Ecosystems CHAPTER 14.

14.1. Every organism has a habitat and a niche. A habitat differs from a niche. Interactions in Ecosystems CHAPTER 14. SECTION 14.1 HABITAT AND NICHE Study Guide KEY CONCEPT Every organism has a habitat and a niche. VOCABULARY habitat ecological niche competitive exclusion ecological equivalent A habitat differs from a

More information

The Effects of Varying Cache Depth and Mammalian Predator Scent on the Foraging Behavior of Peromyscus maniculatus

The Effects of Varying Cache Depth and Mammalian Predator Scent on the Foraging Behavior of Peromyscus maniculatus The Effects of Varying Cache Depth and Mammalian Predator Scent on the Foraging Behavior of Peromyscus maniculatus BIOS 35502: Practicum in Environmental Field Biology Chelsea Gyure Advisor: Dr. Michael

More information

Unit 6 Populations Dynamics

Unit 6 Populations Dynamics Unit 6 Populations Dynamics Define these 26 terms: Commensalism Habitat Herbivory Mutualism Niche Parasitism Predator Prey Resource Partitioning Symbiosis Age structure Population density Population distribution

More information

Chapter 5 Evolution of Biodiversity. Sunday, October 1, 17

Chapter 5 Evolution of Biodiversity. Sunday, October 1, 17 Chapter 5 Evolution of Biodiversity CHAPTER INTRO: The Dung of the Devil Read and Answer Questions Provided Module 14 The Biodiversity of Earth After reading this module you should be able to understand

More information

Oecologia. The effects of owl predation on the foraging behavior of heteromyid rodents

Oecologia. The effects of owl predation on the foraging behavior of heteromyid rodents Oecologia (1988) 76:408-415 Oecologia 9 Springer-Verlag 1988 The effects of owl predation on the foraging behavior of heteromyid rodents Joel S. Brown*, Burt P. Kotler**, Rosemary J. Smith***, and William

More information

Predation risk, unequal competitors and the ideal free distribution

Predation risk, unequal competitors and the ideal free distribution Evolutionary Ecology Research, 1999, 1: 389 409 Predation risk, unequal competitors and the ideal free distribution Tamara C. Grand* and Lawrence M. Dill Behavioural Ecology Research Group, Department

More information

What is behavior? What questions can we ask? Why study behavior? Evolutionary perspective. Innate behaviors 4/8/2016.

What is behavior? What questions can we ask? Why study behavior? Evolutionary perspective. Innate behaviors 4/8/2016. What is behavior? Animal Behavior Behavior everything an animal does & how it does it response to stimuli in its environment Innate (instinct) inherited automatic & consistent learned ability to learn

More information

BIOS 6150: Ecology Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences

BIOS 6150: Ecology Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences BIOS 6150: Ecology Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences Week 14: Roles of competition, predation & disturbance in community structure. Lecture summary: (A) Competition: Pattern vs process.

More information

Chapter 6 Reading Questions

Chapter 6 Reading Questions Chapter 6 Reading Questions 1. Fill in 5 key events in the re-establishment of the New England forest in the Opening Story: 1. Farmers begin leaving 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. Broadleaf forest reestablished 2.

More information

Computational Ecology Introduction to Ecological Science. Sonny Bleicher Ph.D.

Computational Ecology Introduction to Ecological Science. Sonny Bleicher Ph.D. Computational Ecology Introduction to Ecological Science Sonny Bleicher Ph.D. Ecos Logos Defining Ecology Interactions: Organisms: Plants Animals: Bacteria Fungi Invertebrates Vertebrates The physical

More information

BIO S380T Page 1 Summer 2005: Exam 2

BIO S380T Page 1 Summer 2005: Exam 2 BIO S380T Page 1 Part I: Definitions. [5 points for each term] For each term, provide a brief definition that also indicates why the term is important in ecology or evolutionary biology. Where I ve provided

More information

Environmental Influences on Adaptation

Environmental Influences on Adaptation Have you ever noticed how the way you feel sometimes mirrors the emotions of the people with whom you spend a lot of time? For example, when you re around happy people, do you tend to become happy? Since

More information

POPULATIONS and COMMUNITIES

POPULATIONS and COMMUNITIES POPULATIONS and COMMUNITIES Ecology is the study of organisms and the nonliving world they inhabit. Central to ecology is the complex set of interactions between organisms, both intraspecific (between

More information

Patterns of morphology and resource use in North American desert rodent communities

Patterns of morphology and resource use in North American desert rodent communities Great Basin Naturalist Memoirs Volume 7 Biology of Desert Rodents Article 8 8-1-1983 Patterns of morphology and resource use in North American desert rodent communities M. V. Price Department of Biology,

More information

Photoperiodic responsiveness in Mongolian gerbils. Department of Psychology, Shiga University, Hiratsu, Otsu

Photoperiodic responsiveness in Mongolian gerbils. Department of Psychology, Shiga University, Hiratsu, Otsu The Japanese Journal of Animal Psychology, 48, 2, 177-181 (1998) Short report Photoperiodic responsiveness in Mongolian gerbils TAKASHI USUI and NOBUO IBUKA1 Department of Psychology, Shiga University,

More information

Chapter 54: Community Ecology

Chapter 54: Community Ecology Name Period Concept 54.1 Community interactions are classified by whether they help, harm, or have no effect on the species involved. 1. What is a community? List six organisms that would be found in your

More information

Assessment Schedule 2013 Biology: Demonstrate understanding of the responses of plants and animals to their external environment (91603)

Assessment Schedule 2013 Biology: Demonstrate understanding of the responses of plants and animals to their external environment (91603) NCEA Level 3 Biology (91603) 2013 page 1 of 6 Assessment Schedule 2013 Biology: Demonstrate understanding of the responses of plants and animals to their external environment (91603) Assessment Criteria

More information

14.1. KEY CONCEPT Every organism has a habitat and a niche. 38 Reinforcement Unit 5 Resource Book

14.1. KEY CONCEPT Every organism has a habitat and a niche. 38 Reinforcement Unit 5 Resource Book 14.1 HABITAT AND NICHE KEY CONCEPT Every organism has a habitat and a niche. A habitat is all of the living and nonliving factors in the area where an organism lives. For example, the habitat of a frog

More information

Ecology Symbiotic Relationships

Ecology Symbiotic Relationships Ecology Symbiotic Relationships Overview of the Co-evolution and Relationships Exhibited Among Community Members What does Symbiosis mean? How do we define Symbiosis? Symbiosis in the broadest sense is

More information

4. is the rate at which a population of a given species will increase when no limits are placed on its rate of growth.

4. is the rate at which a population of a given species will increase when no limits are placed on its rate of growth. Population Ecology 1. Populations of mammals that live in colder climates tend to have shorter ears and limbs than populations of the same species in warm climates (coyotes are a good example of this).

More information

Tolerance. Tolerance. Tolerance 10/22/2010

Tolerance. Tolerance. Tolerance 10/22/2010 Section 4.2 Mrs. Michaelsen Tolerance Every species has its own range of tolerance: The ability to survive and reproduce under a range of environmental circumstances. Tolerance Stress can result when an

More information

SPATIAL SCALE DEPENDENCE OF RODENT HABITAT USE

SPATIAL SCALE DEPENDENCE OF RODENT HABITAT USE SPATIAL SCALE DEPENDENCE OF RODENT HABITAT USE ERIC E. JORGENSEN AND STEPHEN DEMARAIS 78 South Monte Vista. Ada, OK 7482 (EEl) Department of Wildlife and Fisheries, Mississippi State University, Mississippi

More information

COMPETITIVE INTERACTIONS BETWEEN TIPTON AND HEERMANN'S KANGAROO RATS IN THE SAN JOAQUIN VALLEY, CALIFORNIA

COMPETITIVE INTERACTIONS BETWEEN TIPTON AND HEERMANN'S KANGAROO RATS IN THE SAN JOAQUIN VALLEY, CALIFORNIA 258 The Southwestern Naturalist val. 58, no. 2 THE SouTHWESTERN NATURALIST 58(2): 258-264 COMPETITIVE INTERACTIONS BETWEEN TIPTON AND HEERMANN'S KANGAROO RATS IN THE SAN JOAQUIN VALLEY, CALIFORNIA ERIN

More information

Mammalogy Lecture 15 - Social Behavior II: Evolution

Mammalogy Lecture 15 - Social Behavior II: Evolution Mammalogy Lecture 15 - Social Behavior II: Evolution I. Evolution of Social Behavior In order to understand the evolution & maintenance of social behavior, we need to examine costs & benefits of group

More information

Chapter 54: Community Ecology

Chapter 54: Community Ecology AP Biology Guided Reading Name Chapter 54: Community Ecology Overview 1. What does community ecology explore? Concept 54.1 Community interactions are classified by whether they help, harm, or have no effect

More information

Principles of Ecology BL / ENVS 402 Exam II Name:

Principles of Ecology BL / ENVS 402 Exam II Name: Principles of Ecology BL / ENVS 402 Exam II 10-26-2011 Name: There are three parts to this exam. Use your time wisely as you only have 50 minutes. Part One: Circle the BEST answer. Each question is worth

More information

Chapter 6 Population and Community Ecology

Chapter 6 Population and Community Ecology Chapter 6 Population and Community Ecology Friedland and Relyea Environmental Science for AP, second edition 2015 W.H. Freeman and Company/BFW AP is a trademark registered and/or owned by the College Board,

More information

Chapter 8 Understanding Populations

Chapter 8 Understanding Populations Chapter 8 Understanding Populations Section 2: How Species Interact with Each Other Key Terms: Niche Competition Predation Parasitism Mutualism Commensalism Symbiosis How Species Interact with Each Other

More information

Desert Patterns. Plants Growth and reproduction Water loss prevention Defenses. Animals Growth and reproduction Water loss prevention Defenses

Desert Patterns. Plants Growth and reproduction Water loss prevention Defenses. Animals Growth and reproduction Water loss prevention Defenses Desert Patterns Plants Growth and reproduction Water loss prevention Defenses Animals Growth and reproduction Water loss prevention Defenses Abiotic Features Introduction A major emphasis in ecology is

More information

Competition Among Organisms

Competition Among Organisms A Vote for Ecology Activity 5 Competition Among Organisms GOALS In this activity you will: Observe the effects of competition among plants for space and nutrients. Describe the possible effects of introducing

More information

REVISION: POPULATION ECOLOGY 01 OCTOBER 2014

REVISION: POPULATION ECOLOGY 01 OCTOBER 2014 REVISION: POPULATION ECOLOGY 01 OCTOBER 2014 Lesson Description In this lesson we revise: Introduction to Population Ecology What s Happening in the Environment Human Population: Analysis & Predictions

More information

Comparing male densities and fertilization rates as potential Allee effects in Alaskan and Canadian Ursus maritimus populations

Comparing male densities and fertilization rates as potential Allee effects in Alaskan and Canadian Ursus maritimus populations Comparing male densities and fertilization rates as potential Allee effects in Alaskan and Canadian Ursus maritimus populations Introduction Research suggests that our world today is in the midst of a

More information

AP Environmental Science I. Unit 1-2: Biodiversity & Evolution

AP Environmental Science I. Unit 1-2: Biodiversity & Evolution NOTE/STUDY GUIDE: Unit 1-2, Biodiversity & Evolution AP Environmental Science I, Mr. Doc Miller, M.Ed. North Central High School Name: ID#: NORTH CENTRAL HIGH SCHOOL NOTE & STUDY GUIDE AP Environmental

More information

Topic outline: Review: evolution and natural selection. Evolution 1. Geologic processes 2. Climate change 3. Catastrophes. Niche.

Topic outline: Review: evolution and natural selection. Evolution 1. Geologic processes 2. Climate change 3. Catastrophes. Niche. Topic outline: Review: evolution and natural selection Evolution 1. Geologic processes 2. Climate change 3. Catastrophes Niche Speciation Extinction Biodiversity Genetic engineering http://www.cengage.com/cgi-wadsworth/course_products_wp.pl?fid=m20b&product_isbn_issn=9780495015987&discipline_number=22

More information

AN ABSTRACT OF THE THESIS OF. for the degree of. presented on. Experimental Analysis of Competition Between Desert Rodents in. Andrew R.

AN ABSTRACT OF THE THESIS OF. for the degree of. presented on. Experimental Analysis of Competition Between Desert Rodents in. Andrew R. AN ABSTRACT OF THE THESIS OF Emile Schoffelen for the degree of Master of Science in Zoology presented on Title: Experimental Analysis of Competition Between Desert Rodents in Oregon Abstract approved:

More information

History and meaning of the word Ecology A. Definition 1. Oikos, ology - the study of the house - the place we live

History and meaning of the word Ecology A. Definition 1. Oikos, ology - the study of the house - the place we live History and meaning of the word Ecology. Definition 1. Oikos, ology - the study of the house - the place we live. Etymology - origin and development of the the word 1. Earliest - Haeckel (1869) - comprehensive

More information

III Introduction to Populations III Introduction to Populations A. Definitions A population is (Krebs 2001:116) a group of organisms same species

III Introduction to Populations III Introduction to Populations A. Definitions A population is (Krebs 2001:116) a group of organisms same species III Introduction to s III Introduction to s A. Definitions B. characteristics, processes, and environment C. Uses of dynamics D. Limits of a A. Definitions What is a? A is (Krebs 2001:116) a group of organisms

More information

Evolution. Before You Read. Read to Learn

Evolution. Before You Read. Read to Learn Evolution 15 section 3 Shaping Evolutionary Theory Biology/Life Sciences 7.e Students know the conditions for Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium in a population and why these conditions are not likely to appear

More information

HABITAT HETEROGENEITY, HABITAT ASSOCIATIONS, AND RODENT SPECIES DIVERSITY IN A SAND SHINNERY-OAK LANDSCAPE

HABITAT HETEROGENEITY, HABITAT ASSOCIATIONS, AND RODENT SPECIES DIVERSITY IN A SAND SHINNERY-OAK LANDSCAPE Journal of Mammalogy, 83(3):743 753, 2002 HABITAT HETEROGENEITY, HABITAT ASSOCIATIONS, AND RODENT SPECIES DIVERSITY IN A SAND SHINNERY-OAK LANDSCAPE MICHAEL J. CRAMER* AND MICHAEL R. WILLIG Program in

More information

Biology 182: Study Guide PART IV. ECOLOGY, BEHAVIOR & CONSERVATION: Ch

Biology 182: Study Guide PART IV. ECOLOGY, BEHAVIOR & CONSERVATION: Ch Biology 182: Study Guide PART IV. ECOLOGY, BEHAVIOR & CONSERVATION: Ch. 51-56 The field of ecology has expanded dramatically over the last few decades, with an ever greater focus on the effects of humans

More information

Levels of Ecological Organization. Biotic and Abiotic Factors. Studying Ecology. Chapter 4 Population Ecology

Levels of Ecological Organization. Biotic and Abiotic Factors. Studying Ecology. Chapter 4 Population Ecology Chapter 4 Population Ecology Lesson 4.1 Studying Ecology Levels of Ecological Organization Biotic and Abiotic Factors The study of how organisms interact with each other and with their environments Scientists

More information

Chapter 4 Population Ecology

Chapter 4 Population Ecology Chapter 4 Population Ecology Lesson 4.1 Studying Ecology Levels of Ecological Organization The study of how organisms interact with each other and with their environments Scientists study ecology at various

More information

Maintenance of species diversity

Maintenance of species diversity 1. Ecological succession A) Definition: the sequential, predictable change in species composition over time foling a disturbance - Primary succession succession starts from a completely empty community

More information

Georgia Performance Standards for Urban Watch Restoration Field Trips

Georgia Performance Standards for Urban Watch Restoration Field Trips Georgia Performance Standards for Field Trips 6 th grade S6E3. Students will recognize the significant role of water in earth processes. a. Explain that a large portion of the Earth s surface is water,

More information

Long-term insights into the influence of precipitation on community dynamics in desert rodents

Long-term insights into the influence of precipitation on community dynamics in desert rodents Journal of Mammalogy, 91(4):787 797, 2010 Long-term insights into the influence of precipitation on community dynamics in desert rodents KATHERINE M. THIBAULT,* S. K. MORGAN ERNEST, ETHAN P. WHITE, JAMES

More information

Ch.5 Evolution and Community Ecology How do organisms become so well suited to their environment? Evolution and Natural Selection

Ch.5 Evolution and Community Ecology How do organisms become so well suited to their environment? Evolution and Natural Selection Ch.5 Evolution and Community Ecology How do organisms become so well suited to their environment? Evolution and Natural Selection Gene: A sequence of DNA that codes for a particular trait Gene pool: All

More information

Chapter 16: Competition. It s all mine, stay away!

Chapter 16: Competition. It s all mine, stay away! Chapter 16: Competition It s all mine, stay away! Species Interactions +/+ +/- -/- Basic interaction -/- Pop growth rate of species 1 (dn 1 /dt) is decreased by interaction Pop growth rate of species 2

More information

Chapter 6 Population and Community Ecology. Thursday, October 19, 17

Chapter 6 Population and Community Ecology. Thursday, October 19, 17 Chapter 6 Population and Community Ecology Module 18 The Abundance and Distribution of After reading this module you should be able to explain how nature exists at several levels of complexity. discuss

More information

Population Ecology Density dependence, regulation and the Allee effect

Population Ecology Density dependence, regulation and the Allee effect 2/22/15 Population Ecology Density dependence, regulation and the Allee effect ESRM 450 Wildlife Ecology and Conservation Wildlife Populations Groups of animals, all of the same species, that live together

More information

BIOLOGY WORKSHEET GRADE: Two robins eating worms on the same lawn is an example of

BIOLOGY WORKSHEET GRADE: Two robins eating worms on the same lawn is an example of BIOLOGY WORKSHEET GRADE: 11 Q.1: Choose the letter of the best answer. 1. Two robins eating worms on the same lawn is an example of a. mutualism. b. commensalism. c. competition. d. parasitism. 2. Predation

More information

Understanding Populations Section 1. Chapter 8 Understanding Populations Section1, How Populations Change in Size DAY ONE

Understanding Populations Section 1. Chapter 8 Understanding Populations Section1, How Populations Change in Size DAY ONE Chapter 8 Understanding Populations Section1, How Populations Change in Size DAY ONE What Is a Population? A population is a group of organisms of the same species that live in a specific geographical

More information

1 29 g, 18% Potato chips 32 g, 23% 2 30 g, 18% Sugar cookies 35 g, 30% 3 28 g, 19% Mouse food 27 g, 18%

1 29 g, 18% Potato chips 32 g, 23% 2 30 g, 18% Sugar cookies 35 g, 30% 3 28 g, 19% Mouse food 27 g, 18% 1. When testing the benefits of a new fertilizer on the growth of tomato plants, the control group should include which of the following? A Tomato plants grown in soil with no fertilizer B Tomato plants

More information

Local resource competition. Sex allocation Is the differential allocation of investment in sons vs. daughters to increase RS. Local mate competition

Local resource competition. Sex allocation Is the differential allocation of investment in sons vs. daughters to increase RS. Local mate competition Sex allocation Is the differential allocation of investment in sons vs. daughters to increase RS Local resource competition Biased against the competing sex Fisher: Genetic model predicts 1:1 sex ratio

More information

Ch 5. Evolution, Biodiversity, and Population Ecology. Part 1: Foundations of Environmental Science

Ch 5. Evolution, Biodiversity, and Population Ecology. Part 1: Foundations of Environmental Science Ch 5 Evolution, Biodiversity, and Population Ecology Part 1: Foundations of Environmental Science PowerPoint Slides prepared by Jay Withgott and Heidi Marcum Copyright 2006 Pearson Education, Inc., publishing

More information

4. Identify one bird that would most likely compete for food with the large tree finch. Support your answer. [1]

4. Identify one bird that would most likely compete for food with the large tree finch. Support your answer. [1] Name: Topic 5B 1. A hawk has a genetic trait that gives it much better eyesight than other hawks of the same species in the same area. Explain how this could lead to evolutionary change within this species

More information

ANALYSIS OF CHARACTER DIVERGENCE ALONG ENVIRONMENTAL GRADIENTS AND OTHER COVARIATES

ANALYSIS OF CHARACTER DIVERGENCE ALONG ENVIRONMENTAL GRADIENTS AND OTHER COVARIATES ORIGINAL ARTICLE doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00063.x ANALYSIS OF CHARACTER DIVERGENCE ALONG ENVIRONMENTAL GRADIENTS AND OTHER COVARIATES Dean C. Adams 1,2,3 and Michael L. Collyer 1,4 1 Department of

More information

IUCN Red List Process. Cormack Gates Keith Aune

IUCN Red List Process. Cormack Gates Keith Aune IUCN Red List Process Cormack Gates Keith Aune The IUCN Red List Categories and Criteria have several specific aims to provide a system that can be applied consistently by different people; to improve

More information

Lesson Overview. Niches and Community Interactions. Lesson Overview. 4.2 Niches and Community Interactions

Lesson Overview. Niches and Community Interactions. Lesson Overview. 4.2 Niches and Community Interactions Lesson Overview 4.2 Niches and Community Interactions The Niche What is a niche? A niche is the range of physical and biological conditions in which a species lives and the way the species obtains what

More information

Lecture 8 Insect ecology and balance of life

Lecture 8 Insect ecology and balance of life Lecture 8 Insect ecology and balance of life Ecology: The term ecology is derived from the Greek term oikos meaning house combined with logy meaning the science of or the study of. Thus literally ecology

More information

Chapter 44. Table of Contents. Section 1 Development of Behavior. Section 2 Types of Animal Behavior. Animal Behavior

Chapter 44. Table of Contents. Section 1 Development of Behavior. Section 2 Types of Animal Behavior. Animal Behavior Animal Behavior Table of Contents Section 1 Development of Behavior Section 2 Types of Animal Behavior Section 1 Development of Behavior Objectives Identify four questions asked by biologists who study

More information

Principles of Animal Behavior

Principles of Animal Behavior Animals and Humans! Lee Alan Dugatkin Principles of Animal Behavior THIRD EDITION Chapter 1 Principles of Animal Behavior We are surrounded by animals and many humans like to know them" Early human art

More information

The Effect of Fire on Bird and Small Mammal Communities in the Grasslands of Wind Cave National Park

The Effect of Fire on Bird and Small Mammal Communities in the Grasslands of Wind Cave National Park University of Wyoming National Park Service Research Center Annual Report Volume 7 7th Annual Report, 1983 Article 19 1-1-1983 The Effect of Fire on Bird and Small Mammal Communities in the Grasslands

More information

Chapter 04 Lecture Outline

Chapter 04 Lecture Outline Chapter 04 Lecture Outline William P. Cunningham University of Minnesota Mary Ann Cunningham Vassar College Copyright The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Permission required for reproduction or display. 1

More information

Minimizing interspecific competition by different foraging strategies in two North African desert rodents

Minimizing interspecific competition by different foraging strategies in two North African desert rodents Acta Theriologica 36 (1-2): 109-117,1991. PL IS SN X)01-7051 Minimizing interspecific competition by different foraging strategies in two North African desert rodents Rüdiger SCHRÖPFER and Brigitte KLENNER-FRINGES

More information

Zero Sum, the Niche, and Metacommunities: Long-Term Dynamics of Community Assembly

Zero Sum, the Niche, and Metacommunities: Long-Term Dynamics of Community Assembly vol. 172, no. 6 the american naturalist december 2008 E-Synthesis Zero Sum, the Niche, and Metacommunities: Long-Term Dynamics of Community Assembly S. K. Morgan Ernest, 1,* James H. Brown, 2, Katherine

More information

Weather is the day-to-day condition of Earth s atmosphere.

Weather is the day-to-day condition of Earth s atmosphere. 4.1 Climate Weather and Climate Weather is the day-to-day condition of Earth s atmosphere. Climate refers to average conditions over long periods and is defined by year-after-year patterns of temperature

More information

Case Studies in Ecology and Evolution

Case Studies in Ecology and Evolution 7 Competition (this chapter is still unfinished) Species compete in many ways. Sometimes there are dramatic contests, such as when male bighorns compete for access to mates. Territoriality. That kind of

More information

Age (x) nx lx. Population dynamics Population size through time should be predictable N t+1 = N t + B + I - D - E

Age (x) nx lx. Population dynamics Population size through time should be predictable N t+1 = N t + B + I - D - E Population dynamics Population size through time should be predictable N t+1 = N t + B + I - D - E Time 1 N = 100 20 births 25 deaths 10 immigrants 15 emmigrants Time 2 100 + 20 +10 25 15 = 90 Life History

More information

Competition. Not until we reach the extreme confines of life, in the arctic regions or on the borders of an utter desert, will competition cease

Competition. Not until we reach the extreme confines of life, in the arctic regions or on the borders of an utter desert, will competition cease Competition Not until we reach the extreme confines of life, in the arctic regions or on the borders of an utter desert, will competition cease Darwin 1859 Origin of Species Competition A mutually negative

More information

Bio112 Home Work Community Structure

Bio112 Home Work Community Structure Bio112 Home Work Community Structure Multiple Choice Identify the choice that best completes the statement or answers the question. 1. All of the populations of different species that occupy and are adapted

More information

D. Correct! Allelopathy is a form of interference competition in plants. Therefore this answer is correct.

D. Correct! Allelopathy is a form of interference competition in plants. Therefore this answer is correct. Ecology Problem Drill 18: Competition in Ecology Question No. 1 of 10 Question 1. The concept of allelopathy focuses on which of the following: (A) Carrying capacity (B) Limiting resource (C) Law of the

More information

Multiple choice 2 pts each): x 2 = 18) Essay (pre-prepared) / 15 points. 19) Short Answer: / 2 points. 20) Short Answer / 5 points

Multiple choice 2 pts each): x 2 = 18) Essay (pre-prepared) / 15 points. 19) Short Answer: / 2 points. 20) Short Answer / 5 points P 1 Biology 217: Ecology Second Exam Fall 2004 There should be 7 ps in this exam - take a moment and count them now. Put your name on the first p of the exam, and on each of the ps with short answer questions.

More information

Overview of Chapter 5

Overview of Chapter 5 Chapter 5 Ecosystems and Living Organisms Overview of Chapter 5 Evolution Natural Selection Biological Communities Symbiosis Predation & Competition Community Development Succession Evolution The cumulative

More information

Gary G. Mittelbach Michigan State University

Gary G. Mittelbach Michigan State University Community Ecology Gary G. Mittelbach Michigan State University Sinauer Associates, Inc. Publishers Sunderland, Massachusetts U.S.A. Brief Table of Contents 1 Community Ecology s Roots 1 PART I The Big

More information

Effects of Environmental Variables on Some Physiological Responses of Microtus Montanus under Natural Conditions (summary for 1976)

Effects of Environmental Variables on Some Physiological Responses of Microtus Montanus under Natural Conditions (summary for 1976) Jackson Hole Research Station Annual Report Volume 1976 Jackson Hole Biological Research Station - Annual Report - 1976 Article 11 1976 Effects of Environmental Variables on Some Physiological Responses

More information

Approach to Field Research Data Generation and Field Logistics Part 1. Road Map 8/26/2016

Approach to Field Research Data Generation and Field Logistics Part 1. Road Map 8/26/2016 Approach to Field Research Data Generation and Field Logistics Part 1 Lecture 3 AEC 460 Road Map How we do ecology Part 1 Recap Types of data Sampling abundance and density methods Part 2 Sampling design

More information