Within predator-prey systems, interference has been defined

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Within predator-prey systems, interference has been defined"

Transcription

1 Behavioral Ecology Vol. 11 No. 6: Predator search pattern and the strength of interference through prey depression Richard A. Stillman, John D. Goss-Custard, and Matthew J. Alexander Centre for Ecology and Hydrology, Dorset, Winfrith Technology Centre, Winfrith Newburgh, Dorchester, Dorset DT2 8DE, United Kingdom We develop a model of predators foraging within a single patch, on prey that become temporarily immune to predation (depressed) after detecting a predator. Interference through prey depression occurs because the proportion of vulnerable prey (and hence intake rate) decreases as predator density increases. Predators in our model are not forced to move randomly within the patch, as is the case in other similar models, but can avoid areas of depressed prey and so preferentially forage over vulnerable prey. We compare the extent to which different avoidance rules (e.g., move more quickly over depressed prey or turn if approaching depressed prey) influence the amount of time spent foraging over depressed and vulnerable prey, and how this influences the strength of interference. Although based on a different mechanism, our model produces two similar general predictions to interference models based on direct interactions between predators: the strength of interference increases with (1) increased competitor density and (2) decreased prey encounter rate. This suggests that there are underlying similarities in the nature of interference even when it arises through different processes. Not surprisingly, avoidance of depressed prey can substantially reduce the strength of interference compared with random foraging. However, we identify the region of the model s parameter space in which this reduction is particularly large and show that the only system for which suitable data are available, redshank Tringa totanus feeding on Corophium volutator, falls within this region. The model shows that, by adjusting its search path to avoid areas of depressed prey, a predator can substantially reduce the amount of the interference it experiences and that this applies over a wide range of parameter space, including the region occupied by a real system. This suggests that behavior-based interference models should consider predator search pattern if they are to accurately predict the strength of the interference. Key words: foraging behavior, interference competition, prey refuge, redshank Tringa totanus, search path avoidance. [Behav Ecol 11: (2000)] Within predator-prey systems, interference has been defined as the short-term, reversible decline in intake rate due to the presence of competitors (Goss-Custard, 1980; Sutherland, 1983) and is thought to be one of the major factors influencing the distribution (e.g., Goss-Custard, 1970; Meer and Ens, 1997; Parker and Sutherland, 1986) and survival (e.g., Goss-Custard and Sutherland, 1997) of foraging animals. Several mechanisms of interference have been observed, including prey stealing (kleptoparasitism) (Ens and Goss-Custard, 1984; Goss-Custard et al., 1984; Triplet et al., 1999; Whitehouse and Lubin, 1999), intraspecific aggression (Mansour and Lipcius, 1991), displacement from rich microsites (Bautista et al., 1998; Dolman, 1995), the time-cost of avoiding (Ens and Cayford, 1996) or scanning for potential aggressors (Cresswell, 1997) and prevention of access to prey (Whitehouse and Lubin, 1999). Interference in these systems occurs because of the presence of or risk of interactions between competitors, and several behavioral models have been based on similar mechanisms (e.g., Holmgren, 1995; Moody and Houston, 1995; Ruxton et al., 1992; Stillman et al., 1997, 2000). In contrast, interference through prey depression has received relatively little attention. Prey depression occurs when prey are able to respond to the close proximity of predators (e.g., by retreating down a burrow) and temporarily become immune to predation (e.g., until they re-emerge from their burrow). Interference occurs because, as predator density increases, the proportion of depressed prey also increases and so the intake rate of predators decreases. Interference through prey depression has been recorded in redshank Trin- Address correspondence to R. A. Stillman. rast@ceh.ac.uk. Received 25 August 1999; revised 3 February 2000; accepted 10 April International Society for Behavioral Ecology ga totanus L., feeding on a burrow-digging, amphipod crustacean Corophium volutator (Pallas) (Selman and Goss-Custard, 1988; Yates et al., 2000), which retreats into a burrow after detecting a predator (Goss-Custard, 1970). It has also been portrayed in a mathematical behavior-based model (Ruxton, 1995). Behavior-based models have successfully predicted the shape of the interference function, the relationship between intake rate and competitor density, in shorebirds (Stillman et al., 1996, 1997), and the increased strength of interference, the proportional change in intake rate caused by a proportional change in competitor density, at low prey densities (Cresswell, 1998; Dolman, 1995; Triplet et al., 1999). Although promising, these models do have limitations. In particular, for tractability, mathematical behavior-based models (e.g., Holmgren, 1995; Moody and Houston, 1995; Ruxton, 1995; Ruxton et al., 1992) assume that animals follow random search paths, where in reality, they may actually avoid each other. The potential importance of avoidance in reducing the strength of interference has been stressed by Goss-Custard (1970) and Vines (1980). Recently its importance has been re-emphasized by Norris and Johnstone (1998) who suggest that the avoidance behavior of oystercatchers Haematopus ostralegus L. feeding on cockles Cerastoderma edule L. reduces kleptoparasitism rates below those expected with random searching. These results suggest that behavior-based models may need to incorporate details of avoidance behavior if they are to accurately predict the strength of interference. In this article we present a model of interference through prey depression, which is a development of that previously used to model interference through kleptoparasitism and avoidance (Stillman et al., 1997, 2000). Our model differs from previous mathematical models in that it does not need to assume that predators forage randomly. Instead, by using

2 598 Behavioral Ecology Vol. 11 No. 6 simple rules to alter their search path, they can avoid areas of depressed prey and so forage preferentially over areas of vulnerable prey. We model local avoidance within a single patch; predators do not have the option of moving to other patches. We compare the extent to which different movement rules cause predators to avoid depressed prey. While it is self evident that the avoidance of depressed prey will decrease the strength of interference, the key issues are: (1) in which region of the model s parameter space is the decrease in the strength of interference particularly large, and (2) do any real predator-prey systems fall within this sensitive region. Therefore, we determine the influence of movement rules on the strength of interference throughout the model s parameter space, and parameterize the model for the only system for which data are available, redshank feeding on Corophium. We conclude that, if real systems fall within the region in which the strength of interference is sensitive to search patterns, interference models will need to incorporate these patterns if they are to produce accurate predictions. THE MODEL The model simulates the foraging behavior of a predator population and the predator avoidance behavior of a prey population within a two-dimensional square patch. Prey are uniformly distributed across the patch within a square grid of cm cells. Prey cannot move across the patch but can respond to the presence of a predator and so make themselves temporarily immune to predation. All prey within a single cell respond at the same time to the presence of a predator. Each individual in the predator population is followed continuously as it moves across the patch searching for prey. To remove edge effects, the patch has wrap-around margins, so that a predator moving off of one side reappears on the opposite and prey close to one edge may respond to the presence of a predator close to the opposite. Prey are not depleted during the course of simulations. Simulations progress in discrete time steps. Prey behavior At any point in time, each prey cell is in one of two vulnerability states: (1) vulnerable to attack or (2) depressed and hence immune to attack. Vulnerable prey respond to the presence of a predator when it is within the response distance (D R )of the center of the prey cell. Prey show the same response, regardless of the behavioral state of the predator. They spend a fixed amount of time responding to the predator (T R ) during which they may still be attacked, but after the response time has elapsed, prey become depressed and immune to attack. If the model had assumed that prey became depressed as soon as they detected a predator, rather than only after the response time had elapsed, predators would never have been able to feed; they would always have been moving into prey cells that had already detected them and become depressed. The response time means, in effect, that prey cells do not become depressed until after a predator has passed. After becoming depressed, during each subsequent time interval, each prey cell has a fixed probability (P V ) of returning to the vulnerable state. This assumption means that depressed prey return to the vulnerable state at a constant proportional rate. At the start of simulations all prey are vulnerable. Predator behavior During each point in time, each predator occupies one of two behavioral states: (1) searching for prey or (2) handling and consuming prey. There are no aggressive interactions between predators. Searching predators walk in a straight line in search of prey. If a predator is within a prey cell in which prey are vulnerable, it moves at a fixed speed (S V ) and has a fixed probability ( ) of capturing a prey item during a single time step. Although the model does not specify the actual density of prey within each cell, it assumes that is positively related to prey density (i.e., increases in occur because of increases in prey density). When a predator captures prey it turns into the handling state. Handling predators are stationary and spend a fixed amount of time consuming the prey (T H ) before continuing to search in their previous searching direction. When predators are searching in prey cells in which prey are depressed, they move at a fixed speed (S D ) and are unable to capture prey until they leave the cell. At the start of simulations each predator is set to the searching state, positioned at a random location within the patch and given a random searching direction. Predator movement rules Predators in the model may avoid the search paths of others and reduce the amount of time they spend foraging over depressed prey. The different avoidance rules used in the model differ according to whether predators can detect the vulnerability of the prey and in the distance over which prey vulnerability can be detected. No knowledge: random foraging Predators are unaware of the vulnerability of the prey and search at the same speed (S D S V ) and in the same direction, regardless of whether they are foraging over vulnerable or depressed prey. Local knowledge: change in search speed and fixed turning Predators know the vulnerability of the prey cell they are currently in and the one they are about to move into, but are unaware of the states of more distant cells. Predators may move more quickly over areas of depressed prey than over vulnerable prey (S D S V ), or turn at a fixed angle (a), at random to either the left or right, if about to move from a vulnerable prey cell to one which is depressed. Distant knowledge: optimal turning Predators know the vulnerability of all cells up to the perfect knowledge distance (D K ) from their current location. If about to enter a depressed cell, predators change direction and walk directly towards the center of the nearest vulnerable prey cell within the perfect knowledge distance. If none are available within this distance, they continue walking in their current direction. Mechanism of interference The only mechanism of interference in the model is prey depression. Therefore, the more time a predator spends over vulnerable prey, the higher its intake rate, or conversely, the more time spent over depressed prey, the greater the strength of interference. Predators spend more time over depressed prey when a higher proportion of prey cells are depressed, but can reduce this time if they avoid depressed prey cells and preferentially forage over vulnerable cells. Model parameter values: general predator-prey systems In order to investigate the general properties of the model, we ran simulations over a wide range of parameter values. It was not possible to run simulations based on all possible combinations of parameter values and so each parameter was var-

3 Stillman et al. Search pattern and interference 599 Table 1 Parameter values used to model general predator-prey systems and the redshank-corophium system Parameter Competitor density Prey encounter rate ( ) Handling time (T H ) Searching speed over vulnerable prey (S V ) Searching speed over depressed prey (S D ) Response distance (D R ) Response time (T R ) Recovery rate (P V ) Fixed turning angle (a) Perfect knowledge distance (D K ) General predator-prey systems 1000 (1 1000) 0.05 (0 1) time step 1 25 (0 100) time steps 0.5 (0 1) time step (0 2.5) time step 1 1 (0 5) 10 (0 20) time steps 0.01 (0 0.1) time step 1 0 (0 180) 0 (0 2) Redshank-Corophium system 1000 (1 1000) birds ha (0 1) s (0 1.4) s 0.23 (0 0.46) ms ms (0 1.2) m 5 (0 60) s ( ) s (0 2) m The table shows the default value and range of values for each parameter. ied individually with all others set to their default value. Table 1 shows the default value and range of values used for each parameter. Model parameter values: Redshank-Corophium system The following parameter values were used to model redshank feeding on Corophium volutator (see Table 1). Goss-Custard and Rothery (1976) measured handling time in this system (T H 0.7 (95% c.i. 0.1) s). As handling time is so short, the length of one time step in the model was set to 0.1 s for simulations of this system. Prey encounter rate was calculated from the data used by Yates et al. (2000) ( 0.50 (95% c.i. 0.1) s 1 ). Redshank searching rate over vulnerable prey (S V ) and, unless otherwise stated, over depressed prey (S D ) was calculated from Table 4 of Goss-Custard (1970) (S V S D 0.23 (95% c.i. 0.02) ms 1 ). To ensure that prey cells did not become depressed until a redshank had passed through them, the response time (T R ) was set to 5 s (cells detected redshank when the distance between the bird and the center of the cell was less than 0.6 m (see below); the maximum distance between the center of a m cell and its edge is 0.35 m; therefore, the maximum distance a redshank needs to travel so that it has passed through a cell that has detected it m; it takes a redshank traveling at 0.23 ms 1 5 s to exceed this distance). The range of T R was 0 60 s, the upper limited calculated from Figure 4 of Goss-Custard (1970), which shows that Corophium respond to a passing redshank in less than 60 s, the frequency with which counts were made. Although no quantitative measurements have been made, Corophium actually respond very quickly to vibrations on the substrate surface (Goss-Custard JD, personal observation.), and so the upper limit of the response time is a large overestimate. The rate at which depressed prey returned to the vulnerable state (P V ) was estimated using Figure 4 of Goss- Custard (1970), which plots the increasing numbers of Corophium appearing at the mud surface per min against the time after a redshank had passed over the mud. We used non-linear regression to fit an asymptotic exponential function (N a(1 e bt ); where N number of Corophium appearing at mud surface per min; t time (s) after redshank has passed over mud; a maximum number of Corophium appearing at mud surface; b instantaneous rate at which the numbers of Corophium appearing at the surface increases with time after redshank passes) to each of the three trials performed by Goss- Custard (1970). The following parameter values were estimated for the three trials respectively: a 23.6, 11.4 and 10.9; b , and The values of b were converted from instantaneous rates of change to rates of change during 0.1 second time steps (b 0.1 )(b e 0.1b , , ) and the mean value of b 0.1 was taken as the probability of a prey cell changing from the depressed to vulnerable state during a time step (P V (95% c.i ) 0.1s 1 ). For presentation, P V is expressed per second, rather than per 0.1 second (P V s 1 ). Yates et al. (2000) showed how the feeding rate of redshank in an area of mudflat depended on the time since another redshank had passed through and depressed Corophium within the quadrat. They fitted an asymptotic exponential function to their data and estimated the instantaneous rate of increase in feeding rate with time after the first bird (i.e., the equivalent parameter to b) as s 1. This is another, similar measure of the rate at which Corophium return to the mud surface after being depressed by a redshank, which suggests that the value of P V used in the model is a reliable estimate. The distance over which Corophium detect and respond to redshank (D R ) is unknown, but Yates et al. (2000) showed that, on average, the feeding rate of redshank within a quadrat was 80% lower than its interference-free rate if another redshank had just passed through the quadrat. This interference was due to depression of Corophium within the search path of the first redshank (Goss-Custard, 1970; Yates et al., 2000), suggesting that Corophium were depressed over 80% of the square. A simple simulation model was developed to determine the distance over which Corophium would need to respond to redshank in order to produce this result. In the model redshank were assumed to follow random search paths at 0.23 ms 1 through a m quadrat and depress all prey (which were arranged in a grid of cells) within a fixed distance. Prey returned to the vulnerable state during each time step with probability The response distance was adjusted in different simulations until 80% of prey in the quadrat were depressed. If redshank passed through the quadrat very infrequently, all prey returned to being vulnerable between birds, and the response distance needed to be 1 m for, on average, 80% of the prey to be depressed after a bird passed through. If a sequence of birds passed through the quadrat, one entering as soon as the previous one left, most prey were still depressed when a new bird entered the quadrat. In this case, the response distance only needed to be 0.1 m for the equilibrium percentage of depressed prey to be 80%. The actual rate at which a series of redshank passed through the quadrat was not measured in the field study, but was probably somewhere between these extremes. Therefore, as no other data were available, the average response distance (D R ) of 0.6 m (range m) was used in the model.

4 600 Behavioral Ecology Vol. 11 No. 6 RESULTS Comparison of depressed prey avoidance rules Simple avoidance rules, in which predators had very restricted knowledge of the vulnerability of the prey, had little effect on the proportion of time over depressed prey, and hence the predicted strength of interference (see Figure 1a,b). The proportion of time spent foraging over vulnerable prey was very similar to the proportion of vulnerable prey throughout the patch. The strength of interference actually increased slightly when predators accelerated across depressed prey cells (see Figure 1a) and only decreased slightly when predators changed direction before entering depressed prey cells (see Figure 1b). Increased movement speed had little effect on the strength of interference because it had two opposing actions. By accelerating over depressed prey, predators more rapidly moved into areas of vulnerable prey, but at the same time they also depressed prey close to their search path at a higher rate, thus increasing the overall proportion of depressed prey. Turning at a fixed angle when entering depressed prey had little effect on the strength of interference because predators usually encountered depressed prey even after turning. The strength of interference was decreased to a much greater extent by optimal turning, in which predators had perfect knowledge of prey up to a fixed distance and moved towards the nearest vulnerable prey cell (see Figure 1c). This happened because predators spent a higher proportion of the time searching over vulnerable prey than if they had searched randomly or used more simple forms of avoidance. The percentage of time foraging over vulnerable prey was up to twice as high as the proportion of vulnerable prey throughout the patch. All further simulations compare random foraging with optimal turning. Effect of search pattern on the strength of interference Predators in the model followed independent search paths and so the total proportion of the prey that were depressed increased with increased competitor density. As a result, the intake rate of predators decreased with increased competitor density, both when predators searched at random and when they avoided depressed prey (see Figure 2). For both types of search pattern, log intake rate decreased at an accelerating rate with increased log competitor density, indicating that the strength of interference, the proportional decrease in intake rate due to a proportional increase in density, increased with increased competitor density. Although, the general relationship between competitor density and intake rate was uninfluenced by search pattern, the strength of interference increased more slowly with increased competitor density when predators avoided depressed prey than when they searched randomly. The general relationships between parameter values and the strength of interference were unaffected by search pattern (see Figure 3). Increases in either prey encounter rate or handling time caused predators to spend more time stationary and handling prey rather than moving and searching for prey. Stationary predators depressed fewer prey cells than moving predators, and so increases in either encounter rate (see Figure 3a) or handling time (see Figure 3b) decreased the predicted strength of interference. Increased searching speed increased the chance that a predator had passed through a prey cell before it became depressed (which acted to decrease the strength of interference), but also increased the rate at which predators encountered and so depressed other prey cells (which acted to increase the strength of interference). The overall effect of changes in searching speed depended on the speed from which changes occurred. When searching speed Figure 1 Effect of depressed prey avoidance rules on the strength of interference between predators (solid circles), percentage of time spent by predators over vulnerable prey (squares) and percentage of vulnerable prey (open circles): (a) changes in search speed over depressed prey; (b) fixed turning away from depressed prey; (c) optimal turning towards vulnerable prey. Note that intake rate is expressed as a percentage of that achieved in the absence of competitors. Each graph shows the effect of varying one avoidance parameter with all other parameters set to the default values given in Table 1. Predators do not respond to the prey and so follow random search paths when S D S V 0.5 time step 1, a 0 and D K 0.

5 Stillman et al. Search pattern and interference 601 Figure 2 Predicted shape of the interference function when predators either search at random (squares) or avoid depressed prey by optimal turning with a perfect knowledge distance of 1(circles). Two examples are shown in which the recovery rate of depressed prey is either high (p V 0.01 time step 1 ; open symbols) or low (p V time step 1 ; solid symbols). All other parameters are set to the default values given in Table 1. For consistency with previous studies, both relative intake rate and competitor density are plotted on log scales. was initially low, the main effect of increased speed was that predators were more likely to pass through cells before they became depressed, hence increased speed reduced the strength of interference (see Figure 3c). In contrast, when searching speed was high enough that predators always passed through cells before they became depressed, further increases in speed depressed prey at a higher rate, hence increased speed increased the strength of interference (see Figure 3c). The proportion of depressed prey was increased if prey responded to predators at a greater distance or if they remained depressed for longer. Decreased prey response time decreased the chance that predators had passed through prey cells before they became depressed. Hence, the strength of interference was increased by increased prey response distance (see Figure 3d), decreased response time (see Figure 3e) or decreased recovery rate (see Figure 3f). Although, the general effects of each parameter on the strength of interference were not influenced by search pattern, for most parameter values tested, the actual strength of interference predicted by the model was much lower when animals avoided depressed prey than when they searched at random (see Figure 3). Only when prey encounter rate (see Figure 3a) or recovery rate (see Figure 3f) were very high, or predator searching speed (see Figure 3c), prey response distance (see Figure 3d), response time (see Figure 3e) or recovery rate (see Figure 3f) very low, was the strength of interference relatively unaffected by search pattern. In general, search pattern had little influence on the strength of interference when interference was absent or very weak (high encounter rate, short response distance or rapid recovery rate), or when it was so intense that intake rates were virtually zero (low searching speed, rapid response time, or slow recovery rate). Therefore, the strength of interference was sensitive to the search pattern of predators throughout most of the model s parameter space. Effect of search pattern on the strength of interference in the redshank-corophium system Real redshank do not search at random, but avoid depressed prey with the search paths of competitors (Yates et al., 2000). This suggests that a model incorporating depressed prey avoidance would more closely mimic the behavior of real birds than one based on random searching, even though the actual mechanism of avoidance in redshank is unknown. However, this level of detail would be unnecessary if the redshank-corophium system fell in the area of parameter space within which predator search pattern had little effect on the strength of interference. Therefore, we tested the effect of depressed prey avoidance on the predicted strength of interference in this system, and compared which type of search pattern most accurately predicted the observed strength of interference. As it is unclear how real redshank avoid each others search paths, simulations were run to determine the extent to which the predicted strength of interference depended on searching speed over depressed prey, fixed or optimal turning when about to enter a depressed prey cell. As in the previous simulations, only optimal turning had any substantial effect on the predicted strength of interference, and so the results presented are restricted to this avoidance rule. The general shape of interference function predicted by the model was uninfluenced by search pattern and similar to that observed (see Figure 4a). Both the observed and predicted strengths of interference increased with increased competitor density. However, when redshank searched at random, the predicted strength of interference was greater than that observed. In contrast, optimal turning decreased the strength of interference and matched more closely its observed strength (see Figure 4a). The predicted strength of interference decreased with increased perfect knowledge distance, and when assuming a perfect knowledge distance of 0.75 m, the model accurately predicted intake rate at 1000 birds ha 1 (see Figure 4b). Even though the strength of interference was decreased, the precise shape of interference function predicted by the model was still different to that observed. In reality, interference remained insignificant up to higher competitor densities, but then increased in intensity at a higher rate with increased density (i.e., the observed interference function was more curved than that predicted). These simulations showed that the predicted strength of interference was sensitive to search pattern within the region of parameter space occupied by the redshank-corophium system, and that the discrepancy between observation and prediction could be reduced by allowing redshank to avoid areas of depressed prey as real birds do. However, not all of the model s parameters were measured directly and it could have been errors in parameter estimates that caused the discrepancy rather than search pattern. Therefore, we examined the model s sensitivity to changes in its parameter values. The model was relatively insensitive to changes in prey encounter rate (see Figure 5a), handling time (see Figure 5b) and searching speed (see Figure 5c). These parameter values also had relatively small confidence limits, within which the model always over-predicted the strength of interference when based on random searching, but more accurately predicted the strength of interference when based on avoidance. The model was more sensitive to changes in the prey response time (see Figure 5e) and recovery rate (see Figure 5f), but again, when based on random searching, the strength of interference was over-predicted throughout the confidence limits of these parameters. Avoidance of depressed prey, substantially reduced the strength of interference throughout these limits, except when response time was very low. The model was most sensitive to changes in the prey response distance (see Figure 5d), which has not been measured directly in the field, and could only be estimated within relatively large confidence limits. As a result, within the confidence limits of this parameter, the random search version of the model could either under or overestimate the observed strength of interference. Neverthe-

6 602 Behavioral Ecology Vol. 11 No. 6 Figure 3 Effect of each parameter on the predicted strength of interference when predators either search at random (open circles) or avoid depressed prey by optimal turning with a perfect knowledge distance of 1 (solid circles). Each graph shows the effect of varying one parameter with all others set to the default values given in Table 1. less, within these limits, the avoidance of depressed prey still substantially reduced the strength of interference. In summary, the strength of interference remained sensitive to search pattern throughout the confidence limits of each parameter, except for low response times, and the strength of interference was consistently overestimated by the random search version of the model, except towards the lower confidence limit of response distance. DISCUSSION Many models dealing with ecologically relevant behavioral processes are not able to explore whether the main conclusions of the model apply within the region of parameter space occupied by real animals. In an attempt to avoid this limitation, we parameterized the model for the only system for which most of the necessary data were available. In this way, we were able to show that our conclusion derived from the theoretical model that predator avoidance of depressed prey is likely to have an important influence on the predicted strength of interference seems likely to apply to at least one system. Because there are no reasons to regard that system as unusual among predators, it is probable that the conclusion holds for many more predator-systems than the one for which we are currently able to parameterize the model. Even though we showed that avoidance of depressed prey can substantially reduce the strength of interference, it is unknown whether the mechanisms of avoidance used in the model are the same as those adopted by real animals. In the case of redshank, Yates et al. (2000) showed that real birds crossed each others search paths less frequently than if they had searched randomly, providing evidence for the avoidance of depressed prey, but did not identify the mechanism of this avoidance. The most efficient avoidance mechanism tested, optimal turning, relied on birds being able to detect the vulnerability of prey within 1 m. Whereas redshank can undoubtedly detect the presence of vulnerable Corophium within a very short distance, because they hunt visually for this prey (Goss-Custard, 1970), the maximum distance over which they can do this is uncertain. The other mechanisms of avoidance used in the model, accelerating over depressed prey and fixed turning, only relied on redshank detecting the vulnerability of prey in their immediate vicinity. Similar behavior has been observed in real animals (e.g., Pienkowski, 1983; Smith, 1974) and can cause aggregation within favorable patches (e.g., Stillman and Sutherland, 1990), but in the current model these

7 Stillman et al. Search pattern and interference 603 Figure 4 Effect of depressed prey avoidance on the predicted strength of interference between redshank feeding on Corophium: (a) shape of the interference function when redshank either search at random (open circles) or avoid depressed prey by optimal turning with a perfect knowledge distance of 1 m (solid circles); (b) effect of the perfect knowledge distance at a competitor density of 1000 birds ha 1. The observed strength of interference is shown by the squares in (a) and the broken line in (b) (Yates et al., 2000). For comparison with previous studies, in (a), both intake rate and competitor density are plotted on a log scale. See Table 1 for parameter values. rules did not cause much avoidance of depressed prey. Alternatively, other mechanisms of avoidance could be employed that have the same effect on the birds search paths, but do not rely on the direct detection of vulnerable prey. For example, the absence of other birds from an area could be used as a cue to whether prey are likely to be vulnerable. In this way, animals would only need to monitor the current and recent locations of competitors in order to concentrate their foraging effort in areas that have been unexploited for some time. Avoidance behavior is also frequent in animals in which interference occurs through direct competitive interactions, and is thought to reduce the strength of interference in these systems as well (e.g., Goss-Custard, 1970; Norris and Johnstone, 1998; Vines, 1980). Studies of the precise ways in which predators avoid each other, or each others search paths, are required in order to fully parameterize behavior-based interference models. The observed and predicted interference functions had approximately similar shapes, but the observed function was more curved; interference remained low up to higher competitor densities, but then increased at a higher rate with further increases in density. A possible explanation for this discrepancy could be that the model s parameters were fixed across the full range of competitor densities, whereas in reality they may be density-dependent. For example, prey were assumed to respond in the same manner, regardless of the density of redshank. If the response distance increased with predator density, the strength of interference would increase more rapidly as redshank density increased. Alternatively, if prey became habituated to redshank, the response distance would decrease with increased redshank density. This would lead to the strength of interference increasing less rapidly as redshank density increased. The model s predictions were highly sensitive to the response distance and so even relatively small density-dependent changes could have a large effect on the predicted interference function. Another possibility is that redshank avoidance behavior changes with density. For example, at low densities, they may be able to detect likely areas of vulnerable prey at a long distance, simply from the absence of other birds, but, at high densities, may have to use rules similar to those in the model. Further studies are required to test these ideas. The current model produced two general predictions which are also produced by interference models based on direct competitive interactions, such as kleptoparasitism or avoidance (e.g., Ruxton et al., 1992; Stillman et al., 1997); the strength of interference increased both with increased competitor density and decreased prey encounter rate. Similarly, Ruxton (1995) stressed that his mathematical model of interference through prey depression was structurally identical to another model based on competitive interactions (Ruxton et al., 1992). Field studies of species in which interference occurs through competitive interactions have given support for the predicted shape of interference function (Dolman, 1995; Stillman et al., 1996; Triplet et al., 1999) and the association between the strength of interference and prey abundance (Cresswell, 1998; Dolman, 1995; Triplet et al., 1999). However, further studies are required to test the prediction that the strength of interference caused by prey depression is related to prey abundance. Both types of interference model also predict that the strength of interference is most sensitive to parameters expressing the distance over which interactions occur. In the current model, interference was most sensitive to the response distance of prey to predators, whereas the previous version, based on direct interactions, was most sensitive to the distance over which predators initiate fights (Stillman et al., 2000). This suggests that the predictive power of behavior-based interference models will rely on the accurate measurement of the distance over which interactions occur. These similarities between model predictions suggest that there are underlying similarities in the nature of interference, even when it operates through different mechanisms. It is important to realize that applying the model to redshank should not be regarded as a test of the model. We could not test and validate the model because one important parameter value, the response distance of Corophium to redshank, has not yet been measured, and it is unknown whether the mechanisms of depressed prey avoidance used in the model are the same as those used by real birds. In common with many other theoretical models, formal testing cannot yet be attempted in our case. But what the model does show is that, by avoiding areas of depressed prey, a predator can very substantially reduce the magnitude of the interference it experiences and that this applies over a wide range of parameter space, including the region occupied by the one system for

8 604 Behavioral Ecology Vol. 11 No. 6 Figure 5 Sensitivity analysis of the redshank-corophium model at 1000 birds ha 1. The broken lines show the observed relative intake rate (Yates et al., 2000) and the symbols show changes in predicted relative intake rate caused by changes in each parameter when redshank either search randomly (squares) or avoid depressed prey by optimal turning with a perfect knowledge distance of 1 m (circles). The open symbols indicate default parameter values and the shaded area indicates each parameter s 95% confidence interval (, T H,S V,S D and P V ) or range (D R and T R ). In each graph, one parameter is varied with the others set to the default values given in Table 1. which most of the necessary data are available. This suggests that other modeling attempts should consider this aspect of the system if the models are to provide realistic predictions of the strength of the interference in the real world. We are very grateful to Richard Caldow for many useful discussions and to three anonymous referees who provided valuable comments on the manuscript. R.A.S. and M.J.A. were funded by the Natural Environment Research Council. REFERENCES Bautista LM, Alonso JC, Alonso JA, Foraging site displacement in common crane flocks. Anim Behav 56:

9 Stillman et al. Search pattern and interference 605 Cresswell W, Interference competition at low competitor densities in blackbirds Turdus merula. J Anim Ecol 66: Cresswell W, Variation in the strength of interference with resource density in blackbirds, Turdus merula. Oikos 81: Dolman PM, The intensity of interference varies with resource density: evidence from a field study with snow buntings, Plectrophenox nivalis. Oecologia 102: Ens BJ, Cayford JT, Feeding with other Oystercatchers. In: The Oystercatcher: from individuals to populations (Goss-Custard JD, ed). Oxford: Oxford University Press; Ens BJ, Goss-Custard JD, Interference among oystercatchers, Haematopus ostralegus, feeding on mussels, Mytilus edulis, on the Exe estuary. J Anim Ecol 53: Goss-Custard JD, Feeding dispersion in some overwintering wading birds. In: Social behaviour in birds and mammals (Crook JH, ed). London: Academic Press; Goss-Custard JD, Competition for food and interference amongst waders. Ardea 68: Goss-Custard JD, Clarke RT, Durell SEA le V dit, Rates of food intake and aggression of Oystercatchers, Haematopus ostralegus, on the most and least preferred mussel, Mytilus edulis, beds on the Exe estuary. J Anim Ecol 53: Goss-Custard JD, Rothery P, A method for measuring some components of foraging of certain birds in the field. Anim Behav 24: Goss-Custard JD, Sutherland WJ, Individual behaviour, populations and conservation. In: Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach, 4th ed (Krebs JR, Davies NB, eds). Oxford: Blackwell Science; Holmgren N, The ideal free distribution of unequal competitors: predictions from a behaviour-based functional response. J Anim Ecol 64: Mansour RA, Lipcius RN, Density-dependent foraging and mutual interference in blue crabs. Marine Ecol Prog Ser 72: Moody AL, Houston AI, Interference and the ideal free distribution. Anim Behav 49: Norris KJ, Johnstone IG, Interference competition and the functional response of oystercatchers searching for cockles by touch. Anim Behav 56: Parker GA, Sutherland WJ, Ideal free distribution when individuals differ in competitive ability: phenotype-limited ideal free models. Anim Behav 34: Pienkowski MW, Changes in the foraging pattern of plovers in relation to environmental factors. Anim Behav 31: Ruxton GD, Short term refuge use and stability of predatorprey models. Theor Popul Ecol 47:1 17. Ruxton GD, Gurney WSC, de Roos AM, Interference and generation cycles. Theor Popul Bio 42: Selman J, Goss-Custard JD, Interference between foraging redshank Tringa totanus. Anim Behav 36: Smith JNM, The food searching behaviour of two European thrushes. II. The adaptiveness of the search patterns. Behaviour 49: Stillman RA, Sutherland WJ, The optimal search path in a patchy habitat. J Theor Bio 145: Stillman RA, Caldow RWG, Goss-Custard JD, Alexander MJ, Individual variation in intake rate: the relative importance of foraging efficiency and susceptibility to interference. J Anim Ecol 69: Stillman RA, Goss-Custard JD, Clarke RT, Durell SEA le V dit, Shape of the interference function in a foraging vertebrate. J Anim Ecol 65: Stillman RA, Goss-Custard JD, Caldow RWG, Modelling interference from basic foraging behaviour. J Anim Ecol 66: Sutherland WJ, Aggregation and the ideal free distribution. J Anim Ecol 52: Triplet P, Stillman RA, Goss-Custard JD, Prey abundance and the strength of interference in a foraging shorebird. J Anim Ecol 68: van der Meer J, Ens BJ, Models of interference and their consequences for the spatial distribution of ideal and free predators. J Anim Ecol 66: Vines GA, Spatial consequences of aggressive behaviour in flocks of oystercatchers, Haematopus ostralegus L. Anim Behav 28: Whitehouse MEA, Lubin Y, Competitive foraging in the social spider Stegodyphus dumicola. Anim Behav 58: Yates MG, Stillman RA, Goss-Custard JD, Contrasting interference functions and foraging dispersion in two species of shorebird Charadrii. J Anim Ecol 69:

using behaviour-based models

using behaviour-based models Ecology 2002 71, Predicting the strength of interference more quickly Blackwell Science, Ltd using behaviour-based models RICHARD A. STILLMAN, ALISON E. POOLE, JOHN D. GOSS-CUSTARD, RICHARD W. G. CALDOW,

More information

Foraging in a group is a common pattern of animal behavior,

Foraging in a group is a common pattern of animal behavior, Behavioral Ecology Vol. 11 No. 4: 351 356 An evolutionarily stable strategy for aggressiveness in feeding groups Etienne Sirot Université de Bretagne Sud, 12 avenue Saint-Symphorien, 56 000 Vannes, France

More information

A Hawk-Dove game in kleptoparasitic populations

A Hawk-Dove game in kleptoparasitic populations A Hawk-Dove game in kleptoparasitic populations Mark Broom, Department of Mathematics, University of Sussex, Brighton BN1 9RF, UK. M.Broom@sussex.ac.uk Roger M. Luther, Department of Mathematics, University

More information

An experimental design and a statistical analysis separating interference from exploitation competition

An experimental design and a statistical analysis separating interference from exploitation competition Popul Ecol (8) 5:319 34 DOI 1.17/s1144-8-81-9 FORUM An experimental design and a statistical analysis separating interference from exploitation competition Inon Scharf Æ Ido Filin Æ Ofer Ovadia Received:

More information

BIOS 6150: Ecology Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences

BIOS 6150: Ecology Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences BIOS 6150: Ecology Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences Week 6: Predation and predatory behavior: Lecture summary: Nature of predation. Diet breadth & choice. Optimal foraging. Functional

More information

Foraging. This week in Animal Behaviour. How do animals choose? Do animals choose their food?

Foraging. This week in Animal Behaviour. How do animals choose? Do animals choose their food? This week in Animal Behaviour Lecture 22: Cooperation Lecture 23: Foraging Lecture 24: TBA Text chapters 10-11 LAB: Project 2 proposal seminars Midterm 2 on Tuesday Nov. 8 Foraging 1. Models of foraging

More information

BIOS 3010: Ecology Lecture 8: Predator foraging & prey defense. 2. Predation: 3. Predator diet breadth and preference:

BIOS 3010: Ecology Lecture 8: Predator foraging & prey defense. 2. Predation: 3. Predator diet breadth and preference: BIOS 3010: Ecology Lecture 8: Predator foraging & prey defense 1. Lecture Summary: What is predation? Predator diet breadth. Preference & switching. Optimal foraging. Marginal value theorem. Functional

More information

Citation for published version (APA): Vahl, W. K. (2006). Interference competition among foraging waders s.n.

Citation for published version (APA): Vahl, W. K. (2006). Interference competition among foraging waders s.n. University of Groningen Interference competition among foraging waders Vahl, Wouter Karsten IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from

More information

The ideal free distribution: an analysis of the perceptual limit model

The ideal free distribution: an analysis of the perceptual limit model Evolutionary Ecology Research, 2002, 4: 471 493 The ideal free distribution: an analysis of the perceptual limit model Edmund J. Collins,* Alasdair I. Houston and Alison Lang Centre for Behavioural Biology,

More information

3 Ecological and Evolutionary Principles. Notes for Marine Biology: Function, Biodiversity, Ecology by Jeffrey S. Levinton

3 Ecological and Evolutionary Principles. Notes for Marine Biology: Function, Biodiversity, Ecology by Jeffrey S. Levinton 3 Ecological and Evolutionary Principles Notes for Marine Biology: Function, Biodiversity, Ecology by Jeffrey S. Levinton Ecological hierarchy Population-level processes Individual interactions The Ecological

More information

Predation. Predation & Herbivory. Lotka-Volterra. Predation rate. Total rate of predation. Predator population 10/23/2013. Review types of predation

Predation. Predation & Herbivory. Lotka-Volterra. Predation rate. Total rate of predation. Predator population 10/23/2013. Review types of predation Predation & Herbivory Chapter 14 Predation Review types of predation Carnivory Parasitism Parasitoidism Cannabalism Lotka-Volterra Predators control prey populations and prey control predator populations

More information

Chapter 6 Reading Questions

Chapter 6 Reading Questions Chapter 6 Reading Questions 1. Fill in 5 key events in the re-establishment of the New England forest in the Opening Story: 1. Farmers begin leaving 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. Broadleaf forest reestablished 2.

More information

Kleptoparasitic Melees Modelling Food Stealing Featuring Contests with Multiple Individuals

Kleptoparasitic Melees Modelling Food Stealing Featuring Contests with Multiple Individuals Bull Math Biol 2011 73: 683 699 DOI 10.1007/s11538-010-9546-z ORIGINAL ARTICLE Kleptoparasitic Melees Modelling Food Stealing Featuring Contests with Multiple Individuals M. Broom J. Rychtář Received:

More information

Niche The sum of all interactions a species has with biotic/abiotic components of the environment N-dimensional hypervolume

Niche The sum of all interactions a species has with biotic/abiotic components of the environment N-dimensional hypervolume Niche The sum of all interactions a species has with biotic/abiotic components of the environment N-dimensional hypervolume Each dimension is a biotic or abiotic resource Ecomorphology Ecology (niche)

More information

Additional Case Study: Calculating the Size of a Small Mammal Population

Additional Case Study: Calculating the Size of a Small Mammal Population Student Worksheet LSM 14.1-2 Additional Case Study: Calculating the Size of a Small Mammal Population Objective To use field study data on shrew populations to examine the characteristics of a natural

More information

Building on the Ideal Free Distribution. Tom Tregenza

Building on the Ideal Free Distribution. Tom Tregenza Building on the Ideal Free Distribution Tom Tregenza Tregenza, T. 1995 Building on the ideal free distribution. Advances in Ecological Research 26. 253-302. Building on the ideal free distribution TOM

More information

BIOL 410 Population and Community Ecology. Predation

BIOL 410 Population and Community Ecology. Predation BIOL 410 Population and Community Ecology Predation Intraguild Predation Occurs when one species not only competes with its heterospecific guild member, but also occasionally preys upon it Species 1 Competitor

More information

Discrete evaluation and the particle swarm algorithm

Discrete evaluation and the particle swarm algorithm Volume 12 Discrete evaluation and the particle swarm algorithm Tim Hendtlass and Tom Rodgers Centre for Intelligent Systems and Complex Processes Swinburne University of Technology P. O. Box 218 Hawthorn

More information

OPTIMAL FORAGING MODELS

OPTIMAL FORAGING MODELS 26 OPTIMAL FORAGING MODELS In collaboration with David N. Bonter Objectives Develop a spreadsheet model of foraging choices among two prey types, prey 1 and prey 2. Determine the conditions in which individuals

More information

Levels of Ecological Organization. Biotic and Abiotic Factors. Studying Ecology. Chapter 4 Population Ecology

Levels of Ecological Organization. Biotic and Abiotic Factors. Studying Ecology. Chapter 4 Population Ecology Chapter 4 Population Ecology Lesson 4.1 Studying Ecology Levels of Ecological Organization Biotic and Abiotic Factors The study of how organisms interact with each other and with their environments Scientists

More information

Chapter 4 Population Ecology

Chapter 4 Population Ecology Chapter 4 Population Ecology Lesson 4.1 Studying Ecology Levels of Ecological Organization The study of how organisms interact with each other and with their environments Scientists study ecology at various

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION doi:10.1038/nature11226 Supplementary Discussion D1 Endemics-area relationship (EAR) and its relation to the SAR The EAR comprises the relationship between study area and the number of species that are

More information

Predation risk, unequal competitors and the ideal free distribution

Predation risk, unequal competitors and the ideal free distribution Evolutionary Ecology Research, 1999, 1: 389 409 Predation risk, unequal competitors and the ideal free distribution Tamara C. Grand* and Lawrence M. Dill Behavioural Ecology Research Group, Department

More information

PREDATOR AND PREY HABITAT SELECTION GAMES: THE EFFECTS OF HOW PREY BALANCE FORAGING AND PREDATION RISK

PREDATOR AND PREY HABITAT SELECTION GAMES: THE EFFECTS OF HOW PREY BALANCE FORAGING AND PREDATION RISK ISRAEL JOURNAL OF ZOOLOGY, Vol. 50, 2004, pp. 233 254 PREDATOR AND PREY HABITAT SELECTION GAMES: THE EFFECTS OF HOW PREY BALANCE FORAGING AND PREDATION RISK BARNEY LUTTBEG* AND ANDREW SIH Department of

More information

Interspecific Kleptoparasitism

Interspecific Kleptoparasitism Interspecific Kleptoparasitism submitted by Elizabeth Gallagher for the degree of MSc Mathematical Biology of the University of Bath Department of Mathematical Sciences July-September 2010 COPYRIGHT Attention

More information

HABITAT SELECTION INTRODUCTION. Objectives

HABITAT SELECTION INTRODUCTION. Objectives 25 HABITAT SELECTION In collaboration with David N. Bonter Objectives Develop a spreadsheet model of ideal-free habitat selection. Compare the ideal-free and ideal-despotic habitat selection models. INTRODUCTION

More information

CHAPTER 5. Interactions in the Ecosystem

CHAPTER 5. Interactions in the Ecosystem CHAPTER 5 Interactions in the Ecosystem 1 SECTION 3.3 - THE ECOSYSTEM 2 SECTION 3.3 - THE ECOSYSTEM Levels of Organization Individual one organism from a species. Species a group of organisms so similar

More information

14.1. KEY CONCEPT Every organism has a habitat and a niche. 38 Reinforcement Unit 5 Resource Book

14.1. KEY CONCEPT Every organism has a habitat and a niche. 38 Reinforcement Unit 5 Resource Book 14.1 HABITAT AND NICHE KEY CONCEPT Every organism has a habitat and a niche. A habitat is all of the living and nonliving factors in the area where an organism lives. For example, the habitat of a frog

More information

Comparing male densities and fertilization rates as potential Allee effects in Alaskan and Canadian Ursus maritimus populations

Comparing male densities and fertilization rates as potential Allee effects in Alaskan and Canadian Ursus maritimus populations Comparing male densities and fertilization rates as potential Allee effects in Alaskan and Canadian Ursus maritimus populations Introduction Research suggests that our world today is in the midst of a

More information

BIO S380T Page 1 Summer 2005: Exam 2

BIO S380T Page 1 Summer 2005: Exam 2 BIO S380T Page 1 Part I: Definitions. [5 points for each term] For each term, provide a brief definition that also indicates why the term is important in ecology or evolutionary biology. Where I ve provided

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION Dynamics of predator-prey cycles and the effects of dispersal and the Moran effect Here we describe in more detail the dynamics of predator-prey limit cycles in our model, and the manner in which dispersal

More information

Natal versus breeding dispersal: Evolution in a model system

Natal versus breeding dispersal: Evolution in a model system Evolutionary Ecology Research, 1999, 1: 911 921 Natal versus breeding dispersal: Evolution in a model system Karin Johst 1 * and Roland Brandl 2 1 Centre for Environmental Research Leipzig-Halle Ltd, Department

More information

A population is a group of individuals of the same species, living in a shared space at a specific point in time.

A population is a group of individuals of the same species, living in a shared space at a specific point in time. A population is a group of individuals of the same species, living in a shared space at a specific point in time. A population size refers to the number of individuals in a population. Increase Decrease

More information

Non-independence in Statistical Tests for Discrete Cross-species Data

Non-independence in Statistical Tests for Discrete Cross-species Data J. theor. Biol. (1997) 188, 507514 Non-independence in Statistical Tests for Discrete Cross-species Data ALAN GRAFEN* AND MARK RIDLEY * St. John s College, Oxford OX1 3JP, and the Department of Zoology,

More information

A population is a group of individuals of the same species occupying a particular area at the same time

A population is a group of individuals of the same species occupying a particular area at the same time A population is a group of individuals of the same species occupying a particular area at the same time Population Growth As long as the birth rate exceeds the death rate a population will grow Immigration

More information

Chapter 6 Population and Community Ecology

Chapter 6 Population and Community Ecology Chapter 6 Population and Community Ecology Friedland and Relyea Environmental Science for AP, second edition 2015 W.H. Freeman and Company/BFW AP is a trademark registered and/or owned by the College Board,

More information

14.1 Habitat And Niche

14.1 Habitat And Niche 14.1 Habitat And Niche A habitat differs from a niche. Habitat physical area in which an organism lives Niche each species plays a specific role in an ecosystem niche includes the species habitat, feeding

More information

BIOS 6150: Ecology Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences

BIOS 6150: Ecology Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences BIOS 6150: Ecology Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences Week 7: Dynamics of Predation. Lecture summary: Categories of predation. Linked prey-predator cycles. Lotka-Volterra model. Density-dependence.

More information

Yakın Doğu Üniversitesi Mimarlık Fakültesi Peyzaj Mimarlığı Bölümü. PM 317 Human and Environment Assoc. Prof. Dr. Salih GÜCEL

Yakın Doğu Üniversitesi Mimarlık Fakültesi Peyzaj Mimarlığı Bölümü. PM 317 Human and Environment Assoc. Prof. Dr. Salih GÜCEL Yakın Doğu Üniversitesi Mimarlık Fakültesi Peyzaj Mimarlığı Bölümü PM 317 Human and Environment Assoc. Prof. Dr. Salih GÜCEL Ecology & Ecosystems Principles of Ecology Ecology is the study of the interactions

More information

Ecology Regulation, Fluctuations and Metapopulations

Ecology Regulation, Fluctuations and Metapopulations Ecology Regulation, Fluctuations and Metapopulations The Influence of Density on Population Growth and Consideration of Geographic Structure in Populations Predictions of Logistic Growth The reality of

More information

Chapter 6 Population and Community Ecology. Thursday, October 19, 17

Chapter 6 Population and Community Ecology. Thursday, October 19, 17 Chapter 6 Population and Community Ecology Module 18 The Abundance and Distribution of After reading this module you should be able to explain how nature exists at several levels of complexity. discuss

More information

ON THE INTERPLAY OF PREDATOR SWITCHING AND PREY EVASION IN DETERMINING THE STABILITY OF PREDATOR PREY DYNAMICS

ON THE INTERPLAY OF PREDATOR SWITCHING AND PREY EVASION IN DETERMINING THE STABILITY OF PREDATOR PREY DYNAMICS ISRAEL JOURNAL OF ZOOLOGY, Vol. 50, 2004, pp. 187 205 ON THE INTERPLAY OF PREDATOR SWITCHING AND PREY EVASION IN DETERMINING THE STABILITY OF PREDATOR PREY DYNAMICS TRISTAN KIMBRELL* AND ROBERT D. HOLT

More information

INTERPRETING POPULATION DYNAMICS GRAPH

INTERPRETING POPULATION DYNAMICS GRAPH INTERPRETING POPULATION DYNAMIS GRAPH OJETIVES TASKS Name: To learn about three types of population dynamics graphs To determine which type of graph you constructed from the Pike and Perch Game To interpret

More information

Coevolution of predators and prey

Coevolution of predators and prey Coevolution of predators and prey 1) Evolution of predator-prey interactions 2) Arms race analogy 3) Examples of predator-prey coevolution? 4) Steady-state theory 5) Evolution of aposematic coloration

More information

Predation is.. The eating of live organisms, regardless of their identity

Predation is.. The eating of live organisms, regardless of their identity Predation Predation Predation is.. The eating of live organisms, regardless of their identity Predation 1)Moves energy and nutrients through the ecosystem 2)Regulates populations 3)Weeds the unfit from

More information

Population Ecology. Study of populations in relation to the environment. Increase population size= endangered species

Population Ecology. Study of populations in relation to the environment. Increase population size= endangered species Population Basics Population Ecology Study of populations in relation to the environment Purpose: Increase population size= endangered species Decrease population size = pests, invasive species Maintain

More information

The DEPONS project Disturbance Effects on the Harbour Porpoise Population in the North Sea

The DEPONS project Disturbance Effects on the Harbour Porpoise Population in the North Sea The DEPONS project Disturbance Effects on the Harbour Porpoise Population in the North Sea Jacob Nabe-Nielsen, Aarhus University ASCOBANS meeting Wilhelmshaven, 20 June 2017 Modelling cumulative effects

More information

Community Structure. Community An assemblage of all the populations interacting in an area

Community Structure. Community An assemblage of all the populations interacting in an area Community Structure Community An assemblage of all the populations interacting in an area Community Ecology The ecological community is the set of plant and animal species that occupy an area Questions

More information

Population Ecology Density dependence, regulation and the Allee effect

Population Ecology Density dependence, regulation and the Allee effect 2/22/15 Population Ecology Density dependence, regulation and the Allee effect ESRM 450 Wildlife Ecology and Conservation Wildlife Populations Groups of animals, all of the same species, that live together

More information

The Problem of Where to Live

The Problem of Where to Live April 5: Habitat Selection: Intro The Problem of Where to Live Physical and biotic environment critically affects fitness An animal's needs may be met only in certain habitats, which should select for

More information

Ch.5 Evolution and Community Ecology How do organisms become so well suited to their environment? Evolution and Natural Selection

Ch.5 Evolution and Community Ecology How do organisms become so well suited to their environment? Evolution and Natural Selection Ch.5 Evolution and Community Ecology How do organisms become so well suited to their environment? Evolution and Natural Selection Gene: A sequence of DNA that codes for a particular trait Gene pool: All

More information

The Living World Continued: Populations and Communities

The Living World Continued: Populations and Communities The Living World Continued: Populations and Communities Ecosystem Communities Populations Review: Parts of an Ecosystem 1) An individual in a species: One organism of a species. a species must be genetically

More information

Age (x) nx lx. Population dynamics Population size through time should be predictable N t+1 = N t + B + I - D - E

Age (x) nx lx. Population dynamics Population size through time should be predictable N t+1 = N t + B + I - D - E Population dynamics Population size through time should be predictable N t+1 = N t + B + I - D - E Time 1 N = 100 20 births 25 deaths 10 immigrants 15 emmigrants Time 2 100 + 20 +10 25 15 = 90 Life History

More information

Part 2: Models of Food-Web Structure

Part 2: Models of Food-Web Structure Part 2: Models of Food-Web Structure Stochastic models of food-web structure -Two Parameters: S (species number) and C (connectance) -Randomly assign each species a niche value from to 1 -Use simple rules

More information

Why Complexity is Different

Why Complexity is Different Why Complexity is Different Yaneer Bar-Yam (Dated: March 21, 2017) One of the hardest things to explain is why complex systems are actually different from simple systems. The problem is rooted in a set

More information

Four aspects of a sampling strategy necessary to make accurate and precise inferences about populations are:

Four aspects of a sampling strategy necessary to make accurate and precise inferences about populations are: Why Sample? Often researchers are interested in answering questions about a particular population. They might be interested in the density, species richness, or specific life history parameters such as

More information

D. Correct! Allelopathy is a form of interference competition in plants. Therefore this answer is correct.

D. Correct! Allelopathy is a form of interference competition in plants. Therefore this answer is correct. Ecology Problem Drill 18: Competition in Ecology Question No. 1 of 10 Question 1. The concept of allelopathy focuses on which of the following: (A) Carrying capacity (B) Limiting resource (C) Law of the

More information

Structure Learning of a Behavior Network for Context Dependent Adaptability

Structure Learning of a Behavior Network for Context Dependent Adaptability Georgia State University ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University Computer Science Theses Department of Computer Science 12-7-26 Structure Learning of a Behavior Network for Context Dependent Adaptability

More information

The University of Chicago

The University of Chicago The University of Chicago Optimal Foraging on Arbitrary Food Distributions and the Definition of Habitat Patches Author(s): Roger Arditi and Bernard Dacorogna Source: The American Naturalist, Vol. 131,

More information

Aggregations on larger scales. Metapopulation. Definition: A group of interconnected subpopulations Sources and Sinks

Aggregations on larger scales. Metapopulation. Definition: A group of interconnected subpopulations Sources and Sinks Aggregations on larger scales. Metapopulation Definition: A group of interconnected subpopulations Sources and Sinks Metapopulation - interconnected group of subpopulations sink source McKillup and McKillup

More information

The process of habitat selection frequently requires individuals

The process of habitat selection frequently requires individuals Behavioral Ecology Vol. 13 No. 2: 280 290 Alternative forms of competition and predation dramatically affect habitat selection under foraging predation-risk trade-offs Tamara C. Grand Department of Zoology,

More information

Population Ecology. Text Readings. Questions to Answer in the Chapter. Chapter Reading:

Population Ecology. Text Readings. Questions to Answer in the Chapter. Chapter Reading: Population Ecology Text Readings Chapter Reading: Chapter # 26 in Audesirk, Audesirk and Byers: Population Growth and Regulation Pg. # 513-534. Questions to Answer in the Chapter How Does Population Size

More information

Bob Van Dolah. Marine Resources Research Institute South Carolina Department of Natural Resources

Bob Van Dolah. Marine Resources Research Institute South Carolina Department of Natural Resources Bob Van Dolah Marine Resources Research Institute South Carolina Department of Natural Resources Shoreline Change Will Occur! On our front beaches where it is not easy to retreat Shoreline Change Will

More information

BIOS 5970: Plant-Herbivore Interactions Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences

BIOS 5970: Plant-Herbivore Interactions Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences BIOS 5970: Plant-Herbivore Interactions Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences D. POPULATION & COMMUNITY DYNAMICS Week 13. Herbivory, predation & parasitism: Lecture summary: Predation:

More information

Evolution and Life in the Ocean

Evolution and Life in the Ocean Characteristics of All Living Things Contain matter in a highly organized state Capture, store and transmit energy; all organisms require energy Capable of reproduction Change through time and adapt to

More information

A NUMERICAL STUDY ON PREDATOR PREY MODEL

A NUMERICAL STUDY ON PREDATOR PREY MODEL International Conference Mathematical and Computational Biology 2011 International Journal of Modern Physics: Conference Series Vol. 9 (2012) 347 353 World Scientific Publishing Company DOI: 10.1142/S2010194512005417

More information

Students will observe how population size can vary from generation to generation in response to changing environmental conditions.

Students will observe how population size can vary from generation to generation in response to changing environmental conditions. Activity Description of "The Natural Selection of Forks and Beans" This lab was modified from an activity authored by Mike Basham and is available on Access Excellence (www.accessexcellence.com) Abstract:

More information

Understanding Populations Section 1. Chapter 8 Understanding Populations Section1, How Populations Change in Size DAY ONE

Understanding Populations Section 1. Chapter 8 Understanding Populations Section1, How Populations Change in Size DAY ONE Chapter 8 Understanding Populations Section1, How Populations Change in Size DAY ONE What Is a Population? A population is a group of organisms of the same species that live in a specific geographical

More information

Modelling prey in discrete time predator-prey systems

Modelling prey in discrete time predator-prey systems 2013 IEEE International Conference on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics Modelling prey in discrete time predator-prey systems Rory Mullan, David H. Glass and Mark McCartney School of Computing and Mathematics,

More information

City, University of London Institutional Repository

City, University of London Institutional Repository City Research Online City, University of London Institutional Repository Citation: Broom, M., Crowe, M. L., Fitzgerald, M. R. and Rychtar, J. (2010). The stochastic modelling of kleptoparasitism using

More information

THETA-LOGISTIC PREDATOR PREY

THETA-LOGISTIC PREDATOR PREY THETA-LOGISTIC PREDATOR PREY What are the assumptions of this model? 1.) Functional responses are non-linear. Functional response refers to a change in the rate of exploitation of prey by an individual

More information

REVISION: POPULATION ECOLOGY 01 OCTOBER 2014

REVISION: POPULATION ECOLOGY 01 OCTOBER 2014 REVISION: POPULATION ECOLOGY 01 OCTOBER 2014 Lesson Description In this lesson we revise: Introduction to Population Ecology What s Happening in the Environment Human Population: Analysis & Predictions

More information

Population modeling of marine mammal populations

Population modeling of marine mammal populations Population modeling of marine mammal populations Lecture 1: simple models of population counts Eli Holmes National Marine Fisheries Service nmfs.noaa.gov Today s lecture topics: Density-independent growth

More information

Interspecific Competition

Interspecific Competition Interspecific Competition Intraspecific competition Classic logistic model Interspecific extension of densitydependence Individuals of other species may also have an effect on per capita birth & death

More information

* Population Dynamics

* Population Dynamics * Population Dynamics Populations are dynamic constantly changing Some are seriously declining and threatened with extinction Others are experiencing growth Therefore biologists are constantly studying

More information

Population Dynamics Graphs

Population Dynamics Graphs Dynamics Graphs OJETIVES: - to learn about three types of population dynamics graphs - to determine which type of graph you constructed from the Pike and Perch Game - to interpret (describe, analyze) graphs

More information

arxiv: v1 [math.ho] 25 Feb 2008

arxiv: v1 [math.ho] 25 Feb 2008 A Note on Walking Versus Waiting Anthony B. Morton February 28 arxiv:82.3653v [math.ho] 25 Feb 28 To what extent is a traveller called Justin, say) better off to wait for a bus rather than just start walking

More information

Principles of Ecology BL / ENVS 402 Exam II Name:

Principles of Ecology BL / ENVS 402 Exam II Name: Principles of Ecology BL / ENVS 402 Exam II 10-26-2011 Name: There are three parts to this exam. Use your time wisely as you only have 50 minutes. Part One: Circle the BEST answer. Each question is worth

More information

Predation. Vine snake eating a young iguana, Panama. Vertebrate predators: lions and jaguars

Predation. Vine snake eating a young iguana, Panama. Vertebrate predators: lions and jaguars Predation Vine snake eating a young iguana, Panama Vertebrate predators: lions and jaguars 1 Most predators are insects Parasitoids lay eggs in their hosts, and the larvae consume the host from the inside,

More information

The East of Nantucket Survey. Preliminary Results Presented by Eric Powell to the Habitat PDT on September 14, 2017

The East of Nantucket Survey. Preliminary Results Presented by Eric Powell to the Habitat PDT on September 14, 2017 The East of Nantucket Survey Preliminary Results Presented by Eric Powell to the Habitat PDT on September 14, 2017 Thanks Roger Mann who handled the logistics of the cruise Tom Dameron and others who provided

More information

Discrete Evaluation and the Particle Swarm Algorithm.

Discrete Evaluation and the Particle Swarm Algorithm. Abstract Discrete Evaluation and the Particle Swarm Algorithm. Tim Hendtlass and Tom Rodgers, Centre for Intelligent Systems and Complex Processes, Swinburne University of Technology, P. O. Box 218 Hawthorn

More information

Chapter 9 Population Dynamics, Carrying Capacity, and Conservation Biology

Chapter 9 Population Dynamics, Carrying Capacity, and Conservation Biology Chapter 9 Population Dynamics, Carrying Capacity, and Conservation Biology 9-1 Population Dynamics & Carrying Capacity Populations change in response to enviromental stress or changes in evironmental conditions

More information

Measuring the population-level consequences of predator-induced prey movement

Measuring the population-level consequences of predator-induced prey movement Evolutionary Ecology Research, 2008, 10: 333 350 Measuring the population-level consequences of predator-induced prey movement Peter A. Abrams* Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, Zoology Building,

More information

Module 5: Rise and Fall of the Clockwork Universe. You should be able to demonstrate and show your understanding of:

Module 5: Rise and Fall of the Clockwork Universe. You should be able to demonstrate and show your understanding of: OCR B Physics H557 Module 5: Rise and Fall of the Clockwork Universe You should be able to demonstrate and show your understanding of: 5.2: Matter Particle model: A gas consists of many very small, rapidly

More information

Ecology Symbiotic Relationships

Ecology Symbiotic Relationships Ecology Symbiotic Relationships Overview of the Co-evolution and Relationships Exhibited Among Community Members What does Symbiosis mean? How do we define Symbiosis? Symbiosis in the broadest sense is

More information

Measurements and Data Analysis

Measurements and Data Analysis Measurements and Data Analysis 1 Introduction The central point in experimental physical science is the measurement of physical quantities. Experience has shown that all measurements, no matter how carefully

More information

History and meaning of the word Ecology A. Definition 1. Oikos, ology - the study of the house - the place we live

History and meaning of the word Ecology A. Definition 1. Oikos, ology - the study of the house - the place we live History and meaning of the word Ecology. Definition 1. Oikos, ology - the study of the house - the place we live. Etymology - origin and development of the the word 1. Earliest - Haeckel (1869) - comprehensive

More information

dv dt Predator-Prey Models

dv dt Predator-Prey Models Predator-Prey Models This is a diverse area that includes general models of consumption: Granivores eating seeds Parasitoids Parasite-host interactions Lotka-Voterra model prey and predator: V = victim

More information

Ecology! the study of the relationship between organisms and their environment

Ecology! the study of the relationship between organisms and their environment Ecology Notes Ecology! the study of the relationship between organisms and their environment An ecosystem is an organism s surroundings consisting of both living and nonliving things. Abiotic NONliving

More information

Effects to Communities & Ecosystems

Effects to Communities & Ecosystems Biology 5868 Ecotoxicology Effects to Communities & Ecosystems April 18, 2007 Definitions Ecological Community an assemblage of populations living in a prescribed area or physical habitat [It is] the living

More information

Chapter 8. Biogeographic Processes. Upon completion of this chapter the student will be able to:

Chapter 8. Biogeographic Processes. Upon completion of this chapter the student will be able to: Chapter 8 Biogeographic Processes Chapter Objectives Upon completion of this chapter the student will be able to: 1. Define the terms ecosystem, habitat, ecological niche, and community. 2. Outline how

More information

Lecture 2: Individual-based Modelling

Lecture 2: Individual-based Modelling Lecture 2: Individual-based Modelling Part I Steve Railsback Humboldt State University Department of Mathematics & Lang, Railsback & Associates Arcata, California USA www.langrailsback.com 1 Outline 1.

More information

Name Student ID. Good luck and impress us with your toolkit of ecological knowledge and concepts!

Name Student ID. Good luck and impress us with your toolkit of ecological knowledge and concepts! Page 1 BIOLOGY 150 Final Exam Winter Quarter 2000 Before starting be sure to put your name and student number on the top of each page. MINUS 3 POINTS IF YOU DO NOT WRITE YOUR NAME ON EACH PAGE! You have

More information

Dynamical Systems and Chaos Part II: Biology Applications. Lecture 6: Population dynamics. Ilya Potapov Mathematics Department, TUT Room TD325

Dynamical Systems and Chaos Part II: Biology Applications. Lecture 6: Population dynamics. Ilya Potapov Mathematics Department, TUT Room TD325 Dynamical Systems and Chaos Part II: Biology Applications Lecture 6: Population dynamics Ilya Potapov Mathematics Department, TUT Room TD325 Living things are dynamical systems Dynamical systems theory

More information

Ecology Notes Part 1. Abiotic NONliving components in an ecosystem. Ecosystem

Ecology Notes Part 1. Abiotic NONliving components in an ecosystem. Ecosystem Ecology Notes Part 1 Ecology the study of the relationship between organisms and their environment Ecosystem an organism s surroundings consisting of both living and nonliving things and how that organism

More information

B2 Revision Questions Part 1

B2 Revision Questions Part 1 B2 Revision Questions Part 1 Higher only questions are underlined Question 1 What are the two different ways that things can be classified? Answer 1 Artificially and naturally Question 2 What is natural

More information

Modeling Fish Assemblages in Stream Networks Representation of Stream Network Introduction habitat attributes Criteria for Success

Modeling Fish Assemblages in Stream Networks Representation of Stream Network Introduction habitat attributes Criteria for Success Modeling Fish Assemblages in Stream Networks Joan P. Baker and Denis White Western Ecology Division National Health & Environmental Effects Research Laboratory U.S. Environmental Protection Agency baker.joan@epa.gov

More information

City, University of London Institutional Repository

City, University of London Institutional Repository City Research Online City, University of London Institutional Repository Citation: Broom, M. and Rychtar, J. (016). A model of food stealing with asymmetric information. Ecological Complexity, 6, pp. 137-14.

More information

If you are in sections 2,4,6,8,18 or Bio 105 You will be doing the reverse: Exercise 6 the week of February 11 th.

If you are in sections 2,4,6,8,18 or Bio 105 You will be doing the reverse: Exercise 6 the week of February 11 th. Note: If you are in sections: 1,3,5,7,9,10,11,13,15,17 you will be doing Exercise 5: Population Growth the week of February 11 th and Exercise 6 during the week of February 18 th. If you are in sections

More information

Evolution 1 Star. 6. The different tools used during the beaks of finches lab represented. A. feeding adaptations in finches

Evolution 1 Star. 6. The different tools used during the beaks of finches lab represented. A. feeding adaptations in finches Name: Date: 1. ccording to modern evolutionary theory, genes responsible for new traits that help a species survive in a particular environment will usually. not change in frequency. decrease gradually

More information