using behaviour-based models

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "using behaviour-based models"

Transcription

1 Ecology , Predicting the strength of interference more quickly Blackwell Science, Ltd using behaviour-based models RICHARD A. STILLMAN, ALISON E. POOLE, JOHN D. GOSS-CUSTARD, RICHARD W. G. CALDOW, MICHAEL G. YATES* and PATRICK TRIPLET Centre for Ecology and Hydrology Dorset, Winfrith Technology Centre, Winfrith Newburgh, Dorchester, Dorset DT2 8ZD, UK; *Centre for Ecology and Hydrology Monks Wood, Abbots Ripton, Huntingdon, PE17 2LS, UK; and RN Baie de Somme, SMACOPI, 1 Place Amiral Courbet, F Abbeville, France Summary 1. Interference between foraging animals can be quantified directly only through intensive studies. A quicker alternative is to predict the strength of interference using behaviour-based models. We describe a field method to parameterize an interference model for shorebirds, Charadrii. 2. Kleptoparasitic attack distance is the main factor affecting the strength of interference but has rarely been measured. Attack distance is related to handling time, a frequently measured parameter, allowing the model to be parameterized for systems in which attack distance has not been measured. 3. The model accurately predicts the strength of interference between oystercatchers Haematopus ostralegus L. feeding on cockles Cerastoderma edule L. and the absence of interference between bar-tailed godwits Limosa lapponica L. feeding on lugworms Arenicola marina L. at low competitor densities. 4. We predict the strength of interference in black-tailed godwit Limosa limosa L. and oystercatcher systems in which it has not been measured previously. The strength of interference is almost entirely determined by attack distance; interference is stronger in systems with longer attacks. Interference is usually weaker in black-tailed godwits because handling time is generally shorter and this limits attack distance. 5. The interference model can be parameterized much more quickly than the alternative of measuring interference directly. Behaviour-based models have the potential to be a valuable tool for predicting the strength of interference. Key-words: attack distance, handling time, kleptoparasitism, pace length, shorebirds Charadrii. Ecology (2002) 71, Ecological Society Introduction Interference is a major component of food competition but is difficult to measure in natural animal populations (Ens & Cayford 1996). Empirical studies of vertebrates have usually taken a decline in intake rate with increasing competitor density as evidence for interference (e.g. Zwarts & Drent 1981; Ens & Goss-Custard 1984; Goss-Custard & Durell 1988; Dolman 1995; Stillman et al. 1996; Cresswell 1997; Triplet, Stillman & Goss-Custard 1999), but there is no guarantee that any decline is due to interference, as other confounding Correspondence: Richard Stillman, Centre for Ecology and Hydrology Dorset, Winfrith Technology Centre, Winfrith Newburgh, Dorchester, Dorset DT2 8ZD, UK. Tel: rast@ceh.ac.uk factors may exist (Ens & Cayford 1996). Alternatively, the lack of a relationship does not necessarily mean that interference is absent, only that it could not be detected. Additionally, empirical results may only apply to the situation for which the data were collected, as the strength of interference may vary with environmental conditions (e.g. Stillman et al. 1996; Cresswell 1998; Triplet et al. 1999). To provide convincing, direct evidence for interference, long-term studies are required, and even then, the results may not apply to new situations. An alternative, potentially more rapid and general approach, is to predict the strength of interference using behaviour-based models (e.g. Beddington 1975; Ruxton, Gurney & de Roos 1992; Holmgren 1995; Moody & Houston 1995; Stillman, Goss-Custard & Caldow 1997). These models have made successful

2 533 Predicting the strength of interference qualitative predictions (e.g. Dolman 1995; Stillman et al. 1996; Cresswell 1998; Triplet et al. 1999), but only one has been tested quantitatively and for only one system (Stillman et al. 1997, 2000). The mechanism of interference in most models is direct interaction between animals, such as prey stealing (kleptoparasitism). One key factor determining the strength of interference is the distance over which kleptoparasitic attacks occur (Stillman et al. 1997). However, attack distances have rarely been measured, and are more difficult to measure than parameters such as handling time. In this paper we show how a behaviour-based model can predict the strength of interference more quickly than measuring interference directly. We describe a field method to parameterize the model. Ideally, all parameters should be measured directly, but this may not always be possible. Therefore, we show how kleptoparasitic attack distance may be predicted from handling time, which is more easily and frequently measured, to show how the model can be applied when attack distance is unknown or cannot be measured. To determine the reliability of our approach, we quantitatively test the model for two new systems. We then make predictions for several systems in which interference has not been measured directly and show why the strength of interference varies between these systems. We study three shorebirds, the oystercatcher Haematopus ostralegus L., black-tailed godwit Limosa limosa L. and bar-tailed godwit Limosa Lapponica L. The model The model is described by Stillman et al. (1997, 2000). It progresses in discrete time steps and follows the location and behaviour of each animal within a population as they encounter prey and competitors. Animals can be searching for or handling prey, fighting over prey or avoiding competitors (Fig. 1). They either find prey independently (Fig. 1a) or steal prey from a competitor (Fig. 1b). Interference occurs when animals waste time avoiding (Fig. 1c) and fighting (Fig. 1b), and when prey is stolen (Fig. 1b). The responses to competitors are calculated using intake rate-maximizing decision rules. The model assumes a strict dominance hierarchy, so that the more dominant individual in a dispute always wins regardless of the absolute difference in dominance. We used a model assuming a dominance hierarchy and optimal decision rules because its predictions were more accurate than simpler models (Stillman et al. 1997). Previous model versions contained the following parameters: prey encounter rate, prey handling time, kleptoparasitic attack distance, kleptoparasitic dispute duration, the probability of stealing prey, searching speed, competitor avoidance distance, avoidance time and the amount of individual variation in foraging efficiency. To simplify parameterization, we assumed that avoidance distance equalled attack distance, and that avoidance time was one time step. Avoidance and Fig. 1. How model animals change behaviour (t = time, d = distance between animals). (a) Prey capture a searching animal encounters prey, handles it for T H seconds, consumes it and then resumes searching. (b) Prey stealing a searching animal approaches a less dominant animal which is handling prey to within the attack distance (D A ). It initiates a fight over the prey which lasts for T K seconds, after which (in this example) it wins and starts handling the prey. The loser changes direction and starts searching directly away from the winner. (c) Competitor avoidance two animals approach each other to within the attack distance. The less dominant animal starts avoiding by changing direction and moving directly away from the more dominant animal (for 1 s in this example) until separated by more than the attack distance, after which it resumes searching. The more dominant animal does not change its behaviour. Adapted from Stillman et al. (1997). attack distances were equal in Stillman et al. (1997) and similar in Stillman et al. (2000), and avoidance time was previously two time steps, so the changes to avoidance distance and time were not great. Additionally, the predicted strength of interference is relatively insensitive to these parameters (Stillman et al. 1997, 2000). The changes meant that animals continued to avoid until there were no dominant neighbours within the attack distance (Fig. 1c). We could not measure individual variation in foraging efficiency and so, as in Stillman et al. (1997), this parameter was excluded. The model predicts the intake rate of each individual within the model population. In this paper, we present the average intake rate of all individuals because the observed intake rates used to test the model were also population averages. Stillman et al. (2000) shows how predicted intake rate varies among individuals.

3 534 R.A. Stillman et al. Methods Groups of birds, feeding close enough to each other so that kleptoparasitism would be possible, were located. They were then videoed either until they became too dispersed or until enough data had been recorded. The video field of view was set wide enough to allow several birds to be videoed simultaneously (to increase the chance of observing kleptoparasitism), but not so wide that their behaviour could not be determined. As far as possible, the camera was held still so that the movement of birds could be measured relative to the fixed field of view (see below). The following parameters were recorded. PREY ENCOUNTER RATE (λ) Prey encounter rate was measured by following individual birds from when they appeared on the screen or finished handling prey, until they walked off the screen or located prey. We recorded the amount of time each bird spent searching for prey (excluding time attacking or avoiding others) and whether it found prey. The prey encounter rate for each system was the total number of prey located by all birds observed (N P ) divided by the total time spent searching by all birds (T S ). λ = N P. T eqn 1 In one system, oystercatchers fought over patches of prey, not individual prey. Within a patch, birds fed while standing still or moving slowly, but moved more rapidly between patches. In this case, we recorded the time each bird spent searching for patches and whether it found a patch. N P was the number of patches found by all birds and T S the total time spent searching for patches. PREY HANDLING TIME (T H ) Handling time was measured as the time between a bird s bill first making contact with the prey, or the substrate in the case of buried prey, and the last piece of prey being swallowed. Handling time was only measured for prey captured independently (i.e. not stolen) and over which no kleptoparasitic attacks occurred. In the system in which oystercatchers fought over patches, handling time was the time a bird spent feeding in a patch. KLEPTOPARASITIC DISPUTE TIME (T K ) The duration of kleptoparasitic disputes was calculated as the average time spent by the aggressor moving towards the victim (T A ) and by the loser (nearly always the victim) avoiding the winner (T V ), plus the time spent by the two birds in any escalated fight over the prey (T F ). T K S TA + T = 2 V + T. F eqn 2 The average of attack and avoidance time was calculated to obtain a single estimate the time spent by both the aggressor and victim. Only disputes in which the aggressor reached the victim and the full duration of avoidance was observed were included in calculations. ATTACK DISTANCE (D A ) Birds made kleptoparasitic attacks by moving rapidly towards a stationary victim. In nearly all cases, birds attacked by running, not flying (100% of black-tailed godwit fights, 90% of oystercatcher fights), allowing attack distance to be estimated from the number of paces and pace length. Distance was calculated from either (i) the number of paces taken by aggressors which reached the victim or (ii) where the victim was not reached, from the number of paces taken plus an estimate of the extra number needed to reach the victim. D A = L A (N A + N X ), eqn 3 where L A = attacking pace length, N A = number of paces made by aggressor moving towards victim and N X = number of extra paces needed for aggressor to reach victim. The aggressor usually reached the victim (95% of godwit fights, 90% of oystercatcher fights) and so the average values of N X (0 1 in godwits, 0 3 in oystercatchers) were small compared to N A (5 8 in godwits, 11 3 in oystercatchers). Attacking pace lengths were measured from attacks made directly across the screen. The aggressor s maximum body length, near the start and end of the attack, and attack distance were measured in screen pixels. Body length was calculated as the maximum length from the tail tip to the base of the bill, plus bill length. Two measures were combined because the body and bill seldom formed a straight line. We estimated maximum body length because this approximated most closely the lengths of dead birds (see below). Attack distance was converted from pixels to bird-lengths by dividing the number of pixels covered by the attack by the average length of the bird measured in pixels. This distance was converted from bird-lengths to m by multiplying the number of bird-lengths covered by the attack by the average length of an individual of the appropriate species. Observed bird lengths were the mid-point of the range of lengths given by Hayman, Marchant & Prater (1986) for dead birds: black-tailed godwit = 0 40 m; oystercatcher = 0 43 m. The length of each pace was calculated by dividing the distance covered by the number of paces. SEARCHING SPEED (S S ) We measured searching speed from birds which were constantly searching for prey and not involved in aggressive disputes. Searching speed was calculated from the time spent moving and the number and length of paces.

4 535 Predicting the strength of interference Table 1. Pace lengths and rates (mean ± standard deviation (sample size)) of black-tailed godwits and oystercatchers searching for prey and attacking a competitor. Sample size is the number of searching bouts or attacks from which pace lengths and rates were estimated. To increase sample size, data from different sites have been merged. Pace lengths from three previous studies are also shown Present study Previous studies Species and behaviour Pace rate (s 1 ) Pace length (m) (1) (2) (3) Black-tailed godwit Searching 2 1 ± 1 0 (20) 0 13 ± 0 03 (20) 0 12 Attacking 4 0 ± 1 2 (17) 0 17 ± 0 03 (17) Oystercatcher Searching 2 3 ± 1 0 (20) 0 12 ± 0 03 (20) Attacking 5 9 ± 2 0 (54) 0 18 ± 0 05 (54) (1) Mean value in Table 3 of Goss-Custard (1970), calculated from footprints of birds walking at low pace rates (black-tailed godwit = 0 8 s 1 ; oystercatcher = 1 6 s 1 ). (2) Derived from pace rate using relationships in Table 1 of Speakman & Bryant (1993), calculated from birds walking over a known distance. (3) Derived from pace rate using relationship on page 64 of Sitters (2000), calculated from birds walking over a known distance. S S LN S = T eqn 4 where L S = searching pace length, N S = number of paces while searching and T M = time spent moving. Searching pace lengths were estimated from birds searching directly across the screen using the same approach as for attacking pace length. PROBABILITY OF STEALING PREY AFTER DISPLACING A VICTIM (P S ) An aggressor was recorded as displacing a victim if the victim retreated as the aggressor approached or after a short escalated dispute. The probability of displacing a victim was calculated as the number of disputes in which victims were displaced divided by the total number of disputes. Even if an aggressor displaced a victim it did not necessarily obtain the prey if the victim escaped with the prey or if neither bird relocated the prey. The probability of stealing prey, after displacing a victim, was calculated as the number of disputes in which an aggressor displaced a victim and captured the prey, divided by the total number of disputes in which a victim was displaced. Results M S ACCURACY OF ATTACKING AND SEARCHING PACE LENGTHS Attack distance and searching speed were calculated from the pace lengths of attacking and searching birds. Due to the importance of these parameters (Stillman et al. 1997), we compared our estimates of pace length with previous estimates (Table 1). Pace lengths have not been related previously to behaviour, but have been related to pace rate. Pace rate was more rapid in attacking than in searching birds (Table 1) and so we compared attacking and searching pace lengths with previous estimates from birds with either high or low pacing rates. Previous estimates of pace length in oystercatchers encompassed the values measured in this study: pace lengths in slowly pacing birds ranged from 0 09 to 0 13 m, compared to our figure of 0 12 m for searching birds; and pace lengths in rapidly pacing birds ranged from 0 16 to 0 21 m, compared to our figure of 0 18 m for attacking birds. The previous estimate of pace length in slowly pacing black-tailed godwits, 0 12 m, was also close to our figure for searching birds, 0 13 m. No data were available to compare with attacking pace length in black-tailed godwit, so we compared the attacking pace lengths of godwits with oystercatchers. Attacking godwits had pacing rates 1 9 and pace lengths 1 3 times greater than searching godwits. The equivalent values in oystercatchers were 2 6 and 1 5. These comparisons gave us no reason to suspect the value of attacking pace length in godwits; when attacking, godwits increased both their pacing rates and pace lengths slightly less than oystercatchers. We therefore considered that our estimates of attacking and searching pace lengths were reliable. MECHANISM OF INTERFERENCE IN OYSTERCATCHERS AND BLACK-TAILED GODWITS Despite short handling times in several systems (Table 2), limiting the opportunity for kleptoparasitic attacks, prey stealing was observed in all systems. Attacks were similar in the two species. Attack distance varied from 0 7 to 3 6 m (Table 2). Aggressors nearly always successfully displaced their victims; 91% in black-tailed godwits and 92% in oystercatchers. In virtually all cases, victims immediately retreated, rather than escalating the dispute by standing their ground. Consequently, the overall duration of disputes

5 536 Table 2. Parameter values (mean ± standard deviation (sample size)) for the model. Standard deviations were not calculated for λ and P S. Sample size for R.A. λ is the Stillman number of et prey al. located and for P S the number of fights in which a victim was displaced. Birds fought over prey items in all systems except 8, in which they fought over prey patches; in this system, T H is patch handling time and λ patch encounter rate. Data for systems 4 and 7 were combined to calculate P S, as no prey were stolen successfully in system 7 System Site and habitat T H (s) λ (s 1 ) S S (ms 1 ) D A (m) P S T K (s) Black-tailed Godwit 1. Earthworms Lumbricus sp. Exe Estuary, England (wet grassland) ±4 1 (23) (25) ±0 08 (54) ±0 3 (25) (31) ±0 9 (29) 2. Ragworms Hediste diversicolor Exe Estuary, England (mudflat) ±4 9 (26) (21) ±0 07 (58) ±0 7 (15) (11) ±0 5 (11) 3. Scrobicularia plana Exe Estuary, England (mudflat) ±4 3 (48) (61) ±0 05 (70) ±0 3 (9) (16) ±0 7 (16) Oystercatcher 4. Small Polychaetes Bangor Flats, Wales (mussel bed) ±2 0 (76) (48) ±0 03 (60) ±1 0 (6) (17) ±0 6 (6) 5. Earthworms Lumbricus sp. Exe Estuary, England (grassland) ±3 1 (63) (34) ±0 02 (52) ±0 7 (73) (78) ±0 9 (76) 6. Small Spisula solida Exe Estuary, England (sandflat) ±3 7 (10) (35) ±0 01 (54) ±0 9 (42) (52) ±0 8 (49) 7. Polychaetes Bangor Flats, Wales (mudflat) ±8 1 (51) (61) ±0 03 (53) ±0 6 (10) (17) ±1 5 (11) 8. Diptera larvae patches Kimmeridge Bay, England (strandline) ±8 4 (30) (33) ±0 07 (42) ±1 0 (5) (9) ±0 8 (5) 9. Large Spisula solida Exe Estuary, England (sandflat) ±7 9 (18) (28) ±0 16 (61) ±2 0 (9) (9) ±1 3 (9) 10. Small mussels Mytilus edulis Seine Estuary, France (mussel bed) ±26 6 (30) (33) ±0 01 (55) ±1 2 (65) (86) ±1 7 (84) 11. Cockles Cerastoderma edule Exe Estuary, England (mudflat) ±27 2 (30) (28) ±0 01 (58) ±1 4 (19) (27) ±2 1 (27) 12. Large mussels Mytilus edulis Exe Estuary, England (mussel bed) ±115 8 (59) (31) ±0 01 (52) ±1 4 (53) (67) ±2 0 (67) was relatively short, ranging from 1 5 to 3 2 s. Having displaced a victim, the aggressor successfully stole the prey in 37% of black-tailed godwit fights and 32% of oystercatcher fights. Prey stealing was profitable in all systems; a bird could increase its intake rate by attempting to steal prey from a less dominant bird rather than searching independently (Fig. 2). This was because the time to steal prey was shorter than the time to find prey independently. In contrast, being attacked reduced the intake rate of victims as they wasted time in the dispute and sometimes lost their prey. One potential mechanism of interference was therefore present in all systems. RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN ATTACK DISTANCE AND HANDLING TIME To allow the model to be parameterized for systems in which attack distance cannot or has not been measured, we related attack distance to handling time. We anticipated that attack distance and handling time were related because, unless aggressors can anticipate when a potential victim will start to handle prey, which seems unlikely for the shorebirds systems studied, they have only the duration of handling time in which to complete an attack. Handling time therefore imposes the upper limit on the duration, and hence distance of attacks. Attack distance (D A ) was related non-linearly to handling time (Fig. 3); in the nine systems with handling times of 15 s or less, attack distance was related positively to handling time (linear regression of individual attack distances against system handling times: D A = T H ; P < ; n = 193), whereas there was no relationship in the five systems with handling times longer than 10 s (D A = T H ; P > 0 05; n = 151). We fitted a hyperbolic function to describe this relationship. D A D = T TH, + T MAX 50 H eqn 5 where D MAX = maximum attack distance and T 50 = handling time at which attack distance is 50% of its maximum value. We used this function because (i) it predicts an attack distance of zero when handling time is zero (as handling time imposes the upper limit on the duration of attacks, attack distance must approach zero as handling time approaches zero) and (ii) the linear regressions showed that attack distance approached an asymptotic value as handling time increased. Nonlinear regression (NLIN procedure of SAS version 8) of individual attack distances against system handling times was used to estimate parameter values: D MAX = 2 74 m (95% confidence limits = ) and T 50 = 5 1 s (95% confidence limits = ) (Fig. 3). This model explained 17% of the among-system variation in attack distance compared with 8% explained by a linear regression (D A = T H ; P < ; n = 330). Both equation 5 and the regression equation

6 537 Predicting the strength of interference Fig. 3. Observed relationship between kleptoparasitic attack distance (±95% confidence interval) and handling time in black-tailed godwits (open symbols) and oystercatchers (closed symbols). The line shows equation 5 fitted to the data (see text for parameter values). Fig. 2. Profitability of prey stealing in black-tailed godwits and oystercatchers. For each system, the graph shows the intake rate obtained through independent foraging (1/[(1/ λ) + T H ]; Holling 1959) and the profitability for a bird stealing prey from a subdominant competitor (1/[(T K /P S ) + (T H /2)]; Stillman et al. 1997). had the same number of parameters and so this was further evidence of a nonlinear relationship. TEST OF THE INTERFERENCE MODEL FOR OYSTERCATCHERS AND BAR-TAILED GODWITS Except for mussel Mytilus edulis L.-feeding oystercatchers, the strength of interference has not been measured directly in any of the systems in Table 2, and so predictions could not be tested for these systems. Therefore, we tested the model for the only systems for which data were available. We have shown previously that the model predicts accurately the strength of interference in mussel-feeding oystercatchers (Stillman et al. 1997, 2000). As two further tests, we tested predictions for cockle Cerastoderma edule L.-feeding oystercatchers (Triplet et al. 1999) and lugworm Arenicola marina L.-feeding bar-tailed godwits (Yates, Stillman & Goss-Custard 2000). Attack distances have not been measured in these systems, and so we used equation 5 to predict attack distances from handling times. Table 3 lists the parameter values used. The model predicted accurately the shape of the relationship between intake rate and competitor density (Fig. 4) in cockle-feeding oystercatchers; intake rate varied little with density up to about birds ha 1, after which it declined with increasing density. The model predicted that the intake rate of bar-tailed godwits should not be related to competitor density below about 150 birds ha 1. The observed intake rate was also unrelated to competitor density within this range, but unfortunately no data were available to test the model s predictions above 150 birds ha 1. Predictions overlapped the observed 95% confidence intervals of five of the seven competitor density ranges in oystercatchers and seven of the eight ranges in godwits. These and the previous tests suggest that the predictions for the remaining systems, although untestable, are likely to be similar to the true interference relationships. Table 3. Parameter values used to predict the strength of interference between cockle-feeding oystercatchers in the Baie de Somme, France and lugworm-feeding bar-tailed godwits in The Wash, England Parameter Oystercatcher Bar-tailed godwit Prey encounter rate (λ) s 1 (1) s 1 (2) Prey handling time (T H ) 14 s (1) 12 s (3) Searching speed (S S ) 0 22 ms 1 (4) 0 17 ms 1 (2) Attack distance (D A ) 2 0 m (5) 1 9 m (5) Kleptoparasitic dispute time (T K ) 3 s (4) 2 s (6) Probability of stealing prey (P S ) 0 19 (4) 0 37 (6) (1) Measured from the data used by Triplet et al. (1999). (2) Measured from the data used by Yates et al. (2000). (3) M. G. Yates, personal observation. (4) Measured for cockle-feeding oystercatchers in this study (Table 2). (5) Predicted from handling time using Equation 5. (6) Average value measured for black-tailed godwits in this study (Table 2).

7 538 R.A. Stillman et al. Fig. 4. Predicted and observed strength of interference in (a) cockle-feeding oystercatchers in the Baie de Somme, France and (b) lugworm-feeding bar-tailed godwits in The Wash, England. The lines show the average intake rates of birds within the model. The symbols show the observed intake rates (mean ± 95% confidence limits) of birds feeding in different competitor density ranges. The oystercatcher data are from Fig. 3b of Triplet et al. (1999) and the godwit data are a combination of the data from Figs 1b and 2b of Yates et al. (2000). Intake rates are expressed as a percentage of those achieved in the absence of interference from competitors. See Table 3 for parameter values. PREDICTED STRENGTH OF INTERFERENCE IN OYSTERCATCHERS AND BLACK-TAILED GODWITS Figure 5 shows examples of the interference relationships derived from the parameter values in Table 2. Figure 6 shows two summary statistics for each relationship; D 95, the competitor density at which intake rate is 95% of that in the absence of interference and m 1000, the strength of interference at 1000 birds ha 1, measured as the proportional decrease in intake rate with a proportional change in density. Interference was predicted to reduce intake rate in all systems. The general shape of the interference relationship was the same in all cases (Fig. 5); interference was insignificant at low competitor densities, but increased steadily in intensity as competitor density increased. However, the values of D 95 and m 1000 varied widely among different systems (Fig. 6). In order to determine the causes of this variation, we regressed D 95 and m 1000 against various combinations of the model s parameters. We log 10 transformed D 95 because trials showed Fig. 5. Examples of predicted interference relationships in (a) black-tailed godwits and (b) oystercatchers. These examples were chosen to highlight the range of interference relationships predicted in the two species. The relationships are the average intake rates of birds within the model. Intake rates are expressed as a percentage of those achieved in the absence interference from competitors. See Table 2 for parameter values. that this increased the amount of its variation explained. Attack distance explained the greatest proportion of among-system variation in both variables (Fig. 7): log 10 (D 95 ) = D A, P < 0 001, r 2 = 73 1%; m 1000 = D A, P < 0 001, r 2 = 98 1%. Of the remaining parameters, searching speed was related significantly to both D 95 and m 1000 when regressed in combination with attack distance: log 10 (D 95 ) = D A 1 03 S S, P DA < 0 001, P SS < 0 05, r 2 = 83 2%; m 1000 = D A S S, P DA < 0 001, P SS < 0 05, r 2 = 99 0%. D 95 was related negatively, and m 1000 related positively to both attack distance and searching speed. No other variables were related significantly to either variable when regressed in combination with attack distance. Interference was weaker in godwits than in most oystercatcher systems (Fig. 5); D 95 was larger and m 1000 smaller (Fig. 6). This was because godwit handling times were generally shorter (Table 2), causing attack distances to be shorter (Fig. 3) and hence less interference to be predicted (Fig. 7). Discussion Obtaining direct estimates of the strength of interference in natural animal populations is a difficult

8 539 Predicting the strength of interference Fig. 6. Summary statistics for interference relationships predicted in black-tailed godwits and oystercatchers. (a) Competitor density at which intake rate is 95% of that in the absence of interference (D 95 ). (b) Strength of interference at 1000 birds ha 1 (m 1000 ). Fig. 7. Relationships between kleptoparasitic attack distance and (a) the competitor density at which intake rate is 95% of that in the absence of interference (D 95 ) and (b) the strength of interference at 1000 birds ha 1 (m 1000 ). and time-consuming process. As interference is a key component of food competition, knowing its strength is key to understanding and predicting the dynamics of animal populations (e.g. Goss-Custard & Sutherland 1997). The aim of this study was to show that a behaviour-based interference model can predict accurately the strength of interference much more rapidly. The methods used to estimate the model s parameters were simple. Importantly, we were able to estimate the speed and distances travelled by birds, parameters to which the strength of interference is most sensitive, without the use of reference markers in the habitat (see Moody et al. (1997) for an approach using markers). Therefore, we could collect data rapidly; if a suitable flock of birds was located, they could be filmed and the strength of interference predicted. If we had relied on reference markers, a suitable flock would need to feed within a marked area. As the location of flocks was variable, far fewer data could have been recorded using this approach. By deriving a relationship between attack distance and handling time, we showed how attack distance can be predicted for oystercatcher and godwit systems in which it cannot or has not been measured directly. We have confidence in the shape of this relationship because a hyperbolic function explained a higher proportion of the variation in attack distance than a linear regression, providing evidence that attack distance was related positively to short handling times, but unrelated to longer handling times. Additionally, the interference model accurately predicted the strength of interference in cockle-feeding oystercatchers, and the absence of interference among low-density bar-tailed godwits, when parameterized using the relationship between attack distance and handling time. The interference model could not have been applied to these systems without this relationship. Despite these accurate predictions we believe that, whenever possible, the best approach is to measure each of the model s parameters directly in each new system to which it is applied.

9 540 R.A. Stillman et al. That handling time is an upper limit on attack duration, and hence distance, seems inevitable. Especially when handling time is short, it seems likely that it will be the major factor limiting attack distance, explaining the positive relationship between these variables for short handling times. It is more likely that other factors limit attack distance when handling time is long, for example, whether a victim detects an attacker approaching. The shorebirds studied could usually escape with their prey if they detected an attack, because the prey were relatively small compared to the size of the birds. The vigilance of potential victims could therefore limit attack distance. However, this will not be true in systems in which prey are larger and so cannot be moved or only moved slowly. Additionally, attacking shorebirds never pursued a victim that escaped with its prey. In situations in which such pursuits do occur (e.g. Thompson 1986) the chances of escaping with prey, even if an aggressor is detected will be smaller. Another factor which may limit attack distance is the profitability of kleptoparasitism compared to searching independently (e.g. Brockmann & Barnard 1979; Thompson 1986; Stillman et al. 1997; Broom & Ruxton 1998). Longer attacks are more time-consuming and are less likely to be successful because victims are more likely to finish handling or detect the attack. Therefore, whether it is profitable to attack will depend on the distance over which the attack is to be made. If an aggressor is a long way from a potential victim, the chances of success may be so low and the time cost so great, that it is more profitable to continue searching independently. As this distance decreases, the chances of success will increase and the time cost decrease, and below a certain distance it may become more profitable to attack than search independently. This distance will depend on the profitability of searching independently, which will be higher when prey are more abundant. Therefore, the threshold attack distance will be shorter when prey are more abundant. Because the strength of interference increases with increased attack distance, this will mean that interference will be stronger when prey are rare. A negative relationship between the strength of interference and prey abundance has been identified before (Brockmann & Barnard 1979) and predicted by behaviour-based models (e.g. Moody & Ruxton 1996). Further studies will be required to test the prediction that attack distance decreases as prey abundance increases. Behaviour-based interference models have made a number of successful qualitative predictions and have aided understanding of the basic process of interference (e.g. Ruxton et al. 1992; Holmgren 1995; Moody & Houston 1995). However, to be of applied value they must also produce accurate quantitative predictions. This can be determined only by testing their predictions for a wide range of predator prey systems, but previously only one model has been tested quantitatively and for just one system (Stillman et al. 1997, 2000). The current study therefore increases the number of tests of this model, and provides further evidence that accurate predictions can be made. It is important to note that these were true tests of predictive ability, as no parameters were chosen in order to make the model fit the data. While encouraging, the number of tests is still small and more are needed. However, in the context of the current study, these successful tests increase confidence in the predictions made for systems in which interference had not been measured previously. We have shown previously that the strength of interference is most sensitive to attack distance and that searching speed is the second most important parameter (Stillman et al. 1997). It was not surprising, therefore, that the same result was found in this study, and that variation in the strength of interference among systems was only related significantly to these parameters. Differences in attack distance explained why the predicted strength of interference was usually weaker in godwits than oystercatchers. Interference was usually weaker in godwits because attack distance was usually shorter in this species. Attack distance was probably shorter in godwits because handling time was also shorter, and handling time limited attack distance. Although influencing interference through its relationship with attack distance, handling time itself was not related to the strength of interference, once the influence of attack distance had been removed. Handling time has little influence on the predicted strength of interference because it has two opposing effects (Stillman et al. 1997). When handling time is long, animals handling prey are exposed to the risk of kleptoparasitism for longer (tending to increase the strength of interference), but potential aggressors are also more likely to be handling prey themselves, rather than searching for victims to attack (tending to decrease the strength of interference). Although attack distance and searching speed determine the predicted strength of interference, the other parameters are not redundant. For example, prey encounter rate and handling time determine intake rate in the absence of interference. The datasets with which we have tested the interference model were derived from intensive field studies. In contrast, the data needed to parameterize the model can be collected relatively quickly or can be obtained from the literature. If further tests also support the predictive power of behaviour-based interference models, they will provide a valuable means of predicting the strength of interference. Acknowledgements We are very grateful to two anonymous referees whose comments greatly improved the manuscript. R.A.S. was funded by the Natural Environment Research Council.

10 541 Predicting the strength of interference References Beddington, J.R. (1975) Mutual interference between parasites or predators and its effect on searching efficiency. Ecology, 44, Brockmann, H.J. & Barnard, C.J. (1979) Kleptoparasitism in birds. Animal Behaviour, 27, Broom, M. & Ruxton, G.D. (1998) Evolutionarily stable stealing: game theory applied to kleptoparasitism. Behavioral Ecology, 9, Cresswell, W. (1997) Interference competition at low competitor densities in blackbirds Turdus merula. Journal of Animal Ecology, 66, Cresswell, W. (1998) Variation in the strength of interference with resource density in blackbirds, Turdus merula. Oikos, 81, Dolman, P.M. (1995) The intensity of interference varies with resource density: evidence from a field study with snow buntings, Plectrophenox nivalis. Oecologia, 102, Ens, B.J. & Cayford, J.T. (1996) Feeding with other Oystercatchers. The Oystercatcher: from Individuals to Populations (ed. J.D. Goss-Custard), pp Oxford University Press, Oxford. Ens, B.J. & Goss-Custard, J.D. (1984) Interference among oystercatchers, Haematopus ostralegus, feeding on mussels, Mytilus edulis, on the Exe estuary. Ecology, 53, Goss-Custard, J.D. (1970) Feeding dispersion in some overwintering wading birds. Social Behaviour in Birds and Mammals (ed. J.H. Crook), pp Academic Press, London. Goss-Custard, J.D. & Durell, S.E.A. le V. dit (1988) The effect of dominance and feeding method on the intake rates of oystercatchers, Haematopus ostralegus, feeding on mussels. Ecology, 57, Goss-Custard, J.D. & Sutherland, W.J. (1997) Individual behaviour, populations and conservation. Behavioural Ecology: an Evolutionary Approach, 4th edn (eds J.R. Krebs & N.B. Davies), pp Blackwell Science, Oxford. Hayman, P., Marchant, J. & Prater, A.J. (1986) Shorebirds: an Identification Guide to the Waders of the World. Christopher Helm, London. Holling, C.S. (1959) The components of predation as revealed by a study of small-mammal predation of the European pine sawfly. Canadian Entomologist, 91, Holmgren, N. (1995) The ideal free distribution of unequal competitors: predictions from a behaviour-based functional response. Ecology, 64, Moody, A.L. & Houston, A.I. (1995) Interference and the ideal free distribution. Animal Behaviour, 49, Moody, A.L. & Ruxton, G.D. (1996) The intensity of interference varies with food density: support for behaviour-based models of interference. Oecologia, 108, Moody, A.L., Thompson, W.A., de Bruijn, B., Houston, A.I. & Goss-Custard, J.D. (1997) The analysis of the spacing of animals, with an example based on oystercatchers during the tidal cycle. Ecology, 66, Ruxton, G.D., Gurney, W.S.C. & de Roos, A.M. (1992) Interference and generation cycles. Theoretical Population Biology, 42, Sitters, H.P. (2000) The role of night-feeding in shorebirds in an estuarine environment with specific reference to musselfeeding oystercatchers. PhD Thesis, University of Oxford. Speakman, J.R. & Bryant, D.M. (1993) The searching speeds of foraging shorebirds: redshank (Tringa totanus) and oystercatcher (Haematopus ostralegus). American Naturalist, 142, Stillman, R.A., Caldow, R.W.G., Goss-Custard, J.D. & Alexander, M.J. (2000) Individual variation in intake rate: the relative importance of foraging efficiency and susceptibility to interference. Ecology, 69, Stillman, R.A., Goss-Custard, J.D. & Caldow, R.W.D. (1997) Modelling interference from basic foraging behaviour. Ecology, 66, Stillman, R.A., Goss-Custard, J.D., Clarke, R.T. & Durell, S.E.A. V. le dit (1996) Shape of the interference function in a foraging vertebrate. Ecology, 65, Thompson, D.B.A. (1986) The economics of kleptoparasitism, optimal foraging, host and prey selection by gulls. Animal Behaviour, 34, Triplet, P., Stillman, R.A. & Goss-Custard, J.D. (1999) Prey abundance and the strength of interference in a foraging shorebird. Ecology, 68, Yates, M.G., Stillman, R.A. & Goss-Custard, J.D. (2000) Contrasting interference functions and foraging dispersion in two species of shorebird (Charadrii). Ecology, 69, Zwarts, L. & Drent, R.H. (1981) Prey depletion and the regulation of predator density: oystercatchers (Haematopus ostralegus) feeding on mussels (Mytilus edulis). Feeding and Survival Strategies of Estuarine Organisms (eds N.V. Jones & W.J. Wolff ), pp Plenum Press, New York. Received 6 July 2001; revision received 5 February 2002

Within predator-prey systems, interference has been defined

Within predator-prey systems, interference has been defined Behavioral Ecology Vol. 11 No. 6: 597 605 Predator search pattern and the strength of interference through prey depression Richard A. Stillman, John D. Goss-Custard, and Matthew J. Alexander Centre for

More information

Foraging in a group is a common pattern of animal behavior,

Foraging in a group is a common pattern of animal behavior, Behavioral Ecology Vol. 11 No. 4: 351 356 An evolutionarily stable strategy for aggressiveness in feeding groups Etienne Sirot Université de Bretagne Sud, 12 avenue Saint-Symphorien, 56 000 Vannes, France

More information

A Hawk-Dove game in kleptoparasitic populations

A Hawk-Dove game in kleptoparasitic populations A Hawk-Dove game in kleptoparasitic populations Mark Broom, Department of Mathematics, University of Sussex, Brighton BN1 9RF, UK. M.Broom@sussex.ac.uk Roger M. Luther, Department of Mathematics, University

More information

Citation for published version (APA): Vahl, W. K. (2006). Interference competition among foraging waders s.n.

Citation for published version (APA): Vahl, W. K. (2006). Interference competition among foraging waders s.n. University of Groningen Interference competition among foraging waders Vahl, Wouter Karsten IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from

More information

An experimental design and a statistical analysis separating interference from exploitation competition

An experimental design and a statistical analysis separating interference from exploitation competition Popul Ecol (8) 5:319 34 DOI 1.17/s1144-8-81-9 FORUM An experimental design and a statistical analysis separating interference from exploitation competition Inon Scharf Æ Ido Filin Æ Ofer Ovadia Received:

More information

Foraging. This week in Animal Behaviour. How do animals choose? Do animals choose their food?

Foraging. This week in Animal Behaviour. How do animals choose? Do animals choose their food? This week in Animal Behaviour Lecture 22: Cooperation Lecture 23: Foraging Lecture 24: TBA Text chapters 10-11 LAB: Project 2 proposal seminars Midterm 2 on Tuesday Nov. 8 Foraging 1. Models of foraging

More information

Kleptoparasitic Melees Modelling Food Stealing Featuring Contests with Multiple Individuals

Kleptoparasitic Melees Modelling Food Stealing Featuring Contests with Multiple Individuals Bull Math Biol 2011 73: 683 699 DOI 10.1007/s11538-010-9546-z ORIGINAL ARTICLE Kleptoparasitic Melees Modelling Food Stealing Featuring Contests with Multiple Individuals M. Broom J. Rychtář Received:

More information

Interspecific Kleptoparasitism

Interspecific Kleptoparasitism Interspecific Kleptoparasitism submitted by Elizabeth Gallagher for the degree of MSc Mathematical Biology of the University of Bath Department of Mathematical Sciences July-September 2010 COPYRIGHT Attention

More information

OPTIMAL FORAGING MODELS

OPTIMAL FORAGING MODELS 26 OPTIMAL FORAGING MODELS In collaboration with David N. Bonter Objectives Develop a spreadsheet model of foraging choices among two prey types, prey 1 and prey 2. Determine the conditions in which individuals

More information

Niche The sum of all interactions a species has with biotic/abiotic components of the environment N-dimensional hypervolume

Niche The sum of all interactions a species has with biotic/abiotic components of the environment N-dimensional hypervolume Niche The sum of all interactions a species has with biotic/abiotic components of the environment N-dimensional hypervolume Each dimension is a biotic or abiotic resource Ecomorphology Ecology (niche)

More information

Predation. Predation & Herbivory. Lotka-Volterra. Predation rate. Total rate of predation. Predator population 10/23/2013. Review types of predation

Predation. Predation & Herbivory. Lotka-Volterra. Predation rate. Total rate of predation. Predator population 10/23/2013. Review types of predation Predation & Herbivory Chapter 14 Predation Review types of predation Carnivory Parasitism Parasitoidism Cannabalism Lotka-Volterra Predators control prey populations and prey control predator populations

More information

BIOS 6150: Ecology Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences

BIOS 6150: Ecology Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences BIOS 6150: Ecology Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences Week 6: Predation and predatory behavior: Lecture summary: Nature of predation. Diet breadth & choice. Optimal foraging. Functional

More information

The ideal free distribution: an analysis of the perceptual limit model

The ideal free distribution: an analysis of the perceptual limit model Evolutionary Ecology Research, 2002, 4: 471 493 The ideal free distribution: an analysis of the perceptual limit model Edmund J. Collins,* Alasdair I. Houston and Alison Lang Centre for Behavioural Biology,

More information

3 Ecological and Evolutionary Principles. Notes for Marine Biology: Function, Biodiversity, Ecology by Jeffrey S. Levinton

3 Ecological and Evolutionary Principles. Notes for Marine Biology: Function, Biodiversity, Ecology by Jeffrey S. Levinton 3 Ecological and Evolutionary Principles Notes for Marine Biology: Function, Biodiversity, Ecology by Jeffrey S. Levinton Ecological hierarchy Population-level processes Individual interactions The Ecological

More information

BIOS 3010: Ecology Lecture 8: Predator foraging & prey defense. 2. Predation: 3. Predator diet breadth and preference:

BIOS 3010: Ecology Lecture 8: Predator foraging & prey defense. 2. Predation: 3. Predator diet breadth and preference: BIOS 3010: Ecology Lecture 8: Predator foraging & prey defense 1. Lecture Summary: What is predation? Predator diet breadth. Preference & switching. Optimal foraging. Marginal value theorem. Functional

More information

City, University of London Institutional Repository

City, University of London Institutional Repository City Research Online City, University of London Institutional Repository Citation: Broom, M. and Rychtar, J. (016). A model of food stealing with asymmetric information. Ecological Complexity, 6, pp. 137-14.

More information

BIOL 410 Population and Community Ecology. Predation

BIOL 410 Population and Community Ecology. Predation BIOL 410 Population and Community Ecology Predation Intraguild Predation Occurs when one species not only competes with its heterospecific guild member, but also occasionally preys upon it Species 1 Competitor

More information

Building on the Ideal Free Distribution. Tom Tregenza

Building on the Ideal Free Distribution. Tom Tregenza Building on the Ideal Free Distribution Tom Tregenza Tregenza, T. 1995 Building on the ideal free distribution. Advances in Ecological Research 26. 253-302. Building on the ideal free distribution TOM

More information

Chapter 6 Reading Questions

Chapter 6 Reading Questions Chapter 6 Reading Questions 1. Fill in 5 key events in the re-establishment of the New England forest in the Opening Story: 1. Farmers begin leaving 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. Broadleaf forest reestablished 2.

More information

EVOLUTIONARILY STABLE STRATEGIES AND GROUP VERSUS INDIVIDUAL SELECTION

EVOLUTIONARILY STABLE STRATEGIES AND GROUP VERSUS INDIVIDUAL SELECTION 39 EVOLUTIONARILY STABLE STRATEGIES AND GROUP VERSUS INDIVIDUAL SELECTION Objectives Understand the concept of game theory. Set up a spreadsheet model of simple game theory interactions. Explore the effects

More information

NCEA Level 3 Biology (90716) 2005 page 1 of 5. Q Achievement Achievement with Merit Achievement with Excellence

NCEA Level 3 Biology (90716) 2005 page 1 of 5. Q Achievement Achievement with Merit Achievement with Excellence NCEA Level 3 Biology (90716) 2005 page 1 of 5 Assessment Schedule 2005 Biology: Describe animal behaviour and plant responses (90716) Evidence Statement 1(a) Describes TWO methods of navigation. (orientation

More information

CHEMICALS IN HOST PARASITOID AND PREY PREDATOR RELATIONS

CHEMICALS IN HOST PARASITOID AND PREY PREDATOR RELATIONS CHEMICALS IN HOST PARASITOID AND PREY PREDATOR RELATIONS Lozano C. Estacion Experimental del Zaidin, Consejo Superior de Investigaciones Científicas, Granada, Spain Keywords: semiochemicals, pesticides,

More information

Natal versus breeding dispersal: Evolution in a model system

Natal versus breeding dispersal: Evolution in a model system Evolutionary Ecology Research, 1999, 1: 911 921 Natal versus breeding dispersal: Evolution in a model system Karin Johst 1 * and Roland Brandl 2 1 Centre for Environmental Research Leipzig-Halle Ltd, Department

More information

Population Ecology. Text Readings. Questions to Answer in the Chapter. Chapter Reading:

Population Ecology. Text Readings. Questions to Answer in the Chapter. Chapter Reading: Population Ecology Text Readings Chapter Reading: Chapter # 26 in Audesirk, Audesirk and Byers: Population Growth and Regulation Pg. # 513-534. Questions to Answer in the Chapter How Does Population Size

More information

A population is a group of individuals of the same species, living in a shared space at a specific point in time.

A population is a group of individuals of the same species, living in a shared space at a specific point in time. A population is a group of individuals of the same species, living in a shared space at a specific point in time. A population size refers to the number of individuals in a population. Increase Decrease

More information

Predators feeding on behaviourally responsive prey: some implications for classical models of optimal diet choice

Predators feeding on behaviourally responsive prey: some implications for classical models of optimal diet choice Evolutionary Ecology Research, 2003, 5: 1083 1102 Predators feeding on behaviourally responsive prey: some implications for classical models of optimal diet choice Steven L. Lima,* William A. Mitchell

More information

University of Groningen. Sex-specific foraging Duijns, Sjoerd

University of Groningen. Sex-specific foraging Duijns, Sjoerd University of Groningen Sex-specific foraging Duijns, Sjoerd IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document

More information

Population Ecology. Study of populations in relation to the environment. Increase population size= endangered species

Population Ecology. Study of populations in relation to the environment. Increase population size= endangered species Population Basics Population Ecology Study of populations in relation to the environment Purpose: Increase population size= endangered species Decrease population size = pests, invasive species Maintain

More information

Comparing male densities and fertilization rates as potential Allee effects in Alaskan and Canadian Ursus maritimus populations

Comparing male densities and fertilization rates as potential Allee effects in Alaskan and Canadian Ursus maritimus populations Comparing male densities and fertilization rates as potential Allee effects in Alaskan and Canadian Ursus maritimus populations Introduction Research suggests that our world today is in the midst of a

More information

The Problem of Where to Live

The Problem of Where to Live April 5: Habitat Selection: Intro The Problem of Where to Live Physical and biotic environment critically affects fitness An animal's needs may be met only in certain habitats, which should select for

More information

HABITAT SELECTION INTRODUCTION. Objectives

HABITAT SELECTION INTRODUCTION. Objectives 25 HABITAT SELECTION In collaboration with David N. Bonter Objectives Develop a spreadsheet model of ideal-free habitat selection. Compare the ideal-free and ideal-despotic habitat selection models. INTRODUCTION

More information

A population is a group of individuals of the same species occupying a particular area at the same time

A population is a group of individuals of the same species occupying a particular area at the same time A population is a group of individuals of the same species occupying a particular area at the same time Population Growth As long as the birth rate exceeds the death rate a population will grow Immigration

More information

The Living World Continued: Populations and Communities

The Living World Continued: Populations and Communities The Living World Continued: Populations and Communities Ecosystem Communities Populations Review: Parts of an Ecosystem 1) An individual in a species: One organism of a species. a species must be genetically

More information

CHAPTER. Evolution and Community Ecology

CHAPTER. Evolution and Community Ecology CHAPTER 5 Evolution and Community Ecology Lesson 5.2 Species Interactions The zebra mussel has completely displaced 20 native mussel species in Lake St. Clair. Lesson 5.2 Species Interactions The Niche

More information

Assessment Schedule 2016 Biology: Demonstrate understanding of the responses of plants and animals to their external environment (91603)

Assessment Schedule 2016 Biology: Demonstrate understanding of the responses of plants and animals to their external environment (91603) NCEA Level 3 Biology (91603) 2016 page 1 of 6 Assessment Schedule 2016 Biology: Demonstrate understanding of the responses of plants and animals to their external environment (91603) Evidence Statement

More information

CORRELATION ANALYSIS BETWEEN PALAEMONETES SHRIMP AND VARIOUS ALGAL SPECIES IN ROCKY TIDE POOLS IN NEW ENGLAND

CORRELATION ANALYSIS BETWEEN PALAEMONETES SHRIMP AND VARIOUS ALGAL SPECIES IN ROCKY TIDE POOLS IN NEW ENGLAND CORRELATION ANALYSIS BETWEEN PALAEMONETES SHRIMP AND VARIOUS ALGAL SPECIES IN ROCKY TIDE POOLS IN NEW ENGLAND Douglas F., Department of Biology,, Worcester, MA 01610 USA (D@clarku.edu) Abstract Palamonetes

More information

The evolution of a kleptoparasitic system under adaptive dynamics

The evolution of a kleptoparasitic system under adaptive dynamics J. Math. Biol. 54, 151 177 (007) Mathematical Biology Digil Object Identifier (DOI): 10.1007/s0085-006-0005- M. Broom J. Rychtář The evolution of a kleptoparasitic system under adaptive dynamics Received:

More information

City, University of London Institutional Repository

City, University of London Institutional Repository City Research Online City, University of London Institutional Repository Citation: Broom, M., Crowe, M. L., Fitzgerald, M. R. and Rychtar, J. (2010). The stochastic modelling of kleptoparasitism using

More information

Effects to Communities & Ecosystems

Effects to Communities & Ecosystems Biology 5868 Ecotoxicology Effects to Communities & Ecosystems April 18, 2007 Definitions Ecological Community an assemblage of populations living in a prescribed area or physical habitat [It is] the living

More information

Age (x) nx lx. Population dynamics Population size through time should be predictable N t+1 = N t + B + I - D - E

Age (x) nx lx. Population dynamics Population size through time should be predictable N t+1 = N t + B + I - D - E Population dynamics Population size through time should be predictable N t+1 = N t + B + I - D - E Time 1 N = 100 20 births 25 deaths 10 immigrants 15 emmigrants Time 2 100 + 20 +10 25 15 = 90 Life History

More information

Chapter Niches and Community Interactions

Chapter Niches and Community Interactions Chapter 4 4.2 Niches and Community Interactions Key Questions: 1) What is a niche? 2) How does competition shape communities? 3) How do predation and herbivory shape communites? 4) What are three primary

More information

Modeling Fish Assemblages in Stream Networks Representation of Stream Network Introduction habitat attributes Criteria for Success

Modeling Fish Assemblages in Stream Networks Representation of Stream Network Introduction habitat attributes Criteria for Success Modeling Fish Assemblages in Stream Networks Joan P. Baker and Denis White Western Ecology Division National Health & Environmental Effects Research Laboratory U.S. Environmental Protection Agency baker.joan@epa.gov

More information

Tolerance. Tolerance. Tolerance 10/22/2010

Tolerance. Tolerance. Tolerance 10/22/2010 Section 4.2 Mrs. Michaelsen Tolerance Every species has its own range of tolerance: The ability to survive and reproduce under a range of environmental circumstances. Tolerance Stress can result when an

More information

Understanding Populations Section 1. Chapter 8 Understanding Populations Section1, How Populations Change in Size DAY ONE

Understanding Populations Section 1. Chapter 8 Understanding Populations Section1, How Populations Change in Size DAY ONE Chapter 8 Understanding Populations Section1, How Populations Change in Size DAY ONE What Is a Population? A population is a group of organisms of the same species that live in a specific geographical

More information

BIOS 6150: Ecology Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences

BIOS 6150: Ecology Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences BIOS 6150: Ecology Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences Week 7: Dynamics of Predation. Lecture summary: Categories of predation. Linked prey-predator cycles. Lotka-Volterra model. Density-dependence.

More information

14.1 Habitat And Niche

14.1 Habitat And Niche 14.1 Habitat And Niche A habitat differs from a niche. Habitat physical area in which an organism lives Niche each species plays a specific role in an ecosystem niche includes the species habitat, feeding

More information

Additional Case Study: Calculating the Size of a Small Mammal Population

Additional Case Study: Calculating the Size of a Small Mammal Population Student Worksheet LSM 14.1-2 Additional Case Study: Calculating the Size of a Small Mammal Population Objective To use field study data on shrew populations to examine the characteristics of a natural

More information

Lesson Eight The Meeting of the Dinosaurs Evidence Given by Dinosaur Footprints

Lesson Eight The Meeting of the Dinosaurs Evidence Given by Dinosaur Footprints Lesson Eight The Meeting of the Dinosaurs Evidence Given by Dinosaur Footprints Summary During the first set of activities, students focused on the basics, learning about rocks, minerals, time, and fossils.

More information

2 Ludek Berec Running head: Optimal foraging Send correspondence to: Ludek Berec, Department of Theoretical Biology, Institute of Entomology, Academy

2 Ludek Berec Running head: Optimal foraging Send correspondence to: Ludek Berec, Department of Theoretical Biology, Institute of Entomology, Academy Bulletin of Mathematical Biology (999) 0, {28 Mixed Encounters, Limited Perception and Optimal Foraging LUDEK BEREC Department of Theoretical Biology, Institute of Entomology, Academy of Sciences of the

More information

Ch.5 Evolution and Community Ecology How do organisms become so well suited to their environment? Evolution and Natural Selection

Ch.5 Evolution and Community Ecology How do organisms become so well suited to their environment? Evolution and Natural Selection Ch.5 Evolution and Community Ecology How do organisms become so well suited to their environment? Evolution and Natural Selection Gene: A sequence of DNA that codes for a particular trait Gene pool: All

More information

Chapter 6 Population and Community Ecology. Thursday, October 19, 17

Chapter 6 Population and Community Ecology. Thursday, October 19, 17 Chapter 6 Population and Community Ecology Module 18 The Abundance and Distribution of After reading this module you should be able to explain how nature exists at several levels of complexity. discuss

More information

Community Structure. Community An assemblage of all the populations interacting in an area

Community Structure. Community An assemblage of all the populations interacting in an area Community Structure Community An assemblage of all the populations interacting in an area Community Ecology The ecological community is the set of plant and animal species that occupy an area Questions

More information

Population Ecology. Chapter 44

Population Ecology. Chapter 44 Population Ecology Chapter 44 Stages of Biology O Ecology is the interactions of organisms with other organisms and with their environments O These interactions occur in different hierarchies O The simplest

More information

Multiple choice 2 pts each): x 2 = 18) Essay (pre-prepared) / 15 points. 19) Short Answer: / 2 points. 20) Short Answer / 5 points

Multiple choice 2 pts each): x 2 = 18) Essay (pre-prepared) / 15 points. 19) Short Answer: / 2 points. 20) Short Answer / 5 points P 1 Biology 217: Ecology Second Exam Fall 2004 There should be 7 ps in this exam - take a moment and count them now. Put your name on the first p of the exam, and on each of the ps with short answer questions.

More information

Section 2: How Species Interact with Each Other

Section 2: How Species Interact with Each Other Section 2: How Species Interact with Each Other Preview Bellringer Objectives An Organism s Niche Ways in Which Species Interact Competition Indirect Competition Adaptations to Competition Section 2: How

More information

Lesson Overview. Niches and Community Interactions. Lesson Overview. 4.2 Niches and Community Interactions

Lesson Overview. Niches and Community Interactions. Lesson Overview. 4.2 Niches and Community Interactions Lesson Overview 4.2 Niches and Community Interactions The Niche What is a niche? A niche is the range of physical and biological conditions in which a species lives and the way the species obtains what

More information

PREDATOR AND PREY HABITAT SELECTION GAMES: THE EFFECTS OF HOW PREY BALANCE FORAGING AND PREDATION RISK

PREDATOR AND PREY HABITAT SELECTION GAMES: THE EFFECTS OF HOW PREY BALANCE FORAGING AND PREDATION RISK ISRAEL JOURNAL OF ZOOLOGY, Vol. 50, 2004, pp. 233 254 PREDATOR AND PREY HABITAT SELECTION GAMES: THE EFFECTS OF HOW PREY BALANCE FORAGING AND PREDATION RISK BARNEY LUTTBEG* AND ANDREW SIH Department of

More information

Optimal diet choice for large herbivores: an extended contingency model

Optimal diet choice for large herbivores: an extended contingency model Functional Ecology 1998 ORIGIAL ARTICLE OA 000 E Optimal diet choice for large herbivores: an extended contingency model K. D. FARSWORTH* and A. W. ILLIUS *Macaulay Land Use Research Institute, Aberdeen

More information

What is insect forecasting, and why do it

What is insect forecasting, and why do it Insect Forecasting Programs: Objectives, and How to Properly Interpret the Data John Gavloski, Extension Entomologist, Manitoba Agriculture, Food and Rural Initiatives Carman, MB R0G 0J0 Email: jgavloski@gov.mb.ca

More information

Aggregations on larger scales. Metapopulation. Definition: A group of interconnected subpopulations Sources and Sinks

Aggregations on larger scales. Metapopulation. Definition: A group of interconnected subpopulations Sources and Sinks Aggregations on larger scales. Metapopulation Definition: A group of interconnected subpopulations Sources and Sinks Metapopulation - interconnected group of subpopulations sink source McKillup and McKillup

More information

Effects of rain and foot disturbances on pit size and location preference of antlions (Myrmeleon immaculatus)

Effects of rain and foot disturbances on pit size and location preference of antlions (Myrmeleon immaculatus) Effects of rain and foot disturbances on pit size and location preference of antlions (Myrmeleon immaculatus) Angela Feng University of Michigan Biological Station EEB 381 Ecology August 17, 2010 Professor

More information

GENERAL ECOLOGY STUDY NOTES

GENERAL ECOLOGY STUDY NOTES 1.0 INTRODUCTION GENERAL ECOLOGY STUDY NOTES A community is made up of populations of different organisms living together in a unit environment. The manner in which these organisms relate together for

More information

Chapter 6 Population and Community Ecology

Chapter 6 Population and Community Ecology Chapter 6 Population and Community Ecology Friedland and Relyea Environmental Science for AP, second edition 2015 W.H. Freeman and Company/BFW AP is a trademark registered and/or owned by the College Board,

More information

7. E C. 5 B. 1 D E V E L O P A N D U S E M O D E L S T O E X P L A I N H O W O R G A N I S M S I N T E R A C T I N A C O M P E T I T I V E O R M U T

7. E C. 5 B. 1 D E V E L O P A N D U S E M O D E L S T O E X P L A I N H O W O R G A N I S M S I N T E R A C T I N A C O M P E T I T I V E O R M U T 7. E C. 5 B. 1 D E V E L O P A N D U S E M O D E L S T O E X P L A I N H O W O R G A N I S M S I N T E R A C T I N A C O M P E T I T I V E O R M U T U A L L Y B E N E F I C I A L R E L A T I O N S H I

More information

Coevolution of predators and prey

Coevolution of predators and prey Coevolution of predators and prey 1) Evolution of predator-prey interactions 2) Arms race analogy 3) Examples of predator-prey coevolution? 4) Steady-state theory 5) Evolution of aposematic coloration

More information

What Shapes an Ecosystem Section 4-2

What Shapes an Ecosystem Section 4-2 What Shapes an Ecosystem Section 4-2 Biotic and Abiotic Factors Ecosystems are influenced by a combination of biological and physical factors. Biotic factors are the biological influences on an organism.

More information

Field measurements give biased estimates of functional response parameters, but help explain foraging distributions

Field measurements give biased estimates of functional response parameters, but help explain foraging distributions Journal of Animal Ecology 2015, 84, 565 575 doi: 10.1111/1365-2656.12309 Field measurements give biased estimates of functional response parameters, but help explain foraging distributions Sjoerd Duijns

More information

History and meaning of the word Ecology A. Definition 1. Oikos, ology - the study of the house - the place we live

History and meaning of the word Ecology A. Definition 1. Oikos, ology - the study of the house - the place we live History and meaning of the word Ecology. Definition 1. Oikos, ology - the study of the house - the place we live. Etymology - origin and development of the the word 1. Earliest - Haeckel (1869) - comprehensive

More information

Salamanders. Jefferson Complex

Salamanders. Jefferson Complex Jefferson Complex Salamanders By Alexandra Shipman Above: A series of images of a Jefferson Complex salamander walking blended together using Photoshop. This salamander is about 10 centimeters long. In

More information

Dynamical Systems and Chaos Part II: Biology Applications. Lecture 6: Population dynamics. Ilya Potapov Mathematics Department, TUT Room TD325

Dynamical Systems and Chaos Part II: Biology Applications. Lecture 6: Population dynamics. Ilya Potapov Mathematics Department, TUT Room TD325 Dynamical Systems and Chaos Part II: Biology Applications Lecture 6: Population dynamics Ilya Potapov Mathematics Department, TUT Room TD325 Living things are dynamical systems Dynamical systems theory

More information

Laws of Adaptation. A course on biological evolution in eight lectures by Carlo Matessi. Lecture 7. Ways to diversity, or polymorphic LTE

Laws of Adaptation. A course on biological evolution in eight lectures by Carlo Matessi. Lecture 7. Ways to diversity, or polymorphic LTE 1 Laws of Adaptation A course on biological evolution in eight lectures by Carlo Matessi Lecture 7 Ways to diversity, or polymorphic LTE Part I discrete traits Wednesday October 18, 15:00-16:00 2 Adaptive

More information

Yakın Doğu Üniversitesi Mimarlık Fakültesi Peyzaj Mimarlığı Bölümü. PM 317 Human and Environment Assoc. Prof. Dr. Salih GÜCEL

Yakın Doğu Üniversitesi Mimarlık Fakültesi Peyzaj Mimarlığı Bölümü. PM 317 Human and Environment Assoc. Prof. Dr. Salih GÜCEL Yakın Doğu Üniversitesi Mimarlık Fakültesi Peyzaj Mimarlığı Bölümü PM 317 Human and Environment Assoc. Prof. Dr. Salih GÜCEL Ecology & Ecosystems Principles of Ecology Ecology is the study of the interactions

More information

Discrete evaluation and the particle swarm algorithm

Discrete evaluation and the particle swarm algorithm Volume 12 Discrete evaluation and the particle swarm algorithm Tim Hendtlass and Tom Rodgers Centre for Intelligent Systems and Complex Processes Swinburne University of Technology P. O. Box 218 Hawthorn

More information

The Structure of Ecological Networks and Consequences for Fragility

The Structure of Ecological Networks and Consequences for Fragility The Structure of Ecological Networks and Consequences for Fragility closely connected clustered Emily I. Jones ECOL 596H Feb. 13, 2008 Why ecological network structure matters 2. 3. the network contains

More information

Rocky Intertidal Ecology -- part II The development of experimental ecology. Connell and the experimental revolution

Rocky Intertidal Ecology -- part II The development of experimental ecology. Connell and the experimental revolution Rocky Intertidal Ecology -- part II The development of experimental ecology I. Intertidal Zonation, part II 1. Follow ups on Connell 2. Predation 3. Exceptions II. Horizontal Distribution 1. Variation

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION Dynamics of predator-prey cycles and the effects of dispersal and the Moran effect Here we describe in more detail the dynamics of predator-prey limit cycles in our model, and the manner in which dispersal

More information

Maintenance of Species Diversity by Predation in the Tierra System

Maintenance of Species Diversity by Predation in the Tierra System Maintenance of Species Diversity by Predation in the Tierra System Jie Shao and Thomas S. Ray Department of Zoology, University of Oklahoma, Norman, Oklahoma 7319, USA jshao@ou.edu Abstract One of the

More information

Computational Ecology Introduction to Ecological Science. Sonny Bleicher Ph.D.

Computational Ecology Introduction to Ecological Science. Sonny Bleicher Ph.D. Computational Ecology Introduction to Ecological Science Sonny Bleicher Ph.D. Ecos Logos Defining Ecology Interactions: Organisms: Plants Animals: Bacteria Fungi Invertebrates Vertebrates The physical

More information

Groups of organisms living close enough together for interactions to occur.

Groups of organisms living close enough together for interactions to occur. Community ecology: First, let's define a community: Groups of organisms living close enough together for interactions to occur. First we probably want to describe the community a bit, so we look at: Biodiversity

More information

Treasure Coast Science Scope and Sequence

Treasure Coast Science Scope and Sequence Course: Marine Science I Honors Course Code: 2002510 Quarter: 3 Topic(s) of Study: Marine Organisms and Ecosystems Bodies of Knowledge: Nature of Science and Life Science Standard(s): 1: The Practice of

More information

Field experiments on competition. Field experiments on competition. Field experiments on competition

Field experiments on competition. Field experiments on competition. Field experiments on competition INTERACTIONS BETWEEN SPECIES Type of interaction species 1 species 2 competition consumer-resource (pred, herb, para) mutualism detritivore-detritus (food is dead) Field experiments on competition Example

More information

The Game of Normal Numbers

The Game of Normal Numbers The Game of Normal Numbers Ehud Lehrer September 4, 2003 Abstract We introduce a two-player game where at each period one player, say, Player 2, chooses a distribution and the other player, Player 1, a

More information

Question Answer Marks Guidance 1 (a) 1. 1 CREDIT herbivore / primary consumer,energy. trophic level 2 energy x 100 ; x 100 ; producer energy

Question Answer Marks Guidance 1 (a) 1. 1 CREDIT herbivore / primary consumer,energy. trophic level 2 energy x 100 ; x 100 ; producer energy 1 (a) 1 1 CREDIT herbivore / primary consumer,energy trophic level 2 energy x 100 ; x 100 ; producer energy trophic level 1 energy Plus any 3 of the following: CREDIT sample figures. e.g. if producer energy

More information

Populations Study Guide (KEY) All the members of a species living in the same place at the same time.

Populations Study Guide (KEY) All the members of a species living in the same place at the same time. Populations Study Guide (KEY) 1. Define Population. All the members of a species living in the same place at the same time. 2. List and explain the three terms that describe population. a. Size. How large

More information

Honors Biology Ecology Concept List

Honors Biology Ecology Concept List 1. For each pair of terms, explain how the meanings of the terms differ. a. mutualism and commensalism b. parasitism and predation c. species richness and species diversity d. primary succession and secondary

More information

TURNING FLIGHT OF BATS BY H. D. J. N. ALDRIDGE*

TURNING FLIGHT OF BATS BY H. D. J. N. ALDRIDGE* J. exp. Biol. 128, 419-425 (1987) 419 Printed in Great Britain The Company of Biologists Limited 1987 TURNING FLIGHT OF BATS BY H. D. J. N. ALDRIDGE* Department of Zoology, University of Bristol, Woodland

More information

Optimization and Newton s method

Optimization and Newton s method Chapter 5 Optimization and Newton s method 5.1 Optimal Flying Speed According to R McNeil Alexander (1996, Optima for Animals, Princeton U Press), the power, P, required to propel a flying plane at constant

More information

Name: Characteristics of Life and Ecology Guided Notes (PAP)

Name: Characteristics of Life and Ecology Guided Notes (PAP) Name: Characteristics of Life and Ecology Guided Notes (PAP) I. What is Biology? a. Biology is the study of II. The Eight Characteristics of Life a. Organization & the presence of or more cells b. Response

More information

Modelling evolution in structured populations involving multiplayer interactions

Modelling evolution in structured populations involving multiplayer interactions Modelling evolution in structured populations involving multiplayer interactions Mark Broom City University London Game Theoretical Modelling of Evolution in Structured Populations NIMBioS Knoxville 25-27

More information

THETA-LOGISTIC PREDATOR PREY

THETA-LOGISTIC PREDATOR PREY THETA-LOGISTIC PREDATOR PREY What are the assumptions of this model? 1.) Functional responses are non-linear. Functional response refers to a change in the rate of exploitation of prey by an individual

More information

Visit for Videos, Questions and Revision Notes.

Visit   for Videos, Questions and Revision Notes. Q1. The young of frogs and toads are called tadpoles. Ecologists investigated the effect of predation on three species of tadpole. They set up four artificial pond communities. Each community contained

More information

What standard are we focusing on today?

What standard are we focusing on today? What standard are we focusing on today? Standard H.B.6 The student will demonstrate an understanding that ecosystems are complex, interactive systems that include both biological communities and physical

More information

Structure Learning of a Behavior Network for Context Dependent Adaptability

Structure Learning of a Behavior Network for Context Dependent Adaptability Georgia State University ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University Computer Science Theses Department of Computer Science 12-7-26 Structure Learning of a Behavior Network for Context Dependent Adaptability

More information

Lesson Overview 4.2 Niches and Community Interactions

Lesson Overview 4.2 Niches and Community Interactions THINK ABOUT IT If you ask someone where an organism lives, that person might answer on a coral reef or in the desert. Lesson Overview 4.2 Niches and Community Interactions These answers give the environment

More information

BIOS 5970: Plant-Herbivore Interactions Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences

BIOS 5970: Plant-Herbivore Interactions Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences BIOS 5970: Plant-Herbivore Interactions Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences D. POPULATION & COMMUNITY DYNAMICS Week 13. Herbivory, predation & parasitism: Lecture summary: Predation:

More information

4. is the rate at which a population of a given species will increase when no limits are placed on its rate of growth.

4. is the rate at which a population of a given species will increase when no limits are placed on its rate of growth. Population Ecology 1. Populations of mammals that live in colder climates tend to have shorter ears and limbs than populations of the same species in warm climates (coyotes are a good example of this).

More information

Effect of Species 2 on Species 1 Competition - - Predator-Prey + - Parasite-Host + -

Effect of Species 2 on Species 1 Competition - - Predator-Prey + - Parasite-Host + - Community Ecology Community - a group of organisms, of different species, living in the same area Community ecology is the study of the interactions between species The presence of one species may affect

More information

Community Ecology. Classification of types of interspecific interactions: Effect of Species 1 on Species 2

Community Ecology. Classification of types of interspecific interactions: Effect of Species 1 on Species 2 Community Ecology Community - a group of organisms, of different species, living in the same area Community ecology is the study of the interactions between species The presence of one species may affect

More information

ENVE203 Environmental Engineering Ecology (Nov 05, 2012)

ENVE203 Environmental Engineering Ecology (Nov 05, 2012) ENVE203 Environmental Engineering Ecology (Nov 05, 2012) Elif Soyer Ecosystems and Living Organisms Population Density How Do Populations Change in Size? Maximum Population Growth Environmental Resistance

More information

BIOLOGY WORKSHEET GRADE: Two robins eating worms on the same lawn is an example of

BIOLOGY WORKSHEET GRADE: Two robins eating worms on the same lawn is an example of BIOLOGY WORKSHEET GRADE: 11 Q.1: Choose the letter of the best answer. 1. Two robins eating worms on the same lawn is an example of a. mutualism. b. commensalism. c. competition. d. parasitism. 2. Predation

More information