Putting predators back into behavioral predator prey interactions

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Putting predators back into behavioral predator prey interactions"

Transcription

1 70 Opinion 1 Huey, R.B. et al. (1999) Testing the adaptive significance of acclimation: a strong inference approach. Am. Zool. 39, Prosser, C.L. (1991) Environmental and Metabolic Animal Physiology: Comparative Animal Physiology, 4th edn, Wiley Liss 14 Gibert, P. et al. (001) Locomotor performance of Drosophila melanogaster: interactions among developmental and adult temperatures, age, and geography. Evolution 55, Usher, M.L. et al. (1994) Muscle development in Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar) embryos and the effect of temperature on muscle cellularity. J. Fish Biol. 44, Vieira, V.L.A. and Johnston, I.A. (199) Influence of temperature on muscle-fibre development in larvae of the herring Clupea harengus. Mar. Biol. 11, Johnston, I.A. et al. (1998) Embryonic temperature modulates muscle growth characteristics in larval and juvenile herring. J. Exp. Biol. 01, Hoffmann, A.A. and Hewa-Kapuge, S. (000) Acclimation for heat resistance in Trichogramma nr. brassicae: can it occur without costs? Funct. Ecol. 14, Matschak, T.W. et al. (1995) Is physiological hypoxia the driving force behind temperature effects on muscle development in embryonic Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar)? Differentiation 59, Matschak, T.W. et al. (1998) Metabolic enzyme activities in Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar L.) embryos respond more to chronic changes in oxygen availability than to environmental temperature. Fish Physiol. Biochem. 18, Matschak, T.W. et al. (1998) Temperature and oxygen tension influence the development of muscle cellularity in embryonic rainbow trout. J. Fish Biol. 53, Shine, R. and Downes, S.J. (1999) Can pregnant lizards adjust their offspring phenotypes to environmental conditions? Oecologia 119, Rasmussen, K.M. and Fischbeck, K.L. (1987) Effect of repeated reproductive cycles on pregnancy outcome in ad libitum-fed and chronically foodrestricted rats. J. Nutr. 117, Newman, R.A. (1998) Ecological constraints on amphibian metamorphosis: interactions of temperature and larval density with responses to changing food level. Oecologia 115, Thomson, L.J. et al. (001) Field and laboratory evidence for acclimation without costs in an egg parasitoid. Funct. Ecol. 15, Gibbs, A.G. et al. (1998) Effects of cuticular lipids and water balance in a desert Drosophila: is thermal acclimation beneficial? J. Exp. Biol. 01, Woods, H.A. and Harrison, J.F. (001) The beneficial acclimation hypothesis versus acclimation of specific traits: physiological changes in water-stressed Manduca sexta caterpillars. Physiol. Zool. 74, Zwaan, B. et al. (1995) Artificial selection for developmental time in Drosophila melanogaster in relation to the evolution of aging: direct and correlated responses. Evolution 49, Putting predators back into behavioral predator prey interactions Steven L. Lima In the study of behavioral predator prey interactions, predators have been treated as abstract sources of risk to which prey respond, rather than participants in a larger behavioral interaction. When predators are put back into the picture by allowing them to respond strategically to prey behavior, expectations about prey behavior can change. Something as simple as allowing predators to move in response to prey movements might not only (radically) alter standard expectations of prey behavior, but might also reveal new classes of behavioral phenomena that occur at large spatial scales. Similar revelations undoubtedly await many well-studied aspects of the behavioral interaction between predator and prey. Most examples studied to date, both theoretical and empirical, require attention from this predatory perspective. Putting predators back into the picture will be challenging, but doing so might change the way in which biologists think about predator prey interactions in general. Steven L. Lima Dept of Life Sciences, Indiana State University, Terre Haute, IN 47809, USA. S-Lima@indstate.edu Over the past 0 years, the study of behavioral interactions between predator and prey has shed much light on prey behavior, and it is now clear that almost any aspect of prey decision-making (from foraging behavior to mate choice) can be influenced by the risk of predation [1 3]. A growing literature also suggests that nonlethal interactions between predator and prey (those driven by prey avoidance of predation) might be an important component of predator prey interactions in general [4 8]. Work on behavioral predator prey interactions therefore provides an important bridge between the studies of behavior and ecology. In spite of these many advances, our understanding of behavioral predator prey interactions is limited by a simple oversight: we virtually forgot about the behavior of predators! Historically, we have been so focused on prey behavior that we (myself included) became comfortable with treating predators as unresponsive black boxes rather than participants in a behavioral interaction. This oversight has not only led to an incomplete view of behavioral interactions between predators and prey, but has also obscured an entire class of such interactions that occurs at large spatial scales. My goal is to explore some of the insights gained from putting predators back into behavioral predator prey interactions. How were predators removed from the interaction? The removal of predators from the behavioral predator prey interaction is apparent in the ubiquitous fixed-risk assumptions of constant attack rates over time and patch-specific risks of predation (e.g. Ref. [9]); such assumptions imply that predators are not influenced by prey behavior. As few would argue for the strict validity of this assumption, why were predators relegated to the status of unresponsive entities? In many ways, the fixed-risk approach (i.e. the assumption of unresponsive predators) was a sensible starting point. Characterizing predation risk as an environmental constant seemed reasonable given that predators can strike opportunistically and could be anywhere at a given time. Mathematical convenience might have also played a role: models of antipredator decisionmaking are much simpler under an assumption of fixed risk than they are when both predator and prey are allowed to respond to one another. Furthermore, /0/$ see front matter 00 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. PII: S (01)0393-X

2 Opinion 71 and perhaps most importantly, prey animals are usually much more amenable to observation and experimentation than are predators, and this has naturally led to more emphasis on the prey. Regardless of the reason, this fixed-risk approach has been a conceptual mainstay from the start [10,11]. In retrospect, predators need not have been excluded from the study of behavioral predator prey interactions for so long. For instance, early papers by Iwasa [1] and Sih [13] challenged (in essence) the fixed-risk paradigm, but this challenge was not appreciated as such at the time. A handful of models on predator prey coevolution [14] also allowed predators to respond to prey (albeit on a different time scale). Furthermore, students of foraging behavior have long known that predators are behaviorally sophisticated [15], yet predators were rendered behaviorally inert in the study of antipredator decision-making, in spite of the fact that many of the first students of antipredator decision-making also studied foraging behavior per se (myself, for instance). In moving away from the assumption of behaviorally inert prey in standard foraging models, we simply transferred, for the sake of tractability, the inertness assumption to predators. Conceptual issues in the return of predators Here, I consider some conceptual issues in bringing predators back into predator prey interactions. Most of these issues center on the surprising consequences of simply allowing predators to move about in search of prey. Predators on the move One of the most important realizations is that predators can move about in search of (patchy) prey and that prey can move to locate resources or to avoid predators. Foraging theory provides a rich body of work on how predators should choose among patches of immobile (and unresponsive) prey [15,16]. All else being equal, a predator should feed preferentially in patches with the highest density of prey. If predators compete for prey, then they might reach an ideal free distribution (IFD) determined, in part, by the distribution of prey items across patches or habitats [17]. A large body of theory also addresses patch choice by mobile prey feeding under a fixed risk of predation imposed by effectively immobile predators [16]. All else being equal, prey should prefer the patch with the lowest risk. Competitive patch choice among prey leads to behavior that is considerably more complex than that described by standard IFD theory, and might involve more than one stable distribution of prey in a given setting [18]. What happens when predator and prey can move simultaneously? We can expect that predators will go where prey are more dense, and that prey will simultaneously avoid areas where there are more predators. Both predator and prey have options that can potentially thwart the other, but the consequences of these options are not intuitively obvious. Mathematical game theory provides a natural framework within which to analyse these issues that surround putting predators back into predator prey interactions. Predator prey games of patch or habitat choice actually date back to Iwasa s [1] model of diel vertical migration by zooplankton and predatory fish. These game models typically assume a threetrophic-level system in which a predator species feeds on a prey species, which in turn feeds on its (nonresponsive) resource [19,0]. Predator and prey are free to move to any patch in the system. These models have a similar message: intuitive expectations are sometimes altered radically in a game of predator prey patch choice (Box 1). Under some circumstances, predators might appear to ignore prey and distribute themselves according to the distribution of the resource of the prey. Prey might appear to ignore their resource and base their distribution mainly on the risk imposed by predators [19 1]. Note that these results do not imply that prey decision-making is unresponsive to the risk of predation under some circumstances. Prey responses to risk are, in fact, an integral part of the equations determining evolutionarily stable behavior. Bouskila et al. [] add to the above theme of unintuitive expectations in their model of ontogenetic habitat shifts in developing larvae. These (generic) larval prey must decide at what time or size to shift from their natal habitat to a more dangerous but more energetically favorable habitat. Standard theory (fixed risk and unresponsive predators) suggests that the timing of the shift will be determined by a tradeoff between growth and mortality [4]. However, when predators can move between both habitats, prey might often maximize fitness simply by choosing the habitat that yields the highest growth rate. In essence, predator movement might negate the antipredator benefit of delaying the habitat switch. Conceptually, the most important message in this game theoretical work is the idea that risk is not a fixed property of a particular location when predators can move among patches. Our typical experimental and conceptual paradigm of fixed-risks and unresponsive predators allows us to see only the prey half of the overall interaction. One could question some of the assumptions in these simple game models, and it seems probable that more realistic assumptions (prey that interact socially in thwarting predators, state-dependent behavior, etc.) would change some of these topsy-turvy expectations. Nevertheless, the results are certainly cause for thought. Predators on the move over large areas Predators usually move over areas that are much larger than those typically considered in studies of behavioral predator prey interactions (which are generally on the scale at which individual predator

3 7 Opinion Box 1. The strange world of multitrophic-level games of habitat choice What happens when both predator and prey can move in response to resources and to each other? A few game-theoretical models have addressed this question in varying contexts. Although simplified in many ways, these models provide the clearest available picture of the potential consequences of putting predators back into predator prey interactions. Iwasa [a] first addressed this question in a three-trophic-level system composed of fish, their zooplanktonic prey, and (spatially fixed) phytoplankton. The fish and zooplankton were free to move between a surface habitat (dangerous for zooplankton but phytoplankton-rich) and a deep-water habitat (safe but phytoplankton-poor). His model suggested that zooplankton should base their distribution on the risk imposed by fish across habitat types, rather than on phytoplankton densities. However, phytoplankton densities should influence the distribution of fish, even though fish do not consume phytoplankton. This basic result was duplicated many years later in studies of adaptive predator aggregation and its effects on predator prey population dynamics [b,c]. Many of these topsy-turvy expectations were formalized in a general model by Hugie and Dill [d]. In addition to the predator prey interaction between trophic levels (once again, with a fixed resource for prey), their model included competition within predator and prey populations, as well as interference among predators. Their analysis suggested that the relative distribution of prey across two habitats (1 and ) will be as in Eqn I: N N 1 Here, N 1 and N are the densities of prey in habitats 1 and, P i is the density of predators, and c i is a coefficient of predator interference. Habitat riskiness is given by α i, which represents the joint effect of search rate and the probability of prey capture. For the predator, the relative distribution across habitat types is given by Eqn II: P ar1 mn1 = P ar mn 1 α = α ( 1+ c1p 1) ( 1+ c P ) 1 where R i is the productivity of the prey s resource in habitat i, a is the energy available to prey after subtracting foraging and processing costs, and m is the metabolic rate of the prey. These equations suggest that the relative riskiness of the two habitats will have a strong effect on the distribution of prey, and that the predators will be strongly influenced by the productivity of the prey s resource. Note that with little predator interference (c 0), and relatively low prey metabolic costs (m 0), Eqn I and II reduce to the case where prey match the inverse of habitat riskiness and predators match the productivity of prey resources: N 1 /N α /α 1 and P 1 /P R 1 /R. These counterintuitive matching expectations might also apply in a system with intraguild predation in which the predator can feed on both the prey and on the prey s [I] [II] resource [e]. However, the existence of an alternative resource for predators (unavailable to the prey in question) might result in a nearly fixed-risk situation for prey [e]. Sih [f] extended Hugie and Dill s model still further by incorporating variable degrees of intraspecific competition in both predator and prey populations. His model suggests that both prey and predator distributions might respond strongly to the productivity of the prey s resources across patches. However, the degree to which prey and predators match the resource productivity depends strongly on the degree of intraspecific competition in each trophic level. Generally speaking, weak predator competition favors the strong undermatching of resources by prey (i.e. they tend to avoid the rich patch). Strong predator competition tends to favor overmatching (i.e. stronger preference for the richer patch) by both predator and prey. Some results from earlier models emerge as special cases of Sih s model depending on the degree of intraspecific competition in the prey or predator populations. Finally, intuitive expectations fail again in Bouskila s [g] matrix-game analysis of a one prey two predator system involving snakes, owls and a desert rodent. The rodents and snakes can choose to be in a brushy microhabitat or in the open, whereas owls are found only in the open (owls do not participate directly in the game). Snakes are more effective in the brushy habitat than in the open, and are not themselves subject to predation. One might expect rodents to shift into the open habitat under conditions of no moonlight, increased food availability in the open, and increased dilution of risk in the open. However, Bouskila s analysis suggests that the distribution of rodents will nevertheless be (apparently) insensitive to such factors in the game scenario. However, these same factors cause the snakes to shift into the open habitat, much as one might have expected in the rodents. Some aspects of rodent behavior in the Mojave desert corroborate these basic expectations [h]. References a Iwasa, Y. (198) Vertical migration of zooplankton: a game between predator and prey. Am. Nat. 10, b van Baalen, M. and Sabelis, M.W. (1993) Coevolution of patch selection strategies of predator and prey and the consequences for ecological stability. Am. Nat. 14, c van Baalen, M. and Sabelis, M.W. (1999) Nonequilibrium population dynamics of ideal and free prey and predators. Am. Nat. 154, d Hugie, D.M. and Dill, L.M. (1994) Fish and game: a game theoretic approach to habitat selection by predators and prey. J. Fish Biol. 45, e Heithaus, M.R. (001) Habitat selection by predators and prey in communities with asymmetrical intraguild predation. Oikos 9, f Sih, A. (1998) Game theory and predator prey response races. In Advances in Game Theory and the Study of Animal Behavior (Dugatkin, L.A. and Reeve, H.K., eds), pp. 1 38, Oxford University Press g Bouskila, A. (001) A habitat selection game of interactions between rodents and their predators. Ann. Zool. Fennici 38, h Bouskila, A. (1995) Interactions between predation risk and competition: a field study of kangaroo rats and snakes. Ecology 76, and prey encounter one another). This simple realization raises an interesting question: is prey decision-making influenced by predator movement over the larger ecological landscape? I suspect that there are many behavioral phenomena that emerge as a result of large-scale predator movement (Box ). The common thread in such phenomena is that predator movement acts to link the behavior of prey across the home range of a predator; this linked behavior might, in turn, affect the behavior of the

4 Opinion 73 Box. Some possible large-scale behavioral predator prey interactions Large-scale behavioral interactions are driven fundamentally by the movement of predators across relatively large areas, and can manifest themselves in several ways. Here are three examples of large-scale phenomena that have received scattered attention. Predator prey shell games Predator prey shell games occur when predators search for elusive prey, and prey stay on the move to remain elusive. The possibility of such shell games is generally unrecognized, although the basic idea has been mentioned sporadically [a,b]. This view of prey movement is in marked contrast to the present consensus that movement and activity in general is dangerous for prey [c,d]. Note that predator prey shell games emerge only at a relatively large spatial scale at which prey and predator have many options for feeding sites. Furthermore, shell game movements are evolutionarily stable only when predators have a good spatial memory, and when prey have a reasonable chance of escaping attack; otherwise, movement might actually enhance the risk of predation for prey [e]. Predator prey shell games imply that the risk of predation experienced by prey at a given feeding location is directly related to the degree to which they are predictably at that location. Hence the exclusive patch preferences (i.e. spatially predictable foraging behavior) predicted by many models are unlikely under the shell game scenario; prey might bias their visitation towards the best feeding patches, but still spend a good deal of time foraging in lesser patches. Such partial preferences for patches are presently viewed mainly as the result of state-dependent foraging decisions [f]. The predator pass-along effect The predator pass-along effect is driven by predator movement that is induced by unsuccessful attacks following an unsuccessful attack, a predator s best option should often be to move on in search of other prey rather than pursue further attacks. Thus, an animal that successfully evades an attack simply passes the predator along to other prey individuals. The net result of this predator pass-along is an enhanced level of antipredator behavior in the prey population [g]. This effect implies that mortality per se might be a poor predictor of risk perceived. More fundamentally, the pass-along effect suggests that seemingly isolated prey (or groups of prey) interact indirectly via the (prey-induced) movement of the predator. This effect also suggests that the numerical dilution of risk experienced by a socially feeding animal depends not only on the size of its group, but also on the behavior of other groups in the area. Predator management of prey behavior Predators might manage the antipredator behavior of the prey in their home range in a way that leaves prey easier to capture. In essence, a predator might spread the risk over its many hunting sites to manage its prey s behavior (and the resulting pass-along effect) and its long-term energy intake rate. This idea of risk management (in one form or another) goes all the way back to Charnov et al. [h], and has been alluded to in various modeling contexts [g,i], but it has yet to be explored either theoretically or empirically. Prey management by a predator could be a kind of prudent predation if a predator has no competitors within its home range [h]. More generally, avoiding frequent attacks at a given location might simply be the best strategy for a predator. In any case, prey management requires stable prey and predator home ranges and the spatial and temporal cognitive abilities necessary to remember what has transpired at a particular site. Prey management might also modify the shell game by allowing prey to feed more predictably in their better feeding patches. References a Sonerud, G.A. (1985) Brood movements in grouse and waders as defence against win stay search in their predators. Oikos 44, b Greenberg, R. (000) Birds of many feathers: the formation and structure of mixed-species flocks of forest birds. In On the Move: How and Why Animals Travel in Groups (Boinski, S. and Garber, P.A., eds), pp , University of Chicago Press c Banks, P.B. et al. (000) Non-linearity in the predation risk of prey mobility. Proc. R. Soc. London B Biol. Sci. 67, d Boinski, S. et al. (000) A critical evaluation of the influence of predators on primates: effects on group travel. In On the Move: How and Why Animals Travel in Groups (Boinski, S. and Garber, P.A., eds), pp. 43 7, University of Chicago Press e Mitchell, W.A. and Lima, S.L. Predator prey shell games: large-scale movement and its implications for decision-making by prey. Oikos (in press) f Houston, A.I. and McNamara, J.M. (1999) Models of Adaptive Behavior, Cambridge University Press g Lima, S.L. (1990) Evolutionarily stable antipredator behavior among isolated foragers: on the consequences of successful escape. J. Theor. Biol. 143, h Charnov, E.L. et al. (1976) The ecological implications of resource depression. Am. Nat. 110, i Brown, J.S. et al. (1999) The ecology of fear: optimal foraging, game theory, and trophic interactions. J. Mammal. 80, predators. Some of these phenomena might change the way in which behavioral ecologists think about predator prey interactions at all spatial scales. Predators do more than move One can imagine predator prey games played out in a variety of behavioral contexts: predators can certainly do more than move in response to the behavior of their prey. Nevertheless, very few of these possibilities have been investigated theoretically, and the available models deal mainly with prey activity. The temporal activity of prey should respond to temporal changes in predation risk [3,4]. However, just as patch-specific predation risk reflects a behavioral interaction between predator and prey (Box 1), so too should predation risk over time. Brown et al. [5] make this point nicely in a model of the temporal activity of predator and prey, in which a top predator feeds on a prey species that consumes a depleting resource. Both predator and prey are allowed to be active at any time in response to the activity of the other. The results represent a kind of temporal analog of the results in Box 1: predator activity tends to mirror the level of the resource of the prey, whilst the proportion of active prey remains constant over time. Models that assume constant or

5 74 Opinion fixed predator behavior [3,4] cannot capture these effects. The issue of when to resume activity following an encounter with a predator has been partially analysed by Johansson and Englund [6], who suggest that evolutionary stability might not be a general feature of such interactions (see also Ref. [7]). In addition, predator prey games of activity figure prominently in some recent models that translate behavioral decisions to population-level phenomena [8,5,8]. Empirical studies where do we stand? We stand on shaky ground. As mentioned before, much is known about the behavior of predators feeding on unresponsive prey, and that of prey feeding under a fixed risk of predation. However, empirically, almost nothing is known about the kinds of predator prey interactions outlined above. Surprisingly, even the straightforward and provocative game theoretical analyses of patch choice (Box 1) remain untested (but see Refs [13,1,9]). As for large-scale interactions (Box ), no empirical information exists beyond a few suggestive anecdotes [30,31]. More generally, how much is known about the way in which predator and prey react to each other s behavioral options? These mutual responses form the basis of the game theoretical models. Here again, we come up largely empty-handed. For example, in the study of pursuit-deterrence signaling by prey to predator [3], the crux of the interaction is that predators are indeed deterred from attacking by honest signals of the ability to escape. We nevertheless know surprisingly little about the effects that such signaling has on predators [33]. Similarly, an understanding of the tactics of predator attack (and how they relate to prey behavior) could greatly alter our interpretation of vigilance behavior [34,35]. In spite of decades of research on vigilance [36], only two studies provide any such information [37,38]. Finally, consider the small bird in winter research paradigm, in which small birds must survive the rigors of winter and the onslaught of the hawks: in light of the conceptual prominence of this paradigm in the study of antipredator decision-making [3], the predatory behavior of Accipiter hawks is virtually unstudied [39]. This empirical focus on prey behavior has had other unintended effects. First, our lack of attention to predators leaves us with little information on the effectiveness of antipredator decision-making in reducing predation. Thus, it is troubling that a recent well-worked example suggests that refuging behavior in a stream-dwelling salamander species is an ineffective defense against their main predator [40]. Nevertheless, I suspect that such decision-making is generally effective at lowering the risk of predation, but more confirmation of this suspicion [39,41,4] is certainly desirable. Second, our lack of attention to predators has left us with little understanding of antipredator behavior in a multipredator environment. Because predators were usually treated as unresponsive abstractions, they were effectively lumped together as a single abstract entity. Hence, we have lost sight of the fact that most environments harbor many different types of predators. The behavioral ramifications of a multipredator environment are thus largely unexplored [43 45]. Where do we go from here? Almost all aspects of putting predators back into behavioral predator prey interactions need attention. Theoretically, even the most well-studied aspects of such interactions have only rarely been explored as games between predator and prey. Particularly fruitful might be explorations of predator prey games in the context of classic optimal foraging theory. For example, some failures of standard optimal diet theory [46] might be explicable in terms of a predator prey game. We also need to think big and explore large-scale behavioral phenomena (Box ). Further elaboration of existing game theory should also prove rewarding. One-on-one games between individual predator and prey (e.g. how long should a predator pursue prey during an encounter) demand more attention especially as they relate to conditions favoring the existence of an evolutionary stable strategy. Further refinements of multitrophiclevel games of patch choice (Box 1) could perhaps consider the kinds of information that animals might have available to them (which might vary with the scale of the physical system in question). Such considerations not only add a degree of realism to models, but might also significantly alter current theoretical expectations [47,48] and their implications for population dynamics [8,49]. Empirically, there is even more to be done. In particular, game-theoretical models (Box 1) must be tested. In a related issue, we need much more information on the way in which predator and prey respond to the behavior of the other. Particularly useful will be techniques that are designed to reveal the response of predators to behaviors that prey would not normally use in a given situation or vice versa. Krause and Godin [38] provide a nice example of such a technique in which a large fish (behind a two-way mirror) was allowed to watch smaller fish as the latter fed completely unaware of the former; certain prey feeding postures consistently invoked attacks by the predator, revealing why these postures are rarely used in the presence of predators. This sort of information can help characterize the behavioral dynamics underlying predator prey games. Furthermore, large-scale behavioral phenomena are virtually unexplored and thus in much need of empirical attention. This large-scale work could, in part, address the question of whether prey have large-scale information about predator behavior and that of other prey in the area, or whether large-scale phenomena emerge simply as a consequence of

6 Opinion 75 Acknowledgements I thank William Mitchell, Timothy Roth, Todd Steury and three anonymous reviewers for helpful comments. Mart Gross provided valuable help during the early stages of this project. small-scale decision making. At any scale of study, it might often be necessary to study the natural history of a predator before the finer points of behavioral predator prey interactions can be addressed. Finally, future work will need to confront some difficult questions raised by game theory. For instance, in multitrophic-level games of patch choice (Box 1), will perceptually constrained animals approach the behavioral equilibria suggested by simple game theory? Are there simple behavioral rules-of-thumb [50] that will allow predator and prey to approach Nash equilibria even if prey know little about the behavior of predators and vice versa? What can prey really know about predator behavior in ecological time (beyond the instinctual knowledge of the prey)? Not only are these interesting behavioral questions, but the answers might ultimately also dictate the precision with which simple game theoretical models can predict behavior [48]. In conclusion, there is much to be gained from putting predators back into behavioral predator prey interactions. This task will not be an easy one, especially at larger spatial scales, but the behavioral and ecological insights gained from doing so will be well worth the effort. Can one expect a sweeping revolution in our view of these behavioral interactions? I would not go that far. I do expect, however, that a greater emphasis on the behavior of predators might change the way in which we think about predator prey interactions in general. References 1 Sih, A. (1994) Predation risk and the evolutionary ecology of reproductive behaviour. J. Fish Biol. 45, Kats, L.B. and Dill, L.M. (1998) The scent of death: chemosensory assessment of predation risk by prey animals. Ecoscience 5, Lima, S.L. (1998) Stress and decision making under the risk of predation: recent developments from behavioral, reproductive, and ecological perspectives. Adv. Study Behav. 7, Werner, E.E. and Gilliam, J.F. (1984) The ontogenetic niche and species interactions in size-structured populations. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 15, Kotler, B.P. et al. (1994) The role of predation in shaping the behaviour, morphology, and community organisation of desert rodents. Aust. J. Zool. 4, Anholt, B.R. (1997) How should we test for the role of behaviour in population dynamics? Evol. Ecol. 11, Schmitz, O.J. (1998) Direct and indirect effects of predation and predation risk in old-field interaction webs. Am. Nat. 151, Brown, J.S. et al. (1999) The ecology of fear: optimal foraging, game theory, and trophic interactions. J. Mammal. 80, Lima, S.L. (1985) Maximizing feeding efficiency and minimizing time exposed to predators: a trade-off in the black-capped chickadee. Oecologia 66, Milinski, M. and Heller, R. (1978) Influence of a predator on the optimal foraging behaviour of sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus L.). Nature 75, Mangel, M. and Clark, C.W. (1988) Dynamic Modeling in Behavioral Ecology, Princeton University Press 1 Iwasa, Y. (198) Vertical migration of zooplankton: a game between predator and prey. Am. Nat. 10, Sih, A. (1984) The behavioral response race between predator and prey. Am. Nat. 13, Abrams, P.A. (000) The evolution of predator prey interactions: theory and evidence. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 31, Stephens, D.W. and Krebs, J.R. (1986) Foraging Theory, Princeton University Press 16 Houston, A.I. and McNamara, J.M. (1999) Models of Adaptive Behavior, Cambridge University Press 17 Sutherland, W.J. (1996) From Individual Behaviour to Population Ecology, Oxford University Press 18 Moody, A.L. et al. (1996) Ideal free distributions under predation risk. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 38, Hugie, D.M. and Dill, L.M. (1994) Fish and game: a game theoretic approach to habitat selection by predators and prey. J. Fish Biol. 45, Sih, A. (1998) Game theory and predator prey response races. In Advances in Game Theory and the Study of Animal Behavior (Dugatkin, L.A. and Reeve, H.K., eds), pp. 1 38, Oxford University Press 1 Bouskila, A. (001) A habitat selection game of interactions between rodents and their predators. Ann. Zool. Fennici 38, Bouskila, A. et al. (1998) Life-history decisions under predation risk: importance of a game perspective. Evol. Ecol. 1, Clark, C.W. and Levy, D.A. (1988) Diel vertical migrations by juvenile sockeye salmon and the antipredation window. Am. Nat. 131, McNamara, J.M. et al. (1994) Foraging routines of small birds in winter: a theoretical investigation. J. Avian Biol. 5, Brown, J.S. et al. (001) Ecology of fear: foraging games between predators and prey with pulsed resources. Ann. Zool. Fennici 38, Johansson, A. and Englund, G. (1995) A predator prey game between bullheads and casemaking caddis larvae. Anim. Behav. 50, Vega-Redondo, F. and Hasson, O. (1993) A gametheoretic model of predator prey signaling. J. Theor. Biol. 16, Crowley, P.H. and Hopper, K.R. (1994) How to behave around cannibals: a density-dependant dynamic game. Am. Nat. 143, Formanowicz, D.R., Jr and Bobka, M.S. (1989) Predation risk and microhabitat preference: an experimental study of the behavioral responses of prey and predator. Am. Midl. Nat. 11, Lima, S.L. (1990) Evolutionarily stable antipredator behavior among isolated foragers: on the consequences of successful escape. J. Theor. Biol. 143, Greenberg, R. (000) Birds of many feathers: the formation and structure of mixed-species flocks of forest birds. In On the Move: How and Why Animals Travel in Groups (Boinski, S. and Garber, P.A., eds), pp , University of Chicago Press 3 Caro, T.M. (1995) Pursuit-deterrence revisited. Trends Ecol. Evol. 10, Caro, T.M. et al. (1995) Tail-flagging and other antipredator signals in white-tailed deer: new data and synthesis. Behav. Ecol. 6, Packer, C. and Abrams, P. (1990) Should co-operative groups be more vigilant than selfish groups? J. Theor. Biol. 14, McNamara, J.M. and Houston, A.I. (199) Evolutionarily stable levels of vigilance as a function of group size. Anim. Behav. 43, Bednekoff, P.A. and Lima, S.L. (1998) Randomness, chaos, and confusion in the study of anti-predator vigilance. Trends Ecol. Evol. 13, FitzGibbon, C.D. (1989) A cost to individuals with reduced vigilance in groups of Thomson s gazelles hunted by cheetahs. Anim. Behav. 37, Krause, J. and Godin, J-G.J. (1996) Influence of prey foraging posture on flight behavior and predation risk: predators take advantage of unwary prey. Behav. Ecol. 7, Cresswell, W. (1994) Flocking is an effective antipredation strategy in redshanks, Tringa totanus. Anim. Behav. 47, Sih, A. et al. (000) Does phylogenetic inertia explain the evolution of ineffective antipredator behavior in a sunfish-salamander system? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 49, Godin, J-G.J. and Smith, S.A. (1988) A fitness cost of foraging in the guppy. Nature 333, Clutton-Brock, T.H. et al. (1999) Predation, group size and mortality in a cooperative mongoose, Suricata suricatta. J. Anim. Ecol. 68, Kotler, B.P. et al. (199) Predator facilitation: the combined effect of snakes and owls on the foraging behavior of gerbils. Ann. Zool. Fennici 9, Matsuda, H. et al. (1993) The effect of adaptive antipredator behavior on exploitative competition and mutualism between predators. Oikos 68, Sih, A. et al. (1998) Emergent impacts of multiple predators on prey. Trends Ecol. Evol. 13, Sih, A. and Christensen, B. (001) Optimal diet theory: when does it work, and when does it fail? Anim. Behav. 61, Schwinning, S. and Rosenzweig, M.L. (1990) Periodic oscillations in an ideal-free predator prey distribution. Oikos 59, Bernstein, C. et al. (1999) Predator migration decisions, the ideal free distribution, and predator prey dynamics. Am. Nat. 153, Luttbeg, B. and Schmitz, O.J. (000) Predator and prey models with flexible individual behavior and imperfect information. Am. Nat. 155, Beauchamp, G. (000) Learning rules for social foragers: implications for the producer-scrounger game and ideal free distribution theory. J. Theor. Biol. 07, 1 35

Predators feeding on behaviourally responsive prey: some implications for classical models of optimal diet choice

Predators feeding on behaviourally responsive prey: some implications for classical models of optimal diet choice Evolutionary Ecology Research, 2003, 5: 1083 1102 Predators feeding on behaviourally responsive prey: some implications for classical models of optimal diet choice Steven L. Lima,* William A. Mitchell

More information

PREDATOR AND PREY HABITAT SELECTION GAMES: THE EFFECTS OF HOW PREY BALANCE FORAGING AND PREDATION RISK

PREDATOR AND PREY HABITAT SELECTION GAMES: THE EFFECTS OF HOW PREY BALANCE FORAGING AND PREDATION RISK ISRAEL JOURNAL OF ZOOLOGY, Vol. 50, 2004, pp. 233 254 PREDATOR AND PREY HABITAT SELECTION GAMES: THE EFFECTS OF HOW PREY BALANCE FORAGING AND PREDATION RISK BARNEY LUTTBEG* AND ANDREW SIH Department of

More information

Predation risk, unequal competitors and the ideal free distribution

Predation risk, unequal competitors and the ideal free distribution Evolutionary Ecology Research, 1999, 1: 389 409 Predation risk, unequal competitors and the ideal free distribution Tamara C. Grand* and Lawrence M. Dill Behavioural Ecology Research Group, Department

More information

A spatial game between a predator and social prey in multiple patches*

A spatial game between a predator and social prey in multiple patches* Evolutionary Ecology Research, 2018, 19: 469 476 A spatial game between a predator and social prey in multiple patches* William A. Mitchell Department of Biology, Indiana State University, Terre Haute,

More information

The process of habitat selection frequently requires individuals

The process of habitat selection frequently requires individuals Behavioral Ecology Vol. 13 No. 2: 280 290 Alternative forms of competition and predation dramatically affect habitat selection under foraging predation-risk trade-offs Tamara C. Grand Department of Zoology,

More information

Testing the beneficial acclimation hypothesis

Testing the beneficial acclimation hypothesis 66 Opinion Testing the beneficial acclimation hypothesis Robbie S. Wilson and Craig E. Franklin Recent developments in evolutionary physiology have seen many of the long-held assumptions within comparative

More information

Lecture 2: Individual-based Modelling

Lecture 2: Individual-based Modelling Lecture 2: Individual-based Modelling Part I Steve Railsback Humboldt State University Department of Mathematics & Lang, Railsback & Associates Arcata, California USA www.langrailsback.com 1 Outline 1.

More information

Niche The sum of all interactions a species has with biotic/abiotic components of the environment N-dimensional hypervolume

Niche The sum of all interactions a species has with biotic/abiotic components of the environment N-dimensional hypervolume Niche The sum of all interactions a species has with biotic/abiotic components of the environment N-dimensional hypervolume Each dimension is a biotic or abiotic resource Ecomorphology Ecology (niche)

More information

Fear and loathing on the landscape: What can foraging theory tell us about vigilance and fear? Commentary on Beauchamp on Fear & Vigilance

Fear and loathing on the landscape: What can foraging theory tell us about vigilance and fear? Commentary on Beauchamp on Fear & Vigilance Fear and loathing on the landscape: What can foraging theory tell us about vigilance and fear? Commentary on Beauchamp on Fear & Vigilance Burt P. Kotler Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Israel Joel

More information

Natal versus breeding dispersal: Evolution in a model system

Natal versus breeding dispersal: Evolution in a model system Evolutionary Ecology Research, 1999, 1: 911 921 Natal versus breeding dispersal: Evolution in a model system Karin Johst 1 * and Roland Brandl 2 1 Centre for Environmental Research Leipzig-Halle Ltd, Department

More information

BIOS 6150: Ecology Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences

BIOS 6150: Ecology Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences BIOS 6150: Ecology Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences Week 6: Predation and predatory behavior: Lecture summary: Nature of predation. Diet breadth & choice. Optimal foraging. Functional

More information

Ecology Symbiotic Relationships

Ecology Symbiotic Relationships Ecology Symbiotic Relationships Overview of the Co-evolution and Relationships Exhibited Among Community Members What does Symbiosis mean? How do we define Symbiosis? Symbiosis in the broadest sense is

More information

ON THE INTERPLAY OF PREDATOR SWITCHING AND PREY EVASION IN DETERMINING THE STABILITY OF PREDATOR PREY DYNAMICS

ON THE INTERPLAY OF PREDATOR SWITCHING AND PREY EVASION IN DETERMINING THE STABILITY OF PREDATOR PREY DYNAMICS ISRAEL JOURNAL OF ZOOLOGY, Vol. 50, 2004, pp. 187 205 ON THE INTERPLAY OF PREDATOR SWITCHING AND PREY EVASION IN DETERMINING THE STABILITY OF PREDATOR PREY DYNAMICS TRISTAN KIMBRELL* AND ROBERT D. HOLT

More information

Do short-term behavioural responses of consumers in tri-trophic food chains persist at the population time-scale?

Do short-term behavioural responses of consumers in tri-trophic food chains persist at the population time-scale? Evolutionary Ecology Research, 2004, 6: 1063 1081 Do short-term behavioural responses of consumers in tri-trophic food chains persist at the population time-scale? Vlastimil Křivan 1 * and Etienne Sirot

More information

The ideal free distribution: an analysis of the perceptual limit model

The ideal free distribution: an analysis of the perceptual limit model Evolutionary Ecology Research, 2002, 4: 471 493 The ideal free distribution: an analysis of the perceptual limit model Edmund J. Collins,* Alasdair I. Houston and Alison Lang Centre for Behavioural Biology,

More information

Summary. A Bird s- Eye View of Community and Population Effects of Ontogenetic Development

Summary. A Bird s- Eye View of Community and Population Effects of Ontogenetic Development Chapter one Summary A Bird s- Eye View of Community and Population Effects of Ontogenetic Development Why start with summarizing the contents of a book? In the present case we see at least two good reasons.

More information

The Problem of Where to Live

The Problem of Where to Live April 5: Habitat Selection: Intro The Problem of Where to Live Physical and biotic environment critically affects fitness An animal's needs may be met only in certain habitats, which should select for

More information

Adaptation. Biotic and Abiotic Environments. Eric R. Pianka

Adaptation. Biotic and Abiotic Environments. Eric R. Pianka Adaptation Eric R. Pianka To survive and reproduce, all living organisms must adjust to conditions imposed on them by their environments. An organism's environment includes everything impinging upon it,

More information

Antagonistic and Synergistic Interactions Among Predators

Antagonistic and Synergistic Interactions Among Predators Bulletin of Mathematical Biology 2007 69: 2093 2104 DOI 10.1007/s11538-007-9214-0 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Antagonistic and Synergistic Interactions Among Predators Gary R. Huxel Department of Biological Sciences,

More information

Gary G. Mittelbach Michigan State University

Gary G. Mittelbach Michigan State University Community Ecology Gary G. Mittelbach Michigan State University Sinauer Associates, Inc. Publishers Sunderland, Massachusetts U.S.A. Brief Table of Contents 1 Community Ecology s Roots 1 PART I The Big

More information

BIOS 3010: Ecology Lecture 8: Predator foraging & prey defense. 2. Predation: 3. Predator diet breadth and preference:

BIOS 3010: Ecology Lecture 8: Predator foraging & prey defense. 2. Predation: 3. Predator diet breadth and preference: BIOS 3010: Ecology Lecture 8: Predator foraging & prey defense 1. Lecture Summary: What is predation? Predator diet breadth. Preference & switching. Optimal foraging. Marginal value theorem. Functional

More information

Foraging. This week in Animal Behaviour. How do animals choose? Do animals choose their food?

Foraging. This week in Animal Behaviour. How do animals choose? Do animals choose their food? This week in Animal Behaviour Lecture 22: Cooperation Lecture 23: Foraging Lecture 24: TBA Text chapters 10-11 LAB: Project 2 proposal seminars Midterm 2 on Tuesday Nov. 8 Foraging 1. Models of foraging

More information

Measuring the population-level consequences of predator-induced prey movement

Measuring the population-level consequences of predator-induced prey movement Evolutionary Ecology Research, 2008, 10: 333 350 Measuring the population-level consequences of predator-induced prey movement Peter A. Abrams* Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, Zoology Building,

More information

BIO S380T Page 1 Summer 2005: Exam 2

BIO S380T Page 1 Summer 2005: Exam 2 BIO S380T Page 1 Part I: Definitions. [5 points for each term] For each term, provide a brief definition that also indicates why the term is important in ecology or evolutionary biology. Where I ve provided

More information

Ecology Regulation, Fluctuations and Metapopulations

Ecology Regulation, Fluctuations and Metapopulations Ecology Regulation, Fluctuations and Metapopulations The Influence of Density on Population Growth and Consideration of Geographic Structure in Populations Predictions of Logistic Growth The reality of

More information

Understanding Populations Section 1. Chapter 8 Understanding Populations Section1, How Populations Change in Size DAY ONE

Understanding Populations Section 1. Chapter 8 Understanding Populations Section1, How Populations Change in Size DAY ONE Chapter 8 Understanding Populations Section1, How Populations Change in Size DAY ONE What Is a Population? A population is a group of organisms of the same species that live in a specific geographical

More information

Daniel T. Blumstein and William E. Cooper, Jr.

Daniel T. Blumstein and William E. Cooper, Jr. 17 Afterword Daniel T. Blumstein and William E. Cooper, Jr. 17.1 Introduction As the chapters in this volume have shown, the field of escape behavior is both active and diverse. What has particularly excited

More information

Within predator-prey systems, interference has been defined

Within predator-prey systems, interference has been defined Behavioral Ecology Vol. 11 No. 6: 597 605 Predator search pattern and the strength of interference through prey depression Richard A. Stillman, John D. Goss-Custard, and Matthew J. Alexander Centre for

More information

Adaptive Traits. Natural selection results in evolution of adaptations. Adaptation: trait that enhances an organism's survival and reproduction

Adaptive Traits. Natural selection results in evolution of adaptations. Adaptation: trait that enhances an organism's survival and reproduction Adaptive Traits Adaptive Traits Natural selection results in evolution of adaptations Adaptation: trait that enhances an organism's survival and reproduction Nothing in biology makes sense except in the

More information

Life history evolution

Life history evolution Life history evolution Key concepts ˆ Cole s paradox ˆ Tradeoffs ad the evolution of iteroparity ˆ Bet hedging in random environments Life history theory The life history of a species consists of its life

More information

Metacommunities Spatial Ecology of Communities

Metacommunities Spatial Ecology of Communities Spatial Ecology of Communities Four perspectives for multiple species Patch dynamics principles of metapopulation models (patchy pops, Levins) Mass effects principles of source-sink and rescue effects

More information

Computational Ecology Introduction to Ecological Science. Sonny Bleicher Ph.D.

Computational Ecology Introduction to Ecological Science. Sonny Bleicher Ph.D. Computational Ecology Introduction to Ecological Science Sonny Bleicher Ph.D. Ecos Logos Defining Ecology Interactions: Organisms: Plants Animals: Bacteria Fungi Invertebrates Vertebrates The physical

More information

Biology 11 Unit 1: Fundamentals. Lesson 1: Ecology

Biology 11 Unit 1: Fundamentals. Lesson 1: Ecology Biology 11 Unit 1: Fundamentals Lesson 1: Ecology Objectives In this section you will be learning about: ecosystem structure energy flow through an ecosystem photosynthesis and cellular respiration factors

More information

Interspecific Competition

Interspecific Competition Interspecific Competition Intraspecific competition Classic logistic model Interspecific extension of densitydependence Individuals of other species may also have an effect on per capita birth & death

More information

A Game Theory Based Predation Behavior Model

A Game Theory Based Predation Behavior Model A Game Theory Based Predation Behavior Model Shi Chen,Sheng Bao 2008-01-09 1 Abstract In this paper we first use a simplified non-cooperative zero sum Game Theory model model to investigate how predators

More information

Living in groups 1. What are three costs and three benefits of living in groups?

Living in groups 1. What are three costs and three benefits of living in groups? Study questions Living in groups 1. What are three costs and three benefits of living in groups? 2. What is the dilution effect? What is a key assumption of the dilution effect hypothesis? What is a biological

More information

Community Structure. Community An assemblage of all the populations interacting in an area

Community Structure. Community An assemblage of all the populations interacting in an area Community Structure Community An assemblage of all the populations interacting in an area Community Ecology The ecological community is the set of plant and animal species that occupy an area Questions

More information

OPTIMAL FORAGING MODELS

OPTIMAL FORAGING MODELS 26 OPTIMAL FORAGING MODELS In collaboration with David N. Bonter Objectives Develop a spreadsheet model of foraging choices among two prey types, prey 1 and prey 2. Determine the conditions in which individuals

More information

A population subjected to only density-independent factors can not persist over a long period of time eventually go to extinction

A population subjected to only density-independent factors can not persist over a long period of time eventually go to extinction A population subjected to only density-independent factors can not persist over a long period of time eventually go to extinction K is constant over time does not vary year to year etc. dn / Ndt declines

More information

Name Student ID. Good luck and impress us with your toolkit of ecological knowledge and concepts!

Name Student ID. Good luck and impress us with your toolkit of ecological knowledge and concepts! Page 1 BIOLOGY 150 Final Exam Winter Quarter 2000 Before starting be sure to put your name and student number on the top of each page. MINUS 3 POINTS IF YOU DO NOT WRITE YOUR NAME ON EACH PAGE! You have

More information

Evolutionary Games and Computer Simulations

Evolutionary Games and Computer Simulations Evolutionary Games and Computer Simulations Bernardo A. Huberman and Natalie S. Glance Dynamics of Computation Group Xerox Palo Alto Research Center Palo Alto, CA 94304 Abstract The prisoner s dilemma

More information

Grade

Grade www.abubakrshalaby.com 5 Grade Ecology is the scientific study of the relation of living organisms to each other and their surroundings. Ecology includes the study of plant and animal populations, plant

More information

Revisiting the positive correlation between female size and egg size

Revisiting the positive correlation between female size and egg size Evolutionary Ecology Research, 2003, 5: 421 429 Revisiting the positive correlation between female size and egg size Andrew P. Hendry 1 * and Troy Day 2 1 Redpath Museum and Department of Biology, McGill

More information

Credit or debit? Resource input changes population dynamics of cityslicker

Credit or debit? Resource input changes population dynamics of cityslicker FORUM FORUM FORUM FORUM is intended for new ideas or new ways of interpreting existing information. It provides a chance for suggesting hypotheses and for challenging current thinking on ecological issues.

More information

Ski touring and fauna: which interactions?

Ski touring and fauna: which interactions? Ski touring and fauna: which interactions? Antoine DUPARC Workshop "Wildlife and winter sport activities" Lescheraines March 4th 2016 Increase of interaction human/fauna Increase of human activities all

More information

Principles of Animal Behavior

Principles of Animal Behavior Animals and Humans! Lee Alan Dugatkin Principles of Animal Behavior THIRD EDITION Chapter 1 Principles of Animal Behavior We are surrounded by animals and many humans like to know them" Early human art

More information

Composable Group Behaviors

Composable Group Behaviors Composable Group Behaviors Perpetual Amoah Undergraduate S. Researcher Sam Rodriguez Graduate Student Mentor Jyh-Ming Lien Graduate Student Mentor John Maffei Graduate Student Mentor Nancy M. Amato Faculty

More information

Comparison Among Species

Comparison Among Species Comparison Among Species Scientific Rationales for Comparison Theoretical: Provides insights into evolution and adaptive function of behavior (including our own) Insights into mechanism: discover deeper

More information

Student Name: Teacher: Date: District: London City. Assessment: 07 Science Science Test 4. Description: Life Science Final 1.

Student Name: Teacher: Date: District: London City. Assessment: 07 Science Science Test 4. Description: Life Science Final 1. Student Name: Teacher: Date: District: London City Assessment: 07 Science Science Test 4 Description: Life Science Final 1 Form: 301 1. A food chain is shown. Sunlight Grass Rabbit Snake What is the abiotic

More information

BIOS 5970: Plant-Herbivore Interactions Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences

BIOS 5970: Plant-Herbivore Interactions Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences BIOS 5970: Plant-Herbivore Interactions Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences D. POPULATION & COMMUNITY DYNAMICS Week 13. Herbivory, predation & parasitism: Lecture summary: Predation:

More information

Ch.5 Evolution and Community Ecology How do organisms become so well suited to their environment? Evolution and Natural Selection

Ch.5 Evolution and Community Ecology How do organisms become so well suited to their environment? Evolution and Natural Selection Ch.5 Evolution and Community Ecology How do organisms become so well suited to their environment? Evolution and Natural Selection Gene: A sequence of DNA that codes for a particular trait Gene pool: All

More information

Alternatives to competition. Lecture 13. Facilitation. Functional types of consumers. Stress Gradient Hypothesis

Alternatives to competition. Lecture 13. Facilitation. Functional types of consumers. Stress Gradient Hypothesis Lecture 13 Finishing Competition and Facilitation Consumer-Resource interactions Predator-prey population dynamics Do predators regulate prey? Lotka-Volterra predator-prey model Predator behavior matters:

More information

A habitat selection game of interactions between rodents and their predators

A habitat selection game of interactions between rodents and their predators Ann. Zool. Fennici 38: 55 70 ISSN 0003-455X Helsinki 2001 Finnish Zoological and Botanical Publishing Board 2001 A habitat selection game of interactions between rodents and their predators Amos Bouskila

More information

Ecology of fear: Foraging games between predators and prey with pulsed resources

Ecology of fear: Foraging games between predators and prey with pulsed resources Ann. Zool. Fennici 38: 71 87 ISSN 0003-455X Helsinki 2001 Finnish Zoological and Botanical Publishing Board 2001 Ecology of fear: Foraging games between predators and prey with pulsed resources Joel S.

More information

Phenotypic variation 3/6/17. Phenotypic plasticity in amphibians

Phenotypic variation 3/6/17. Phenotypic plasticity in amphibians Phenotypic plasticity in amphibians Goals Overview of phenotypic plasticity Summary of different types of plasticity Discuss costs and benefits of plasticity Discuss complexity of plasticity Readings Wells:

More information

On Evolutionary Stability in Predator-Prey Models with Fast Behavioral Dynamics

On Evolutionary Stability in Predator-Prey Models with Fast Behavioral Dynamics Wilfrid Laurier University Scholars Commons @ Laurier Mathematics Faculty Publications Mathematics 2009 On Evolutionary Stability in Predator-Prey Models with Fast Behavioral Dynamics Vlastimil Křivan

More information

Case Studies in Ecology and Evolution

Case Studies in Ecology and Evolution 7 Competition (this chapter is still unfinished) Species compete in many ways. Sometimes there are dramatic contests, such as when male bighorns compete for access to mates. Territoriality. That kind of

More information

Coevolution of predators and prey

Coevolution of predators and prey Coevolution of predators and prey 1) Evolution of predator-prey interactions 2) Arms race analogy 3) Examples of predator-prey coevolution? 4) Steady-state theory 5) Evolution of aposematic coloration

More information

Ecology Notes CHANGING POPULATIONS

Ecology Notes CHANGING POPULATIONS Ecology Notes TEK 8.11 (B) Investigate how organisms and populations in an ecosystem depend on and may compete for biotic and abiotic factors such as quantity of light, water, range of temperatures, or

More information

3/24/10. Amphibian community ecology. Lecture goal. Lecture concepts to know

3/24/10. Amphibian community ecology. Lecture goal. Lecture concepts to know Amphibian community ecology Lecture goal To familiarize students with the abiotic and biotic factors that structure amphibian communities, patterns in species richness, and encourage discussion about community

More information

Lesson Overview 4.2 Niches and Community Interactions

Lesson Overview 4.2 Niches and Community Interactions THINK ABOUT IT If you ask someone where an organism lives, that person might answer on a coral reef or in the desert. Lesson Overview 4.2 Niches and Community Interactions These answers give the environment

More information

Stability with Inheritance in the Conditional Strategy

Stability with Inheritance in the Conditional Strategy J. theor. Biol. (1998) 192, 445 453 Stability with Inheritance in the Conditional Strategy MART R. GROSS* AND JOE REPKA * Department of Zoology, University of Toronto, 25Harbord Street, Toronto, Ontario,

More information

AP Curriculum Framework with Learning Objectives

AP Curriculum Framework with Learning Objectives Big Ideas Big Idea 1: The process of evolution drives the diversity and unity of life. AP Curriculum Framework with Learning Objectives Understanding 1.A: Change in the genetic makeup of a population over

More information

11.6. Patterns in Evolution. Evolution through natural selection is not random.

11.6. Patterns in Evolution. Evolution through natural selection is not random. 11.6 Patterns in Evolution VOCABULARY convergent evolution divergent evolution coevolution extinction punctuated equilibrium adaptive radiation > Key Concept Evolution occurs in patterns. MAIN IDEAS Evolution

More information

Non-independence in Statistical Tests for Discrete Cross-species Data

Non-independence in Statistical Tests for Discrete Cross-species Data J. theor. Biol. (1997) 188, 507514 Non-independence in Statistical Tests for Discrete Cross-species Data ALAN GRAFEN* AND MARK RIDLEY * St. John s College, Oxford OX1 3JP, and the Department of Zoology,

More information

Stability in negotiation games and the emergence of cooperation

Stability in negotiation games and the emergence of cooperation Received 3 September 23 Accepted 4 November 23 Published online 3 January 24 Stability in negotiation games and the emergence of cooperation Peter D. Taylor * and Troy Day Department of Mathematics and

More information

Community and Population Ecology Populations & Communities Species Diversity Sustainability and Environmental Change Richness and Sustainability

Community and Population Ecology Populations & Communities Species Diversity Sustainability and Environmental Change Richness and Sustainability 1 2 3 4 Community and Population Ecology Chapter 6 Populations & Communities Biosphere> ecosystems> communities> populations> individuals A population is all of the individuals of the same species in a

More information

Comparing male densities and fertilization rates as potential Allee effects in Alaskan and Canadian Ursus maritimus populations

Comparing male densities and fertilization rates as potential Allee effects in Alaskan and Canadian Ursus maritimus populations Comparing male densities and fertilization rates as potential Allee effects in Alaskan and Canadian Ursus maritimus populations Introduction Research suggests that our world today is in the midst of a

More information

EVOLUTIONARILY STABLE STRATEGIES AND GROUP VERSUS INDIVIDUAL SELECTION

EVOLUTIONARILY STABLE STRATEGIES AND GROUP VERSUS INDIVIDUAL SELECTION 39 EVOLUTIONARILY STABLE STRATEGIES AND GROUP VERSUS INDIVIDUAL SELECTION Objectives Understand the concept of game theory. Set up a spreadsheet model of simple game theory interactions. Explore the effects

More information

Risk of predation and the feeding behavior of juvenile eoho salmon (Oncorhynchus kisutch)

Risk of predation and the feeding behavior of juvenile eoho salmon (Oncorhynchus kisutch) Behav Ecol Sociobiol (1984) 16:65-71 Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 9 Springer-Verlag 1984 Risk of predation and the feeding behavior of juvenile eoho salmon (Oncorhynchus kisutch) Lawrence M. Dill*

More information

Parameter Sensitivity In A Lattice Ecosystem With Intraguild Predation

Parameter Sensitivity In A Lattice Ecosystem With Intraguild Predation Parameter Sensitivity In A Lattice Ecosystem With Intraguild Predation N. Nakagiri a, K. Tainaka a, T. Togashi b, T. Miyazaki b and J. Yoshimura a a Department of Systems Engineering, Shizuoka University,

More information

ALTRUISM OR JUST SHOWING OFF?

ALTRUISM OR JUST SHOWING OFF? ALTRUISM OR JUST SHOWING OFF? Soha Sabeti ISCI 330 April 12/07 Altruism or Just Showing Off? Among the many debates regarding the evolution of altruism are suggested theories such as group selection, kin

More information

Chapter 6 Reading Questions

Chapter 6 Reading Questions Chapter 6 Reading Questions 1. Fill in 5 key events in the re-establishment of the New England forest in the Opening Story: 1. Farmers begin leaving 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. Broadleaf forest reestablished 2.

More information

The influence of vigilance on intraguild predation

The influence of vigilance on intraguild predation Journal of Theoretical Biology 249 (2007) 218 234 www.elsevier.com/locate/yjtbi The influence of vigilance on intraguild predation Tristan Kimbrell a,, Robert D. Holt a, Per Lundberg b a Department of

More information

Age (x) nx lx. Population dynamics Population size through time should be predictable N t+1 = N t + B + I - D - E

Age (x) nx lx. Population dynamics Population size through time should be predictable N t+1 = N t + B + I - D - E Population dynamics Population size through time should be predictable N t+1 = N t + B + I - D - E Time 1 N = 100 20 births 25 deaths 10 immigrants 15 emmigrants Time 2 100 + 20 +10 25 15 = 90 Life History

More information

Definition Types of Competition. Theory. Animal How Many Fruit Loops? Green: Red: Orange: Blue: Yellow: Purple: Competition. Let the Games Begin!

Definition Types of Competition. Theory. Animal How Many Fruit Loops? Green: Red: Orange: Blue: Yellow: Purple: Competition. Let the Games Begin! Competition Definition Types of Competition Theory The between organisms or species, in which the (health) of one is lowered by the presences of another. Interspecific competition (over between species)

More information

ANALYSIS OF CHARACTER DIVERGENCE ALONG ENVIRONMENTAL GRADIENTS AND OTHER COVARIATES

ANALYSIS OF CHARACTER DIVERGENCE ALONG ENVIRONMENTAL GRADIENTS AND OTHER COVARIATES ORIGINAL ARTICLE doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2007.00063.x ANALYSIS OF CHARACTER DIVERGENCE ALONG ENVIRONMENTAL GRADIENTS AND OTHER COVARIATES Dean C. Adams 1,2,3 and Michael L. Collyer 1,4 1 Department of

More information

Rank-abundance. Geometric series: found in very communities such as the

Rank-abundance. Geometric series: found in very communities such as the Rank-abundance Geometric series: found in very communities such as the Log series: group of species that occur _ time are the most frequent. Useful for calculating a diversity metric (Fisher s alpha) Most

More information

Letter to the Editor

Letter to the Editor J. theor. Biol. (1998) 195, 413 417 Article No. jt980693 Letter to the Editor A Note on Errors in Grafen s Strategic Handicap Models In two papers published in this Journal, Grafen (1990a,b) has provided

More information

Survival of the Fittest: Fish in Patchy Environments Show IFD

Survival of the Fittest: Fish in Patchy Environments Show IFD Eukaryon Volume 1 Article 6 1-2005 Survival of the Fittest: Fish in Patchy Environments Show IFD Nathan S. Schilling Lake Forest College Follow this and additional works at: http://publications.lakeforest.edu/eukaryon

More information

Joseph Priestly ECOSYSTEMS. Part

Joseph Priestly ECOSYSTEMS. Part ECOSYSTEMS Part 2 Joseph Priestly Joseph Priestly was an English clergyman and scientist. He noticed that if he put a burning candle in a jar, the candle went out after a few minutes and when he put a

More information

Big Idea 1: The process of evolution drives the diversity and unity of life.

Big Idea 1: The process of evolution drives the diversity and unity of life. Big Idea 1: The process of evolution drives the diversity and unity of life. understanding 1.A: Change in the genetic makeup of a population over time is evolution. 1.A.1: Natural selection is a major

More information

ENVE203 Environmental Engineering Ecology (Nov 05, 2012)

ENVE203 Environmental Engineering Ecology (Nov 05, 2012) ENVE203 Environmental Engineering Ecology (Nov 05, 2012) Elif Soyer Ecosystems and Living Organisms Population Density How Do Populations Change in Size? Maximum Population Growth Environmental Resistance

More information

Population Ecology. Study of populations in relation to the environment. Increase population size= endangered species

Population Ecology. Study of populations in relation to the environment. Increase population size= endangered species Population Basics Population Ecology Study of populations in relation to the environment Purpose: Increase population size= endangered species Decrease population size = pests, invasive species Maintain

More information

HOMEWORK ASSIGNMENTS FOR: Grade

HOMEWORK ASSIGNMENTS FOR: Grade HOMEWORK ASSIGNMENTS FOR: Date 4/25/18 Wednesday Teacher Ms. Weger Subject/Grade Science 7 th Grade In-Class: REVIEW FOR CH. 22 TEST Go over the 22-3 Think Questions Look at the data from the Oh Deer!

More information

Predators select against high growth rates and risk-taking behaviour in domestic trout populations

Predators select against high growth rates and risk-taking behaviour in domestic trout populations Predators select against high growth rates and risk-taking behaviour in domestic trout populations Peter A. Biro 1*, Mark V. Abrahams 1, John R. Post 2 and Eric A. Parkinson 3 1 Department of Zoology,

More information

Alana Schick , ISCI 330 Apr. 12, The Evolution of Cooperation: Putting gtheory to the Test

Alana Schick , ISCI 330 Apr. 12, The Evolution of Cooperation: Putting gtheory to the Test Alana Schick 43320027, ISCI 330 Apr. 12, 2007 The Evolution of Cooperation: Putting gtheory to the Test Evolution by natural selection implies that individuals with a better chance of surviving and reproducing

More information

Two Views of Adaptation

Two Views of Adaptation Mar 22: Adaptation--definitions Two Views of Adaptation Adaptation as a process The process by which an organism becomes better fitted to its environment, and stays that way Reflects role of natural selection

More information

14.1. KEY CONCEPT Every organism has a habitat and a niche. 38 Reinforcement Unit 5 Resource Book

14.1. KEY CONCEPT Every organism has a habitat and a niche. 38 Reinforcement Unit 5 Resource Book 14.1 HABITAT AND NICHE KEY CONCEPT Every organism has a habitat and a niche. A habitat is all of the living and nonliving factors in the area where an organism lives. For example, the habitat of a frog

More information

Computation of Efficient Nash Equilibria for experimental economic games

Computation of Efficient Nash Equilibria for experimental economic games International Journal of Mathematics and Soft Computing Vol.5, No.2 (2015), 197-212. ISSN Print : 2249-3328 ISSN Online: 2319-5215 Computation of Efficient Nash Equilibria for experimental economic games

More information

Environmental Influences on Adaptation

Environmental Influences on Adaptation Have you ever noticed how the way you feel sometimes mirrors the emotions of the people with whom you spend a lot of time? For example, when you re around happy people, do you tend to become happy? Since

More information

Structure Learning of a Behavior Network for Context Dependent Adaptability

Structure Learning of a Behavior Network for Context Dependent Adaptability Georgia State University ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University Computer Science Theses Department of Computer Science 12-7-26 Structure Learning of a Behavior Network for Context Dependent Adaptability

More information

WTHS Biology Keystone Exams

WTHS Biology Keystone Exams WTHS Biology Keystone Exams Biology Keystone Review Packet 10 th / 11 th Grade Keystone Test Prep This packet contains helpful information for you to prepare for the upcoming Biology Keystone Test on May

More information

Lab #6: Predator Prey Interactions

Lab #6: Predator Prey Interactions Lab #6: Predator Interactions This exercise illustrates how different populations interact within a community, and how this interaction can influence the process of evolution in both species. The relationship

More information

MCDB 1111 corebio 2017 Midterm I

MCDB 1111 corebio 2017 Midterm I MCDB 1111 corebio 2017 Midterm I Name: There are 18 questions, each worth a maximum of 6 points. Multiple choice questions are worth 3 points, while explanations are worth 3 points. If you need to, use

More information

Chapter 4 Ecosystems and Living Organisms

Chapter 4 Ecosystems and Living Organisms Chapter 4 Ecosystems and Living Organisms I. Evolution A. The cumulative genetic changes that occur in a population of organisms over time 1. Current theories proposed by Charles Darwin, a 19 th century

More information

BIOS 5970: Plant-Herbivore Interactions Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences

BIOS 5970: Plant-Herbivore Interactions Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences BIOS 5970: Plant-Herbivore Interactions Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences Week 5. Plant defense theory 2: Development: Lecture summary: Resource Availability Theory: Coley, Bryant

More information

Complex Systems Theory and Evolution

Complex Systems Theory and Evolution Complex Systems Theory and Evolution Melanie Mitchell and Mark Newman Santa Fe Institute, 1399 Hyde Park Road, Santa Fe, NM 87501 In Encyclopedia of Evolution (M. Pagel, editor), New York: Oxford University

More information

Hamilton s rule meets the Hamiltonian: kin selection on dynamic characters

Hamilton s rule meets the Hamiltonian: kin selection on dynamic characters Hamilton s rule meets the Hamiltonian: kin selection on dynamic characters TROY DAY and PETER D. TAYLOR Department of Mathematics and Statistics, Queen s University, Kingston, ON, Canada K7L 3N6 (4tmcd@jeff-lab.queensu.ca,

More information

Lecture 8 Insect ecology and balance of life

Lecture 8 Insect ecology and balance of life Lecture 8 Insect ecology and balance of life Ecology: The term ecology is derived from the Greek term oikos meaning house combined with logy meaning the science of or the study of. Thus literally ecology

More information