Event-related fmri. Christian Ruff. Laboratory for Social and Neural Systems Research Department of Economics University of Zurich

Similar documents
FIL. Event-related. fmri. Rik Henson. With thanks to: Karl Friston, Oliver Josephs

1st level analysis Basis functions, parametric modulation and correlated regressors

Experimental Design. Rik Henson. With thanks to: Karl Friston, Andrew Holmes

Overview. Experimental Design. A categorical analysis. A taxonomy of design. A taxonomy of design. A taxonomy of design. 1. A Taxonomy of Designs

The General Linear Model (GLM)

Analysis of fmri Timeseries:

The General Linear Model (GLM)

The General Linear Model. Guillaume Flandin Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging University College London

Contents. design. Experimental design Introduction & recap Experimental design «Take home» message. N εˆ. DISCOS SPM course, CRC, Liège, 2009

Extracting fmri features

Data Analysis I: Single Subject

Contents. Introduction The General Linear Model. General Linear Linear Model Model. The General Linear Model, Part I. «Take home» message

Jean-Baptiste Poline

Optimization of Designs for fmri

Experimental design of fmri studies

Experimental design of fmri studies

Overview of SPM. Overview. Making the group inferences we want. Non-sphericity Beyond Ordinary Least Squares. Model estimation A word on power

Experimental design of fmri studies & Resting-State fmri

Statistical Analysis of fmrl Data

Statistical Inference

Statistical Inference

Experimental design of fmri studies

Experimental design of fmri studies

Modelling temporal structure (in noise and signal)

Homework #2 Due date: 2/19/2013 (Tuesday) Translate the slice Two Main Paths in Lecture Part II: Neurophysiology and BOLD to words.

Group Analysis. Lexicon. Hierarchical models Mixed effect models Random effect (RFX) models Components of variance

Group analysis. Jean Daunizeau Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging University College London. SPM Course Edinburgh, April 2010

Causal modeling of fmri: temporal precedence and spatial exploration

Bayesian Estimation of Dynamical Systems: An Application to fmri

LINEAR SYSTEMS. J. Elder PSYC 6256 Principles of Neural Coding

Detecting fmri activation allowing for unknown latency of the hemodynamic response

Signal Processing for Functional Brain Imaging: General Linear Model (2)

What is NIRS? First-Level Statistical Models 5/18/18

NeuroImage. Modeling the hemodynamic response function in fmri: Efficiency, bias and mis-modeling

Therefore the new Fourier coefficients are. Module 2 : Signals in Frequency Domain Problem Set 2. Problem 1

Effective Connectivity & Dynamic Causal Modelling

Neuroimaging for Machine Learners Validation and inference

The General Linear Model Ivo Dinov

Contrasts and Classical Inference

General linear model: basic

Mixed effects and Group Modeling for fmri data

HST 583 FUNCTIONAL MAGNETIC RESONANCE IMAGING DATA ANALYSIS AND ACQUISITION A REVIEW OF STATISTICS FOR FMRI DATA ANALYSIS

MIXED EFFECTS MODELS FOR TIME SERIES

Robust regression and non-linear kernel methods for characterization of neuronal response functions from limited data

Bayesian modelling of fmri time series

Overview of Spatial Statistics with Applications to fmri

Dynamic Causal Modelling for fmri

Searching for Nested Oscillations in Frequency and Sensor Space. Will Penny. Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging. University College London.

The homogeneous Poisson process

Neural Encoding: Firing Rates and Spike Statistics

Piotr Majer Risk Patterns and Correlated Brain Activities

A Multivariate Time-Frequency Based Phase Synchrony Measure for Quantifying Functional Connectivity in the Brain

The general linear model and Statistical Parametric Mapping I: Introduction to the GLM

Contents. Data. Introduction & recap Variance components Hierarchical model RFX and summary statistics Variance/covariance matrix «Take home» message

Introduction to Biomedical Engineering

Bayesian inference J. Daunizeau

Basic MRI physics and Functional MRI

3. ESTIMATION OF SIGNALS USING A LEAST SQUARES TECHNIQUE

Estimating representational dissimilarity measures

An introduction to Bayesian inference and model comparison J. Daunizeau

Statistical parametric mapping for event-related potentials (II): a hierarchical temporal model

Describing Spike-Trains

Beyond Univariate Analyses: Multivariate Modeling of Functional Neuroimaging Data

Recipes for the Linear Analysis of EEG and applications

Bayesian Treatments of. Neuroimaging Data Will Penny and Karl Friston. 5.1 Introduction

Statistical inference for MEG

Bayesian inference J. Daunizeau

Dynamic Causal Models

Chapter 1 Fundamental Concepts

Comparing integrate-and-fire models estimated using intracellular and extracellular data 1

On Signal to Noise Ratio Tradeoffs in fmri

Semi-supervised subspace analysis of human functional magnetic resonance imaging data

The Discrete-time Fourier Transform

Full Brain Blood-Oxygen-Level-Dependent Signal Parameter Estimation Using Particle Filters

Σ S(x )δ x. x. Σ S(x )δ x ) x Σ F(S(x )δ x ), by superposition x Σ S(x )F(δ x ), by homogeneity x Σ S(x )I x. x. Σ S(x ) I x (y ) = x

2A1H Time-Frequency Analysis II

Commentary on the statistical properties of noise and its implication on general linear models in functional near-infrared spectroscopy

Comparison of stochastic and variational solutions to ASL fmri data analysis

Communication Signals (Haykin Sec. 2.4 and Ziemer Sec Sec. 2.4) KECE321 Communication Systems I

DETECTION OF VISUAL EVOKED POTENTIALS USING RAMANUJAN PERIODICITY TRANSFORM FOR REAL TIME BRAIN COMPUTER INTERFACES

Introduction to functional MRI in humans. Michael Hallquist University of Pittsburgh

22/04/2014. Economic Research

Location Prediction of Moving Target

Gaussian with mean ( µ ) and standard deviation ( σ)

Timbral, Scale, Pitch modifications

An Implementation of Dynamic Causal Modelling

Fourier Methods in Digital Signal Processing Final Exam ME 579, Spring 2015 NAME

Mid Year Project Report: Statistical models of visual neurons

What is the neural code? Sekuler lab, Brandeis

Signal Modeling Techniques in Speech Recognition. Hassan A. Kingravi

ESS Finite Impulse Response Filters and the Z-transform

Consider the following spike trains from two different neurons N1 and N2:

Latent state estimation using control theory

System Identification of Adapting Neurons

First Technical Course, European Centre for Soft Computing, Mieres, Spain. 4th July 2011

FUNCTIONAL CONNECTIVITY ANALYSIS OF FMRI TIME-SERIES DATA

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn.2283

Practical Spectral Estimation

Detection Power, Estimation Efficiency, and Predictability in Event-Related fmri

2. SPECTRAL ANALYSIS APPLIED TO STOCHASTIC PROCESSES

Transcription:

Event-related fmri Christian Ruff Laboratory for Social and Neural Systems Research Department of Economics University of Zurich Institute of Neurology University College London With thanks to the FIL methods group, in particular Rik Henson

Image time-series Kernel Design matrix Statistical parametric map (SPM) Realignment Smoothing General linear model Normalisation Statistical inference Gaussian field theory Template p <0.05 Parameter estimates

Overview 1. Block/epoch vs. event-related fmri 2. (Dis)Advantages of efmri 3. GLM: Convolution 4. BOLD impulse response 5. Temporal Basis Functions 6. Timing Issues 7. Design Optimisation Efficiency

Block/epoch designs vs event-related designs Block/epoch designs examine responses to series of similar stimuli U1 U2 U3 P1 P2 P3 P = Pleasant U = Unpleasant Event-related designs account for response to each single stimulus U1 P1 U2 U3 P2 ~4s Data Model

Advantages of event-related fmri 1. Randomised trial order

efmri: Randomised trial order Blocked designs may trigger expectations and cognitive sets Unpleasant (U) Pleasant (P) Intermixed designs can minimise this by stimulus randomisation Pleasant (P) Unpleasant (U) Unpleasant (U) Pleasant (P) Unpleasant (U)

Advantages of event-related fmri 1. Randomised trials order 2. Post-hoc subjective classification of trials

efmri: Post-hoc classification of trials Participant response: was not shown as picture was shown as picture Items with wrong memory of picture ( hat ) were associated with more occipital activity at encoding than items with correct rejection ( brain ) Gonsalves & Paller (2000) Nature Neuroscience

Advantages of event-related fmri 1. Randomised trials order 2. Post-hoc subjective classification of trials 3. Some events can only be indicated by participant

efmri: Online event definition

Advantages of event-related fmri 1. Randomised trials order 2. Post-hoc subjective classification of trials 3. Some events can only be indicated by participant 4. Some events cannot be blocked due to stimulus context

efmri: Stimulus context Oddball time

Advantages of event-related fmri 1. Randomised trials order 2. Post-hoc subjective classification of trials 3. Some events can only be indicated by participant 4. Some events cannot be blocked due to stimulus context 5. More accurate model even for epoch/block designs?

Event model of block design Epoch model assumes constant neural processes throughout block U1 U2 U3 P1 P2 P3 Event model may capture state-item interactions (with longer SOAs) P = Pleasant U = Unpleasant U1 U2 U3 P1 P2 P3 Data Model

Modeling block designs: Epochs vs events Designs can be blocked or intermixed, BUT models for blocked designs can be epoch- or event-related Epochs are periods of sustained stimulation (e.g, box-car functions) Events are impulses (delta-functions) Near-identical regressors can be created by 1) sustained epochs, 2) rapid series of events (SOAs<~3s) In SPM8, all conditions are specified in terms of their 1) onsets and 2) durations epochs: variable or constant duration events: zero duration Sustained epoch Series of events => Classic Boxcar function Delta functions Convolved with HRF

Modeling block designs: Epochs vs events Blocks of trials can be modeled as boxcars or runs of events Rate = 1/4s Rate = 1/2s β=3 β=5 BUT: interpretation of the parameter estimates may differ Consider an experiment presenting words at different rates in different blocks: An epoch model will estimate parameter that increases with rate, because the parameter reflects response per block An event model may estimate parameter that decreases with rate, because the parameter reflects response per word β=11 β=9

Disadvatages of intermixed designs 1. Less efficient for detecting effects than blocked designs (see later ) 2. Some psychological processes have to/may be better blocked (e.g., if difficult to switch between states, or to reduce surprise effects)

Overview 1. Block/epoch vs. event-related fmri 2. (Dis)Advantages of efmri 3. GLM: Convolution 4. BOLD impulse response 5. Temporal Basis Functions 6. Timing Issues 7. Design Optimisation Efficiency

BOLD impulse response Function of blood oxygenation, flow, volume Peak Peak (max. oxygenation) 4-6s poststimulus; baseline after 20-30s Initial undershoot can be observed Similar across V1, A1, S1 but possible differences across: - other regions - individuals Brief Stimulus Initial Undershoot Undershoot

BOLD impulse response Early event-related fmri studies used a long Stimulus Onset Asynchrony (SOA) to allow BOLD response to return to baseline However, overlap between successive responses at short SOAs can be accommodated if the BOLD response is explicitly modeled, particularly if responses are assumed to superpose linearly Short SOAs are more sensitive; see later Brief Stimulus Initial Undershoot Peak Undershoot

General Linear (Convolution) Model GLM for a single voxel: y(t) = u(t) h(τ) + ε(t) u(t) = neural causes (stimulus train) u(t) = δ (t - nt) h(τ) = hemodynamic (BOLD) response u(t) T 2T 3T... convolution h(τ)= ß i f i (τ) h(τ) = ß i f i (τ) sampled each scan f i (τ) = temporal basis functions y(t) = ß i f i (t - nt) + ε(t) y = X ß + ε Design Matrix

General Linear Model in SPM Stimulus every 20s Gamma functions ƒ i (τ) of peristimulus time τ (Orthogonalised) SPM{F} Sampled every TR = 1.7s Design matrix, X [x(t) ƒ 1 (τ) x(t) ƒ 2 (τ)...] 0 time {secs} 30

Overview 1. Block/epoch vs. event-related fmri 2. (Dis)Advantages of efmri 3. GLM: Convolution 4. BOLD impulse response 5. Temporal Basis Functions 6. Timing Issues 7. Design Optimisation Efficiency

Temporal basis functions

Temporal basis functions Fourier Set - Windowed sines & cosines - Any shape (up to frequency limit) - Inference via F-test Finite Impulse Response - Mini timebins (selective averaging) - Any shape (up to bin-width) - Inference via F-test

Temporal basis functions Fourier Set / FIR - Any shape (up to frequency limit / bin width) - Inference via F-test Gamma Functions - Bounded, asymmetrical (like BOLD) - Set of different lags - Inference via F-test Informed Basis Set - Best guess of canonical BOLD response - Variability captured by Taylor expansion - Magnitude inferences via t-test?

Informed basis set Canonical Canonical HRF (2 gamma functions)

Informed basis set Canonical Temporal Canonical HRF (2 gamma functions) plus Multivariate Taylor expansion in: - time (Temporal Derivative)

Informed basis set Canonical Temporal Canonical HRF (2 gamma functions) plus Multivariate Taylor expansion in: - time (Temporal Derivative)

Informed basis set Canonical Temporal Dispersion Canonical HRF (2 gamma functions) plus Multivariate Taylor expansion in: - time (Temporal Derivative) - width (Dispersion Derivative)

Informed basis set Canonical Temporal Dispersion Canonical HRF (2 gamma functions) plus Multivariate Taylor expansion in: - time (Temporal Derivative) - width (Dispersion Derivative)

Informed basis set Canonical Temporal Dispersion Canonical HRF (2 gamma functions) plus Multivariate Taylor expansion in: - time (Temporal Derivative) - width (Dispersion Derivative) Magnitude inferences via t-test on canonical parameters (providing canonical is a reasonable fit) Latency inferences via tests on ratio of derivative : canonical parameters

Which temporal basis set? In this example (rapid motor response to faces, Henson et al, 2001) Canonical + Temporal + Dispersion + FIR canonical + temporal + dispersion derivatives appear sufficient to capture most activity may not be true for more complex trials (e.g. stimulus-prolonged delay (>~2 s)-response) but then such trials better modelled with separate neural components (i.e., activity no longer delta function) + constrained HRF

Overview 1. Block/epoch vs. event-related fmri 2. (Dis)Advantages of efmri 3. GLM: Convolution 4. BOLD impulse response 5. Temporal Basis Functions 6. Timing Issues 7. Design Optimisation Efficiency

Timing issues: Sampling Typical TR for 60 slice EPI at 3 mm spacing is ~ 3-4s Scans TR=4s

Timing issues: Sampling Typical TR for 60 slice EPI at 3 mm spacing is ~ 3-4s Scans TR=4s Sampling at [0,4,8,12 ] post- stimulus may miss peak signal Stimulus (synchronous) SOA=8s Sampling rate=4s

Timing issues: Sampling Typical TR for 60 slice EPI at 3 mm spacing is ~ 3-4s Scans TR=4s Sampling at [0,4,8,12 ] post- stimulus may miss peak signal Stimulus (random jitter) Sampling rate=2s Higher effective sampling by: 1. Asynchrony; e.g., SOA=1.5TR 2. Random Jitter; e.g., SOA=(2±0.5)TR Better response characterisation

Timing issues: Slice Timing T1 = 0 s T16 = 2 s T=16, TR=2s T 0 =9 T 0 =16 o o x 2 x 3 0 Scan 1

Timing issues: Slice Timing Slice-timing Problem : Top Slice Bottom Slice Slices acquired at different times, yet model is the same for all slices TR=3s different results (using canonical HRF) for different reference slices (slightly less problematic if middle slice is selected as reference, and with short TRs) SPM{t} Interpolated SPM{t} Solutions: 1. Temporal interpolation of data but less good for longer TRs Derivative SPM{t} 2. More general basis set (e.g., with temporal derivatives) but inferences via F-test SPM{F}

Overview 1. Block/epoch vs. event-related fmri 2. (Dis)Advantages of efmri 3. GLM: Convolution 4. BOLD impulse response 5. Temporal Basis Functions 6. Timing Issues 7. Design Optimisation Efficiency

Design efficiency HRF can be viewed as a filter (Josephs & Henson, 1999) We want to maximise the signal passed by this filter Dominant frequency of canonical HRF is ~0.04 Hz The most efficient design is a sinusoidal modulation of neural activity with period ~24s (e.g., boxcar with 12s on/ 12s off)

Sinusoidal modulation, f = 1/33 Stimulus ( Neural ) HRF Predicted Data = = A very efficient design!

Blocked, epoch = 20 sec Stimulus ( Neural ) HRF Predicted Data = = Blocked-epoch (with small SOA) quite efficient

Blocked (80s), SOAmin=4s, highpass filter = 1/120s Stimulus ( Neural ) HRF Predicted Data = Effective HRF (after highpass filtering) (Josephs & Henson, 1999) = Very ineffective: Don t have long (>60s) blocks!

Randomised, SOAmin=4s, highpass filter = 1/120s Stimulus ( Neural ) HRF Predicted Data = = Randomised design spreads power over frequencies

Design efficiency T-statistic for a given contrast: T = c T b / var(c T b) For maximum T, we want maximum precision and hence minimum standard error of contrast estimates (var(c T b)) Var(c T b) = sqrt(σ 2 c T (X T X) -1 c) (i.i.d) If we assume that noise variance (σ 2 ) is unaffected by changes in X, then our precision for given parameters is proportional to the design efficiency: e(c,x) = { c T (X T X) -1 c } -1 We can influence e (a priori) by the spacing and sequencing of epochs/events in our design matrix e is specific for a given contrast!

Design efficiency: Trial spacing Design parametrised by: - SOA min Minimum SOA - p(t) Probability of event at each SOA min Deterministic p(t)=1 iff t=nsoamin Stationary stochastic p(t)=constant Dynamic stochastic p(t) varies (e.g., blocked) Blocked designs most efficient! (with small SOAmin)

Design efficiency: Trial spacing e 90.0 However, block designs are often not advisable due to interpretative difficulties (see before) Event trains may then be constructed by modulating the event probabilities in a dynamic stochastic fashion 67.5 45.0 22.5 0 Block Dyn stoch Randomised This can result in intermediate levels of efficiency 3 sessions with 128 scans Faces, scrambled faces SOA always 2.97 s Cycle length 24 s

Design efficiency: Trial sequencing Design parametrised by: SOA min Minimum SOA p i (h) Probability of event-type i given history h of last m events With n event-types p i (h) is a n x n Transition Matrix Differential Effect (A-B) Common Effect (A+B) Example: Randomised AB A B A 0.5 0.5 B 0.5 0.5 => ABBBABAABABAAA...

Design efficiency: Trial sequencing Example: Alternating AB A B A 0 1 B 1 0 => ABABABABABAB... Permuted (A-B) Alternating (A-B) Example: Permuted AB A B AA 0 1 AB 0.5 0.5 BA 0.5 0.5 BB 1 0 => ABBAABABABBA...

Design efficiency: Trial sequencing Example: Null events A B A 0.33 0.33 B 0.33 0.33 => AB-BAA--B---ABB... Null Events (A-B) Null Events (A+B) Efficient for differential and main effects at short SOA Equivalent to stochastic SOA (Null Event like third unmodelled event-type)

Design efficiency: Conclusions Optimal design for one contrast may not be optimal for another Blocked designs generally most efficient (with short SOAs, given optimal block length is not exceeded) However, psychological efficiency often dictates intermixed designs, and often also sets limits on SOAs With randomised designs, optimal SOA for differential effect (A-B) is minimal SOA (>2 seconds, and assuming no saturation), whereas optimal SOA for main effect (A+B) is 16-20s Inclusion of null events improves efficiency for main effect at short SOAs (at cost of efficiency for differential effects) If order constrained, intermediate SOAs (5-20s) can be optimal If SOA constrained, pseudorandomised designs can be optimal (but may introduce context-sensitivity)