SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL. Authors: Alan A. Stocker (1) and Eero P. Simoncelli (2)

Similar documents
Residual migration in VTI media using anisotropy continuation

Chapter 2: 1D Kinematics Tuesday January 13th

Astrometric Errors Correlated Strongly Across Multiple SIRTF Images

Chapter 1 Solutions Engineering and Chemical Thermodynamics 2e Wyatt Tenhaeff Milo Koretsky

UNDERSTAND MOTION IN ONE AND TWO DIMENSIONS

Probabilistic Engineering Design

A Geometric Review of Linear Algebra

Asymptotic Normality of an Entropy Estimator with Exponentially Decaying Bias

Classical Mechanics NEWTONIAN SYSTEM OF PARTICLES MISN NEWTONIAN SYSTEM OF PARTICLES by C. P. Frahm

On the Linear Threshold Model for Diffusion of Innovations in Multiplex Social Networks

Online Companion to Pricing Services Subject to Congestion: Charge Per-Use Fees or Sell Subscriptions?

[Abdallah, 5(1): January 2018] ISSN DOI /zenodo Impact Factor

A possible mechanism to explain wave-particle duality L D HOWE No current affiliation PACS Numbers: r, w, k

The International Association for the Properties of Water and Steam

A Geometric Review of Linear Algebra

Dynamic Vehicle Routing with Moving Demands Part II: High speed demands or low arrival rates

The International Association for the Properties of Water and Steam

Min Chen Department of Mathematics Purdue University 150 N. University Street

6.3 Vectors in a Plane

DEVIL PHYSICS THE BADDEST CLASS ON CAMPUS AP PHYSICS

The International Association for the Properties of Water and Steam

Functional form of motion priors in human motion perception

Optimal Joint Detection and Estimation in Linear Models

(a) Taking the derivative of the position vector with respect to time, we have, in SI units (m/s),

Estimation of Efficiency with the Stochastic Frontier Cost. Function and Heteroscedasticity: A Monte Carlo Study

Supplementary Information Microfluidic quadrupole and floating concentration gradient Mohammad A. Qasaimeh, Thomas Gervais, and David Juncker

Interpreting enzyme and receptor kinetics: keeping it simple, but not too simple 1

Sparse Functional Regression

Real Gas Thermodynamics. and the isentropic behavior of substances. P. Nederstigt

Dynamics ( 동역학 ) Ch.2 Motion of Translating Bodies (2.1 & 2.2)

Purpose of the experiment

A014 Uncertainty Analysis of Velocity to Resistivity Transforms for Near Field Exploration

4. A Physical Model for an Electron with Angular Momentum. An Electron in a Bohr Orbit. The Quantum Magnet Resulting from Orbital Motion.

ERAD THE SEVENTH EUROPEAN CONFERENCE ON RADAR IN METEOROLOGY AND HYDROLOGY

VISUAL PHYSICS ONLINE RECTLINEAR MOTION: UNIFORM ACCELERATION

Physics 111. Help sessions meet Sunday, 6:30-7:30 pm in CLIR Wednesday, 8-9 pm in NSC 098/099

Lateral organization & computation

Noise constrained least mean absolute third algorithm

Evolution Analysis of Iterative LMMSE-APP Detection for Coded Linear System with Cyclic Prefixes

A one-dimensional analytical calculation method for obtaining normal shock losses in supersonic real gas flows

Trajectory Estimation for Tactical Ballistic Missiles in Terminal Phase Using On-line Input Estimator

arxiv: v1 [stat.ml] 15 Feb 2018

Kinematics on oblique axes

Introduction to Thermodynamic Cycles Part 1 1 st Law of Thermodynamics and Gas Power Cycles

An Optimal Split-Plot Design for Performing a Mixture-Process Experiment

Relativistic Energy Derivation

SLIP MODEL PERFORMANCE FOR MICRO-SCALE GAS FLOWS

A matrix Method for Interval Hermite Curve Segmentation O. Ismail, Senior Member, IEEE

Representational Power of Restricted Boltzmann Machines and Deep Belief Networks

0 a 3 a 2 a 3 0 a 1 a 2 a 1 0

d dt T R m n p 1. (A) 4. (4) Carnot engine T Refrigerating effect W COPref. = 1 4 kw 5. (A)

ME224 Lab 5 - Thermal Diffusion

Сollisionless damping of electron waves in non-maxwellian plasma 1

Magnetic Fields Part 3: Electromagnetic Induction

Assignment 4 (Solutions) NPTEL MOOC (Bayesian/ MMSE Estimation for MIMO/OFDM Wireless Communications)

Brake applications and the remaining velocity Hans Humenberger University of Vienna, Faculty of mathematics

P(c y) = W n (y c)p(c)

Section 6: PRISMATIC BEAMS. Beam Theory

On computing Gaussian curvature of some well known distribution

Status: Unit 2, Chapter 3

EXPERIMENT 8 BALLISTIC PENDULUM. Figure 1 Setup to determine the initial speed of the projectile using the Blackwood Pendulum

Algebra Based Physics. Motion in One Dimension. 1D Kinematics Graphing Free Fall 2016.notebook. August 30, Table of Contents: Kinematics

4 Fundamentals of Continuum Thermomechanics

4-vectors. Chapter Definition of 4-vectors

Adaptive Velocity Tuning for Visual Motion Estimation

Supplemental Data: Appendix

Dynamic Vehicle Routing with Moving Demands Part II: High speed demands or low arrival rates

Optimized Concatenated LDPC Codes for Joint Source-Channel Coding

AN APPLICATION OF THE DOUBLE SUMUDU TRANSFORM

A Small-Signal Analysis of a BJT

v v Downloaded 01/11/16 to Redistribution subject to SEG license or copyright; see Terms of Use at

Math 144 Activity #9 Introduction to Vectors

Lect-19. In this lecture...

Chapter 5 Solution P5.2-2, 3, 6 P5.3-3, 5, 8, 15 P5.4-3, 6, 8, 16 P5.5-2, 4, 6, 11 P5.6-2, 4, 9

Motion in Two and Three Dimensions

Fu Yuhua 1. Beijing, China

Lecture 21: Physical Brownian Motion II

arxiv: v1 [physics.comp-ph] 17 Jan 2014

Chapter 14 Thermal Physics: A Microscopic View

Stresses in Sedimentary Strata, including Coals, and the Effects of Fluid Withdrawal on Effective Stress and Permeability

Motion in Two and Three Dimensions

CRITICAL MASS FLOW RATE THROUGH CAPILLARY TUBES

Fractional-RWHet: An enhanced solver for solute transport with both spatiotemporal memory and conditioning on local aquifer properties

DO PHYSICS ONLINE. WEB activity: Use the web to find out more about: Aristotle, Copernicus, Kepler, Galileo and Newton.

The Kinetic Theory of Gases

Damage detection in a sandwich composite beam using wavelet transforms

Nonexistence of Plane Symmetric Micro Model in Presence of Cosmological Constant in Barber s Modified Theory of General Relativity

Cases of integrability corresponding to the motion of a pendulum on the two-dimensional plane

Distribution of the Ratio of Normal and Rice Random Variables

(a) During the first part of the motion, the displacement is x 1 = 40 km and the time interval is t 1 (30 km / h) (80 km) 40 km/h. t. (2.

Note: the net distance along the path is a scalar quantity its direction is not important so the average speed is also a scalar.

Mathisson s New Mechanics : Its Aims and Realisation. W G Dixon, Churchill College, Cambridge, England

On the Nature of Coherent Turbulent Structures in Channel Bends: Burst-Sweep Orientations in Three-Dimensional Flow Fields

arxiv: v1 [cond-mat.stat-mech] 5 Oct 2018

Transmission lines using a distributed equivalent circuit

Measuring Crowd Collectiveness

MANAGEMENT SCIENCE doi /mnsc ec pp. ec1 ec31

Bayesian Multiview Manifold Learning Applied to Hippocampus Shape and Clinical Score Data

A Regularization Framework for Learning from Graph Data

Unified Architecture Level Energy-Efficiency Metric

Transcription:

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL Authors: Alan A. Stocker () and Eero P. Simoncelli () Affiliations: () Dept. of Psychology, Uniersity of Pennsylania 34 Walnut Street 33C Philadelphia, PA 94-68 U.S.A. () Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Center for Neural Science and Courant Institute of Mathematical Sciences New York Uniersity 4 Washington Place Rm 89 New York, NY 3-56 U.S.A. Correspondence: Alan A. Stocker email: astocerk@sas.upenn.edu phone: + 5 573 934 Journal: Journal of Vision Classification: Biological Sciences (Psychology/Neuroscience) Statistics: 8 pages, 5 figures, tables Philadelphia, May 3, 9

Supplementary Material Extended Signature of Adaptation The concept of a signature of adaptation can be extended to include changes in ariability of the perceptual estimate. More specifically, if we assume a gain-change model as the underlying mechanism for adaptation, and assume an appropriate noise model for the channel responses, we can predict what the characteristic change in estimation ariability should be. For example, assuming the channels responses for a gien stimulus to follow a Poisson distribution, we can demonstrate with simulations that the relatie change in ariability of the estimate is symmetric about the adaptor, typically increased at the adaptor and decreased at adjacent alues (see also []). Again, since the details are not important in the context of this paper, we use a simple, single-parameter function (second deriatie of a Gaussian) that captures the main characteristics of the relatie change in ariability. Figure shows the full signature of adaptation, for the combined changes in the mean and ariability of perceptual estimates, here expressed as the difference in means (µ () µ()) and the relatie change in standard deiation of the estimation distributions (log σ ()/σ()). a response a + Poisson noise bias + - change in ariability a b Fig.. Extended signature of adaptation. (a) The gain-change model mechanism can be extended by considering response ariability []. (b) Decoding channel actiity assuming Poisson noise statistics leads to an perceptual signature in bias and in change in estimation ariability. The signature for ariability (here expressed as the relatie change of standard deiations log σ ()/σ()) is symmetric around the adaptor, is increased at the adaptor and decreased for stimuli that are further away in perceptual space.

3 ^ a adapted µ ( ) thres! ( thres) ^ reference ^ µ *( ) t!*( ) t t thres pse bias Fig.. Signal detection theory and the relationship between perception and estimation. The two gray-scale plots illustrate the distributions of a subject s estimate ˆ() under adaptation and reference conditions oer a broad range of stimulus elocities. Presumably, adaptation changes the mean µ () as well as the standard deiation σ () of these distributions as a function of the adaptor a and the stimulus elocity. These changes in the subjects estimation of elocity are then expressed as changes in the point-of-subjectie-equality pse and discrimination threshold thres. Signal detection theory links these perceptual measures to estimator bias (µ() µ()) and relatie change in ariability (log σ ()/σ()). Linking estimation ariability to perceptual AFC data While the estimation bias can be directly measured from the shift of the psychometric function under adapted ersus control conditions, the changes in estimation ariability are only indirectly reflected in changes in perceptual discriminability as measured by the slope of the psychometric function. This relationship is further complicated by our single-sided AFC adaptation protocol (see Methods in the main text). Alternatiely, discriminability could hae been extracted directly from a separate two-sided adaptation experiment in which both, reference and test location, were adapted. Howeer, this would hae resulted in doubling the length of the already extensie experiment because the single-sided adaptation condition is still needed to compute the bias. In the following, we use signal detection theory [3], [4] to extract the changes in ariability from the subjects psychometric functions. We assume that when performing a AFC motion discrimination task between a reference and a test stimulus with elocities r and t, subjects perform independent estimates ˆ( r ) and ˆ( t ) of each stimulus elocity, and then take a decision accordingly []. Due to noise, these estimates will fluctuate oer repeated trials, leading to distributions of estimates. By approximating the distributions of estimates

4 with Gaussians of mean µ() and standard deiation σ(), we can define the perceptual discriminability between a reference and a test stimulus as d ( r, t )= µ( r ) µ( t ) ( σ ( r )+σ ( t ) ). () In general, the estimator for the test and the reference stimulus are different (see Fig.). In our single-sided adaptation experiment, the estimator of the reference stimulus was always in the unadapted state, whereas the estimator of the test was in one of the four adaptation conditions or the control condition. For each t under each adaptation condition we fit the subjects psychometric function with a cumulatie Gaussian, and then extracted two alues of r from the fit that correspond to two particular alues of d : the point-of-subjectie-equality pse ( t ) for which d =, and the discrimination threshold elocity thres ( t ) that is associated with a certain (arbitrarily chosen) cumulatie probability α on the psychometric cure (here, we use α =.875, with an associated alue of d α =.67). This is illustrated graphically in Fig.. For each adaptation condition (fully adapted, control adapted, unadapted), Eq. () may be used to define these two special alues of r through implicit equations. For example, the alues pse( t ) and thres ( t) for the fully adapted condition are determined by = µ ( pse( t )) µ ( t ) () d α = µ (thres ( t)) µ ( t ) (, (3) (σ (thres ( t))) +(σ ( t )) ) for each test elocity t under each condition, where the superscripts {, } indicate the adaptation state (fully adapted, and unadapted, respectiely). In order to sole for µ ( t ) and σ ( t ), we make the following assumptions: (i) Without loss of generality, we assume that the estimator for the reference stimulus is eridical, i.e. µ ( r )= r. (ii) Furthermore, we also assume that the ariability of the estimator changes smoothly, i.e. it remains roughly constant oer the elocity range of the discrimination threshold. Under assumption (i), Eq.() implies that µ ( t ) = pse ( t). Thus, we can extract the estimation bias directly as the difference in the measured point-of-subjectie-equality under full adaptation and control adaptation conditions: µ ( t ) µ c ( t )= pse ( t) c pse ( t). We can sole for the estimation ariability, σ ( t ), by substituting pse( t ) for µ ( t ) in Eq.(3) and rewriting as σ ( t )= d ( thres ( t) pse( t ) ) ( σ (thres ( t)) ). (4) α

5 a= a=.8 a= 4. a = 8.5 [deg/s] change in ariability log(! " /! c ) 4 - -4 4 - -4 subject subject 4 - -4 subject 3 - -5-5 - -5-5 - -5-5 - -5-5 test motion [deg/s] t Fig. 3. Changes in estimation ariability due to adaptation. Plotted points indicate the adaptation-induced changes in estimation ariability under all adaptation conditions and all three subjects. Note, that they are not equialent to, but rather extracted from the measured perceptual discrimination thresholds using the analysis as discussed in the text. Changes are shown as the log-ratio of the standard deiations in adaptation and control conditions. As in the case of bias, the change in ariability typically increases in amplitude with increasing adaptor speeds. Bold red lines represent fits according to our model. Gray shaded area indicates the 5-95 % quantile oer bootstrap samples of the data. An analogous expression may be deried for the control condition (superscript {c}), and the two may be combined to compute the relatie change in estimation ariability log (σ ( t )/σ c ( t )). A second control experiment measured discrimination thresholds under unadapted conditions, and proided the estimates of σ () that are required in Eq. (4). We assume the unadapted ariability at the test location to be approximately equal to that at the reference location when ealuated at the threshold elocity: σ ( thres ( t)) σ ( t ) for all t. Substituting this into Eq. (), and again assuming that µ () = gies σ ( t ) thres ( t) t d. (5) α The alues of σ () required in Eq. (4) are obtained by linear interpolation. Figure 3 shows the adaptation-induced changes in estimation ariability under all adaptation conditions and for all three subjects, extracted with aboe analysis. Signal detection

6 change in ariability a i 6 subject A W i b ND * R b s ND non-directional 4 subject subject 3 + A i W b D * R bs D directional = a b R b A b Fig. 4. Superposition of directional and non-directional mechanism. (a) For each subject, we used the parameter alues for signature amplitudes and widths oer all adaptation conditions that were fit to the estimation bias (alues shown in Fig.3 in the main text). Again, the non-directional signature was centered at zero motion, while the directional signature was centered at the adaptor elocity. (b) To account for potential oerall scale differences in signature width and amplitude, we allowed two extra parameters R b and A b. theory allowed us to extract both estimation bias and changes in ariability from a single one-sided adaptation experiment (see also Fig. ), where the bias directly follows from the measured point-of-subjectie-equality pse(t), but the extraction of the estimation ariability requires both, measurements of pse(t) and thres(t). Model prediction and fit for changes in ariability In order to test whether the superposition of signatures also extends to estimation ariability, we predicted the expected relatie changes in estimation ariability based on the model parameters fit to the bias data alone (Fig. in main-text), and then compared this prediction to the measured ariability data shown in Fig. 3. More specifically, we applied the same linear superposition of signatures as for the bias (Fig. 4), allowing only two additional parameters that account for potential scale differences in amplitude and size between the signatures for bias and relatie change in ariability. The fit in Fig.3 (red cures) shows that although the data are rather noisy, they are in agreement with the prediction from our model. We hae shown in preious work [5], [], that it is important to account for both, estimation bias and ariance in order to alidate and constrain perceptual models with data. In this regard, it is reassuring that our hypothesis of a superposition of two isomorphic adaptation

7 stage non-directional channels high speed channel adaptation: gain change response a non-directional - a bias low speed channel log(! " /! ) change in ariability stage elocity tuned channels non-directional gain change (inherited from preious stage) non-directional + directional - - bias + directional gain change log(! " /! ) change in ariability - a - -5-5 [deg/s] b - -5-5 [deg/s] Fig. 5. Simulated estimation ariability of the cascade model. (a) The first stage consists of two non-directional channels, tuned for low and high speeds. Adaptation changes the relatie response gain of the channels based on which channel is responding more strongly to the adapting stimulus. These channels proide input to a second processing stage containing more narrowly tuned direction-selectie elocity channels, which inherit the adaptation-induced gain changes of the first stage, and also undergo additional (directional) gain changes in response to the adapting stimulus. (b) Simulation results for biases and changes in ariability for the case of inherited gain reduction from the first stage only (blue), as well as the full model with gain changes in both stages (red). The mean response of each channel is determined by its tuning cure (shown in (a)) and response ariability was assumed to follow a Poisson process. The population response was decoded by a simple population aerage read-out (i.e., the sum of responses, weighted by the preferred speed of each channel). mechanisms is supported not only by the measured bias but also the changes in subjects ariabilities. Simulation of the cascade model including estimation ariability We consider the same two-stage cascade model as described in the main text, expanding it to include response ariability in the channels. We assume that each channel response follows a Poisson distribution with rate determined by the associated tuning cure. As shown in Fig.5, not only bias but also estimation ariability of the simulated model are in close agreement with the proposed extended signature (Fig.).

8 REFERENCES [] O. Schwartz, A. Hsu, and P. Dayan. Space and time in isual context. Nature Reiews Neuroscience, 8:5 535, July 7. [] A.A. Stocker and E.P. Simoncelli. Noise characteristics and prior expectations in human isual speed perception. Nature Neuroscience, pages 578 585, April 6. [3] D.M. Green and J.A. Swets. Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics. Wiley, New York, 966. [4] T D Wickens. Elementary Signal Detection Theory. Oxford Uniersity Press,. [5] A.A. Stocker and E.P. Simoncelli. Constraining a Bayesian model of human isual speed perception. In Lawrence K. Saul, Yair Weiss, and Léon Bottou, editors, Adances in Neural Information Processing Systems NIPS 7, pages 36 368. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 5.