ISSN LIGHT & ENGINEERING. Volume 20, Number 1, 2012 ENGINEERING LIGHT & Znack Publishing House, Moscow

Similar documents
L ight color influence on obstacle recognition in road lighting. 1. Introduction

Mesopic Photometry for SSL. Teresa Goodman Metrology for SSL Meeting 24 th April 2013

TECHNICAL NOTE. Specifying Product Performance for Mesopic Applications

Lab NotesIssue. Disability Glare in the Outdoor Workplace

White Paper Luminance & Illuminance. Brief explanation of photometry for the application of tunnel lighting control

CZECH TECHNICAL UNIVERSITY IN PRAGUE FACULTY OF TRANSPORTATION SCIENCES

2008 Znack Publishing House. Reprinted with permission.

DEA 350: HUMAN FACTORS: THE AMBIENT ENVIRONMENT (Spring 2008)

DEA 3500: HUMAN FACTORS: THE AMBIENT ENVIRONMENT (Fall 2017)

INSPIRATION FROM NATURE

Review of optimal road surface luminance considering dazzling by headlight of the car running in the opposite direction

MODELING PERIPHERAL VISIBILITY UNDER HEADLAMP ILLUMINATION

Disability glare: A study in simulated road lighting conditions

NIST Role in Supporting Solid State Lighting Initiative

Islamic University of Gaza - Palestine. Department of Industrial Engineering

The Control of Glare by the AS1680 Systems

(65th GRE, March 2011, agenda item 4(d))

Key objectives in Lighting design

Fundamentals of light

Fundamentals of light

UNIVERSITY OF HONG KONG Faculty of Engineering M.Sc.(Eng) in Building Services Engineering MEBS6002 LIGHTING ENGINEERING

Measurement method for the proficiency testing program

Predicting the Effects of Discomfort Glare on Visual Performance of Pilots during Nighttime Flying

ROAD TUNNEL LIGHTING Lighting power controllers

Lighting fundamentals

An Experimental Approach to a Definition of the Mesopic Adaptation Field

DEA 3500: HUMAN FACTORS: THE AMBIENT ENVIRONMENT (Spring 2013)

Introduction to Colorimetry

A Study of Mesopic Adaptation conditions for Night Drivers using an approximation for Oncoming headlight glare

TECHNICAL NOTE. Relating Photochemical and Photobiological Quantities to Photometric Quantities

Spectral and photopic studies for high intensity discharge (HID) lamps

DESIGN CRITERIA FOR TUNNEL LIGHTING

IALA Recommendation E On Marine Signal Lights Part 2 - Calculation, Definition and Notation of Luminous Range. Edition 1 December 2008

Glare and visibility by headlight for pedestrian and elderly driver. 22 October 2018 National Traffic Safety and Environment Laboratory

Photometric measurements for visibility level computations

POST-PROCESSING OF RADIANCE IMAGES: VIRTUAL LIGHTING LABORATORY

UNIVERSITY OF HONG KONG

Improvement Uncertainty of Total luminous Flux Measurements by Determining Some Correction Factors

Light pollution by luminaires in roadway lighting

A COMPARISON BETWEEN DGI OBTAINED THROUGH DIFFERENT METHODS AND PERCEIVED EVALUATION INDEX DDG

Photometric Report. 42 Partners Limited 21 Woden Road Wolverhampton WV10 0AU UK

STUDY OVER LUMINOUS CHARACTERISTICS OF THE TRACER COMPOSITIONS

LUMINOUS MEASUREMENTS

Simultaneous lightness contrast with double increments

TECHNICAL NOTE. Interim Recommendation for Practical Application of the CIE System for Mesopic Photometry in Outdoor Lighting

This is a repository copy of Measure for Measure. Semi-cylindrical illuminance: a semi-conceived measure?.

THE MEASUREMENT OF COLOR IN A QUALITY CONTROL SYSTEM: TEXTILE COLOR MATCHING

HONOURTEK Delux Array Series HRA0303 Preliminary

A conventional approach to nighttime visibility in adverse weather conditions

Using a Quantitative Measure to Achieve a Qualitative Lighting Design Effect! Introduction What is Luminance?

A NEW DIGITAL ILLUMINANCE-METER WITH A SPECIAL LUMINANCE TUBEADAPTER FOR UNIVERSAL APPLICATIONS IN LIGHTING ENGINEERING

Motor vehicle and pedal cycle conspicuity - part 3: retroflective and fluorescent materials, disability glare of red markings.

Collective Road Lighting class ME 5

The Quantum Model of the Hydrogen Atom

2007 Technical University of Cluj Napoca, Lighting Engineering Center

Uncertainty determination of correlated color temperature for high intensity discharge lamps.

PHASED OUT. LED light engines LED linear / area. Module CLE Quadrant G2 ADV Modules CLE ADVANCED

LED light engines LED linear / area. Module CLE Quadrant G3 ADV Modules CLE ADVANCED

Artificial light calculation

Solar reflected glare affecting visual performance

AN INSTRUMENT FOR THE MEASUREMENT OF ROAD SURFACE REFLECTION PROPERTIES

AN AUTOMATED GONIOPHOTOMETER FOR LUMINAIRE CHARACTERIZATION

Vision Experiment on White Light Chromaticity for Lighting Duv levels Perceived Most Natural

Uncertainty determination of correlated color temperature for high intensity discharge lamps

Theory of colour measurement Contemporary wool dyeing and finishing

HONOURTEK Delux Array Series HRA0407 Preliminary

ASSESSMENT OF NON-COHERENT LIGHT SOURCES

REPORT NO. Projection Lighting Date of Issue: 12/10/11 (Life) Issue G. Description: LED Luminaire Status at: 11/10/11

ISOFOOTCANDLE LINES OF HORIZONTAL ILLUMINATION

Radiometry, photometry, measuring color

DAYLIGHT, TWILIGHT, AND NIGHT VARIATION IN ROAD ENVIRONMENT-RELATED FREEWAY TRAFFIC CRASHES IN KOREA

Experiment 4 Radiation in the Visible Spectrum

GEOMETRICAL OPTICS Practical 1. Part II. OPTICAL SYSTEMS

OLLA White Paper on the Necessity of Luminous Efficacy Measurement Standardisation of OLED Light Sources

Introduction to atmospheric visibility estimation

Working of a SUNLUX SUNSCREEN

LED light engines LED compact. Module SLE G7 ADV Modules SLE ADVANCED

LED Measurement Instrumentation

Starlight & Lighting Master Plan in Amurrio. Starlight & Lighting Master Plan in Amurrio (Spain)

DRAFT MANUSCRIPT. Title: Legibility of Text on Traffic Signs as a Function of Luminance and Size

SOCCER FIELDS LEVELS FOR UEFA COMPETITIONS ELITE LEVEL A LEVELS : A-B-C-D

LABORATORY WRITE-UP MICHELSON INTERFEROMETER LAB AUTHOR S NAME GOES HERE STUDENT NUMBER:

PHASED OUT. LED light engine / OLED LED linear / area. Umodule STARK QLE G2 CLASSIC STARK QLE

Characterisation & Use of Array Spectrometers

Chapter 16 Assignment Solutions

Photobiological Safety of Luminaires: Refining the New Approach

Proposed Diffuse Ambient Contrast Measurement Methods for Flat Panel Displays

OPAC 101 Introduction to Optics

LightLab I N T E R N A T I O N A L

Analysis of visibility level in road lighting using image processing techniques

Report of Test LLIA A

ISOFOOTCANDLE LINES OF HORIZONTAL ILLUMINATION

SPECIFICATIONS FOR COB SERIES WHITE LED

Electric Lighting Design. Lighting Technology

This is the third of 3 parts detailing my experience of auto guiding for astrophotography.

Effects of Asymmetric Distributions on Roadway Luminance

Prüf-, Überwachungs- und Zertifizierungsgemeinschaft

2x led tubes 150 cm in build-in luminaire ARM2x1500-A-INB-G4-NW by Led Light Europe

Best color perception in spotlighting and downlighting

Report of Test LLIA A

Transcription:

LIGHT & ENGINEERING Volume 20, Number 1, 2012 ISSN 0236-2945 LIGHT & ENGINEERING Volume 20, Number 1, 2012 Znack Publishing House, Moscow

LIGHT & ENGINEERING (Svetotekhnika) Editor-in-Chief: Associate editor: S Editorial Board: N S S I S I Foreign Editorial Advisory Board: Lou Bedocs Wout van Bommel N Peter R. Boyce S Lars Bylund S N Stanislav Darula I S Peter Dehoff Marc Fontoynont N N Franz Hengstberger N I S Warren G. Julian S Zeya Krasko S S S Evan Mills S Lucia R. Ronchi S S I Janos Schanda Nicolay Vasilev S Jennifer Veitch N Moscow, 2012

Light & Engineering Svetotekhnika Vol. 20, No. 1, pp. 121-128, 2012 No. 6, 2011, pp. 37-43 A COMPARISON BETWEEN DIFFERENT LIGHT SOURCES INDUCED GLARE ON PERCEIVED CONTRAST* Paola Iacomussi 1, Giuseppe Rossi 1, and Laura Rossi 2 1 INRiM, Division of Optics, Torino, Italy 2 INRiM, Division of Thermodynamics, Torino, Italy g.rossi@inrim.it ABSTRACT 1The aim of this research is to compare the influence of two different luminous sources (incandescent light and LED light) on perceived contrast of objects in a disability glare context. About 25 subjects were involved in an experiment in order to monitor the variations in their contrast perception threshold, with and without glare sources, using a calibrated computer LCD display. In this first phase the spectral influence is investigate. Typical applications of this research results, could be road and tunnel lighting requirements on disability glare restrictions when LED sources are used. Keywords: subjective experiment, contrast threshold, disability glare, LED 1. INTRODUCTION 1.1. LED We chose to investigate disability glare due to solid state lighting and comparing it with a traditional source, because nowadays LEDs are a great promise in lighting engineering. This is true, not only for energy saving, lifetime, flexibility in luminaries design and range of luminous flux control, but also for appearance and quality of lighting. Considering road and tunnel lighting the spectral behaviour of the LED light improve the vision conditions, especially if peripheral vision in involved. The S/P (scotopic/photopic) ratio of these lumi- * On basis of the report presented at the 27 th CIE Session in Sun City, South Africa, July 2011 naires suggests an increase of the mesopic luminance at road levels (up to 2 cd/ m 2 in European standards (EN 2004)) or in tunnel lighting (internal zone) where, in Italy, the great luminance required values is 3 cd/ m 2 (UNI 2003). For these reasons the Italian standard for the selection of lighting classes gives the possibility to adopt a class with lower requirements in the maintained luminance or illuminance values if LED luminaires with CRI greater of 60 is adopted. Unfortunately, in some installations drivers report higher sensation of glare if compared with traditional discharge lamps installations with practically the same calculated values of TI. Also, the present state of knowledge doesn t provide a reliable quantitative description of some visual perception parameters when LED light is involved: colour rendering, glare and contrast evaluation need improvements in their definitions (CIE 2007). As a matter of fact, psychophysical quantitative parameters (e.g. CRI and TI), and even vision models (e.g. STV), as defined in CIE technical reports or international and national standards, show low or doubtful correlation with the real world vision conditions when LEDs are involved. Lighting experts are aware of the problem and several researches about LED and glare have been starting to be carried out (i.e. (EMRP 2009)). Our work takes place in this kind of researches. It is divided into three phases: the first two are carried out in laboratory conditions (influence of the source spectra, influence of the source dimensions). The last one (dynamic evaluation of visual performances) will be carried out next year considering real 121

installations. The range of tested luminance is between 0,5 to 20 cd / m 2 for considering the last part of the transition zone in tunnel installations too. 1.2. Disability glare and contrast Glare is a very crucial matter because it affects negatively our ability in vision, reducing contrast and for this reason, our ability to perceive objects and obstacles. For example, in road lighting, the visibility of objects is the key condition for motorised traffic safety, and their perception is strongly linked to contrast, glare and, in a lesser extent, colour rendering conditions. These problems are deeply considered in CIE both in Division 1 and 4 where TC is involved in their study. Usually subjective experiments concerning disability glare have been performed considering lighting sources of different intensity and angular extension and their influences on perceived contrast. From these experiments results, several vision models have been proposed, in order to be applied in lighting systems design and in defining maximum acceptable values. For road lighting, CIE suggests several design rules and parameters (average value of luminance or illuminance, uniformity, threshold increment, etc., CIE 2010) adopted in national and international standards as lighting classes (EN 2004), where the performance values are given according to road and traffic conditions. These values, based on several decades of experiences in installations, are well fitted to common discharge light sources usually adopted in lighting plants. Only disability glare is considered and a maximum level of TI is given, correlated to the average illuminance or luminance on the road surface. Installations where TI values are lower then the required limit are considered without glare, installations with TI values greater then this limit are not allowed. This represent a simplify approach of a continuous varying phenomena, where the increase of glare requires more light to maintain constant the traffic safety conditions. To reduce the environmental impact of lighting installations and save energy some more complex rules could be proposed (EMRP 2009) especially for adaptive systems. For these reason, glare and contrast evaluation needs to be reviewed and improved for characterising LED luminaries in road lighting installations. 1.3. Models for vision As said, glare is always a very negative matter in vision: discomfort glare is annoying while high levels of disability glare can totally hamper vision (e.g. this is the case of glare weapons). The main goal of this research work, considering the three phases described above is to verify and correlate the STV (Small Target Visibility) model and the CIE disability glare model when LED luminaires are used for road lighting. In night vision, especially during driving, glare can affect the safety of people. For this reason, different methods have been proposed in order to generate a model for night vision, especially concerning road lighting. The safety key issue of a road lighting system is the ability to make visible an obstacle at a distance of vision, conventionally considered equal at 83 m. STV method is an algorithm (ADRIAN, W. 1989), (ADRIAN, W. 1993) to quantify the ability of a lighting system to make visible a series of normalized obstacles, properly distributed on the illuminated area. It was proposed as an alternative to the usual luminance design method (IES 2000), based on the achievement of minimum values of average luminance and uniformity. In Italy, a standard is under development: it permits the comparison between systems formally similar in terms of lighting classes, but with different characteristics in the ability to make objects visible on the road. The veiling luminance due to the light sources in peripheral vision is evaluated following the CIE General Disability Glare Equation (CIE 2002). 10 5 0, 1 = + + 3 2 θ θ θ 4 1 + 62, 5 + 0,025, (1) where: E glare is the illuminance in lux on the observer eye due to the glare sources; q is the angle between the observation direction and the glare source (in degree); P represents the eye pigmentation factor (0 for black eyes, 0,5 for brown eyes, 1,0 for light eyes and 1,2 for very light-blue eyes); A is the observer age in years. This model is applicable for angles q between 0,1 and 100. 122

Fig. 1. Experimental set-up 2. THE EXPERIMENT 2.1. Description The research described in this paper is a preliminary test carried out with the aim to gain knowledge about the influence of the different spectra between white LED and an incandescent source on glare in the vision of small virtual targets in order to design both a static and a dynamic experiment in an ad hoc real installation, with significant parameters in the more critical range. This experimental session investigates the influence of different light sources on the perceived contrast, using self luminous images of a target on a monitor in a dark and quiet room (Fig. 1). Behind a dark panel, on the two sides of the screen, two holes (100 mm diameter) accommodate the LED source on the right and the reference source on the left (Fig. 1). The angular displacement of the sources was variable with the random position of the target in the display (direction of observation) in the range from 14 to 46. The emitted spectra of the two sources are diagrammed in Fig. 2. This test considers as reference source an incandescent lamp at 3000 K. Incandescent sources are not representative of traditional sources for road lighting, but of sources used in experiments about glare and vision modelling. In the next phase, a second static experiment, to evaluate the influence of the source luminous surface (a large surface of uniform high luminance for traditional luminaires and a number of bright point uniformly distributed on a low luminance surface for LED luminaires), sources more representative of road lighting will be used (i.e. sodium high pressure). The third and final phase will consider a real installation and dynamic experiments. The subject was seated in front of the calibrated monitor at a distance of 0,6 m. The luminance of the screen was set at two nominal values (9 and 22 cd/ m 2 ). Its uniformity was measured using a calibrated ILMD (Imaging Luminance Measurement Device) placed at the observer position. The average values were 9.04 cd/ m 2 and 20.83 cd/ m 2 with a uniformity (minimum value / maximum value) of 0.89 and 0.91 respectively. The targets position on the display was defined using a pseudo-random algorithm for every screen luminance and glare conditions (no source, LED source, incandescent source) and was the same for every subject. To improve the accuracy in the calculation of intrinsic contrast, the luminance of target and luminance of the background (a square area centred round the target with a side of three times the target side) were measured with the same ILMD at every random position of the target. In front of both lamps two diffusing glasses were put in order to obtain two uniform sources of the same angular extended. Using this expedient only the spectrum influences on subject performance has been investigated. Since it is very difficult to obtain high uniformities of the source surfaces, their luminance were measured with the same ILMD, pixel by pixel and these values used to calculate the veiling luminance (eqn. 1) considering the correct angle between the 123

Fig. 2. Spectral density of the two sources used in the experiment Fig. 3. Target sequence ordered by luminance contrast target on the screen, the measured luminance of elementary surface framed by the pixel and converting this luminance value in the equivalent illuminance on the observer eye. Twenty-two subjects with good visual capacities, distinguished by age (22 to 30: young group, 35 to 55 elderly group), iris colour (light and dark) and eyeglass, attended the experiment. Targets dimension and source position satisfy the constraints of the CIE model for glare (CIE 2002). All targets have the same squared shape and dimension (4 mm) and are surely supra-threshold (referring to dimension). The experiment was divided in two sections: in the first part, a set of eighteen targets, with different luminance, was generated and randomly positioned on the screen. When the observer saw a target, had to click with the mouse on it. Two different backgrounds luminance (see above) have been used, and presented alternatively to the subjects. In the second part of the experiment, subjects were asked to put in order all targets they detected before, and displayed in the same place as in the first experiment part, from the lower to the higher contrast as shown in Fig. 3. Both experiment parts were performed by all subjects in three different visual conditions: Dark; With only the incandescent source on, providing glare and 479 lx on the subject s eyes; With only the LED source on, providing glare and 503 lx on the subject s eyes. In this way it was possible to investigate: The contribution of glare due to a specific light source on perceived contrast (considering also the time needed to fulfil the test and the average time from the detection to a target and its subsequent) in order to delineate the perceptual threshold, with particular attention to the specific influence of lights with different spectrum; The influence of glare on the ability of subject in discriminating differences between target with different contrast; The validity of the Adrian model of STV for glare coming from LED sources. 2.2. Measurement accuracies All the measurements were carried out with calibrated instruments and the measurement uncertainty of 1.5 % for illuminance and 2 % for luminance. The power light sources and display operated with stabilized power supply to guaranty high stability of the photometric parameters for all the experiment time. The operating conditions were tested periodically during the experiment days. Because the position of the head of the subject was weakly defined, depending on the observer head dimensions and small movements during the experiment, the Illuminance on the subject eye could have been only estimated. For this purpose the eyes illuminace was measured in 15 points of a parallelepiped 20 mm x 40 mm x 60 mm centred at the nominal eye positions, Fig. 4. The uniformity of illuminance in this space was 0.63 for the incandescent source and 0.53 for the LED source. These non-uniformities are 124

Fig. 4. Measurement points of illuminance around observer s eye Fig. 5. Contrast perception threshold in the dark condition, using low luminance background (left) and high luminance background (right), for the group 1 (age from 22 to 30) and group 2 (age from 35 to 55) gets. This means that the selected group of observers was suitable for this visual task. Considering all subjects together, the contrast perception threshold shift due to the presence of glaring sources is visualized in Figs. 8 and 9. This shift becomes significant with a low luminance background and an incandescent glare source in negative contrasts. With a high luminance background the two glare source have practically the same influence. The resolution in the luminance increments of the computer display and its graphic driver reduce the possibility to better investigation in the near underthreshold zone. For what regards the second part of the test, the subjects were asked to align all the targets they selected before ordering them by contrast (Fig. 3). Considered the ideal right alignment for each target selection made by each subject, we then calcuthe main source of uncertainty in the evaluation of the veiling luminance and of the perceived contrasts. 2.3. Data analysis and results As data analysis, we first calculated the contrast perception threshold for each environmental condition (dark, incandescent light and LED), for each background used for the target visualization (low and high luminance) and for each group of subjects (group 1: age from 22 to 30, group 2: age from 35 to 55, Figs. 5 7). For what regards the first condition (in darkness), in general we see that not so many differences comes out from the age parameter as contrast perception thresholds are similar between the two groups (Fig. 5). On the other hand, the threshold shift is considerable if a lighter background is used instead of a darker one for the visualisation of tar- 125

Fig. 6. Contrast perception threshold in the glare condition caused by incandescent light, using a low luminance background (left) and a high luminance background (right), for the group 1 (age from 22 to 30) and group 2 (age from 35 to 55) Fig. 7. Contrast perception threshold in the glare condition caused by LED light, using a low luminance (left) and a high luminance background (right), for the group 1 (age from 22 to 30) and group 2 (age from 35 to 55) lated all the mistakes committed by subjects doing this operation. A mistake in positioning a target in the correct place considering its contrast, as regards the one before and the one after, has been considered one error and so on for all the sequence starting from the first target at left. Then we obtained a mistake index for each subject, dividing the number of mistakes committed by the number of targets ordered (this number is different for each subject because of course someone can detect more target than someone else). Doing this operation we could compare the influence of glare in the capacity of the subjects in ordering targets by contrast, that means distinguish very little luminance differences. The test stated in dark conditions and low luminance background (see Fig. 10) therefore the great number of mistake we recorded in the dark condition can be biased by the necessity for the subject to have a small training to understand clearly the experiment behaviour. At low luminance background, the two sources give practically the same result with a low accuracy in doing the visual task. Experiments carried out at high luminance background did not show the bias conditions because the subject now has experience in the task and, expect for the incandescent source show a great improvement in accuracy (Fig. 11). 3. CONCLUSIONS The preliminary experiments described in this paper try to obtain data for compare the spectral influence on disability glare for application in road lighting of LED luminaires. In road lighting the safety traffic condition is described by the capability to detect an obstacle on the carriageway at a distance of about 83 m, when the 126

Fig. 8. Comparison between the three contrast perception thresholds (dark condition, glare by incandescent light and glare by LED light) considering all the subjects and using a low luminance background for the target visualization Fig. 9. Comparison between the three contrast perception thresholds (dark condition, glare by incandescent light and glare by LED light) considering all the subjects and using a high luminance background for the target visualization Fig. 10. Comparison between the three testing condition (dark condition, glare by incandescent light and glare by LED light) considering all the subjects and using a low luminance background for the target visualization Fig. 11. Comparison between the three testing condition (dark condition, glare by incandescent light and glare by LED light) considering all the subjects and using a high luminance background for the target visualization 127

eye is adapted at the average road luminance and in the presence of disability glare due to luminaires. The first part of the experiment investigate this situation: the effect of glare is practically equivalent for the two source but at lower adaptation luminance the LED gives better performances for negative contrast. The second safety condition is to clearly identify the obstacle or the lighted environments. This can be correlated to the ability to perceived and understand small luminance differences, i.e. the visual task studies in the second part of the experiment. The effect of glare is practically independent from the glare source type at higher adaptation luminances (22 cd m -2 ) while at lower adaptation luminances LED (9 cd m -2 ) LED sources give better performances then incandescent lamps. REFERENCES 1. ADRIAN, W. 1989. Visibility of Targets: Model for Calculation. Lighting Res. Technol., 21, 181 188. 2. ADRIAN, W. 1993, The Physiological Basis of the Visibility Concept, Proceedings of 2 nd Inter- national Symposium on Visibility and Luminance in Roadway Lighting October 26 27 1993 Orlando Florida. 3. CIE 2002. CIE 146:2002, CIE 147:2002, Collection on Glare:, Vienna: CIE. 4. CIE 2007, CIE 177:2007, Colour Rendering of White LED Sources, Vienna: CIE. 5. CIE 2010, CIE 115:2010, Lighting of Road for Motor and Pedestrian Traffi c, 2 nd Edition, Vienna: CIE. 6. EINHORN, H.M., PAUL, B.M. 1999. Discomfort glare from small light sources, Lighting Res. Technol., 31 (4) 139 144. 7. EMRP 2009 Joint Research Project Protocol JRP 09 Metrology for Solid State Lighting 09, VSL 2010. 8. EN 2004. EN 13201 2:2004. Road lighting Part2: Performance Requirements. 9. IES 2000 IES RP-8 00 (R2005) Roadway Lighting ANSI Approved, New York: Illuminating Engineering Society. 10. UNI 2003. UNI 10095:2003. Tunnel Lighting, Milano UNI (in Italian). 11. UNI 2007. UNI 11248:2007. Road Lighting Selection of Lighting Classes, Milano UNI (in Italian). Laura Rossi, Doctor in Philosophy of Science and now she is Ph.D. student in metrology at INRiM, Department of Thermodynamics. She is an expert in soft metrology and her fields of interest are visual and auditory perception, subjective experiments design and cognitive psychology Paola Iacomussi, Doctor in physics, is a senior researcher at INRiM, Department of Optics. She is the Italian member of CIE in Division 8 and expert in photometry, and works for art illumination Giuseppe Rossi, Ph.D. in Metrology, is a senior researcher at INRiM, Department of Optics. He is the Italian member of CIE Division 4 and expert in photometry, optical characterization of materials and road lighting installation 128