Indirect Clinical Evidence of Driver Inattention as a Cause of Crashes

Similar documents
Using High-Resolution Detector and Signal Data to Support Crash Identification and Reconstruction. Indrajit Chatterjee Gary Davis May, 2011

A Case Control Study of Speed and Crash Risk. Technical Report 2

A Case Control Study of Speed and Crash Risk. Technical Report 3. Speed as a Risk Factor in Run-off Road Crashes

Evaluation of fog-detection and advisory-speed system

The Influence of Fog on Motion Discrimination Thresholds in Car Following

Perth County Road Fatal Head-on Collision - A Common and Dangerous Issue

To convert a speed to a velocity. V = Velocity in feet per seconds (ft/sec) S = Speed in miles per hour (mph) = Mathematical Constant

CHAPTER 3. CAPACITY OF SIGNALIZED INTERSECTIONS

CHAPTER 2. CAPACITY OF TWO-WAY STOP-CONTROLLED INTERSECTIONS

Research Article Headway Distributions Based on Empirical Erlang and Pearson Type III Time Methods Compared

Local Calibration Factors for Implementing the Highway Safety Manual in Maine

Statistical Model Checking Applied on Perception and Decision-making Systems for Autonomous Driving

Traffic Modelling for Moving-Block Train Control System

Checklist: Deposing the Driver in an Auto Accident

A FIELD EXPERIMENT ON THE ACCURACY OF VISUAL ASSESSMENT OF WINTER ROAD CONDITIONS

Answers to Problem Set Number 02 for MIT (Spring 2008)

Warning Timing, Driver Distraction, and Driver Response to Imminent Rear-End Collisions

Choosing a Safe Vehicle Challenge: Analysis: Measuring Speed Challenge: Analysis: Reflection:

EXAMINATION OF THE SAFETY IMPACTS OF VARYING FOG DENSITIES: A CASE STUDY OF I-77 IN VIRGINIA

Overcoming the Limitations of Conservation of Linear Momentum by Including External Impulse

Subject: Desert Palisades Specific Plan - Tram Way Access Sight Distance

CE351 Transportation Systems: Planning and Design

Report of a Complaint Handling Review in relation to Police Scotland

05724 BUTLER TWP PD /04/ :31 Sun 39:54: :11: F F T 1

Traffic safety research: a challenge for extreme value statistics

MODELING OF 85 TH PERCENTILE SPEED FOR RURAL HIGHWAYS FOR ENHANCED TRAFFIC SAFETY ANNUAL REPORT FOR FY 2009 (ODOT SPR ITEM No.

Chapter 5 Traffic Flow Characteristics

Uncertainty in the Yellow Change Interval

Revision checklist. Step Learning outcome Had a look Nearly there Nailed it!

FORMULATION OF DRIVER JUDGMENT PROCESS AROUND CURVES FOR DEVIATED STATE DETECTION. Tokyo, Japan

Safe Transportation Research & Education Center UC Berkeley

Airborne Traffic Flow Data and Traffic Management. Mark Hickman and Pitu Mirchandani

1. The diagram below shows water falling from a dam. Each minute kg of water falls vertically into the pool at the bottom.

Spatial discrete hazards using Hierarchical Bayesian Modeling

Modelling and Simulation for Train Movement Control Using Car-Following Strategy

EDITORIAL NOTE: PERSONAL/COMMERCIAL DETAILS ONLY HAVE BEEN DELETED. IN THE DISTRICT COURT AT AUCKLAND CIV [2016] NZDC 12626

Simulation study of traffic accidents in bidirectional traffic models

Egg Crash! Designing a Collision Safety Device

5) A stone is thrown straight up. What is its acceleration on the way up? 6) A stone is thrown straight up. What is its acceleration on the way down?

FIRST MIDTERM - REVIEW PROBLEMS

CHALLENGE #1: ROAD CONDITIONS

Understand FORWARD COLLISION WARNING WHAT IS IT? HOW DOES IT WORK? HOW TO USE IT?

a) An object decreasing speed then increasing speed in the opposite direction.

Chapter 9 Review. Block: Date:

REVIEW SET MIDTERM 1

ANALYSES OF HEADWAYS FOR ASSESSMENT OF RISK REAR- END COLLISION

2.1 KINEMATICS HW/Study Packet

Traffic flow theory involves the development of mathematical relationships among

Team # Page 1 of 20

Modeling: Start to Finish

FORM 4 PHYSICS TIME: 1h 30min

Real-time, Adaptive Prediction of Incident Delay for Advanced Traffic Management Systems

Document downloaded from: This paper must be cited as:

Effect of Environmental Factors on Free-Flow Speed

Problem Set Number 01, MIT (Winter-Spring 2018)

Smoke Fog Superfog - Smoke and Fog mixture Heavy rain and wind

Frames of reference. Objectives. Assessment. Physics terms. Equations. What is a frame of reference? 5/19/14

Vehicle Motion Equations:

Variable Speed Approach for Congestion Alleviation on Boshporus Bridge Crossing

Show all workings for questions that involve multiple choice.

FORM 4 PHYSICS TIME: 1h 30min

A first car following model

A New Paradigm for Rear-end Crash Prevention Driving Performance

CHAPTER 2 DESCRIBING MOTION: KINEMATICS IN ONE DIMENSION

Responsive Traffic Management Through Short-Term Weather and Collision Prediction

EVALUATION OF SAFETY PERFORMANCES ON FREEWAY DIVERGE AREA AND FREEWAY EXIT RAMPS. Transportation Seminar February 16 th, 2009

Factors Affecting the Severity of Injuries Sustained in Collisions with Roadside Objects

Speed-Flow and Bunching Relationships for Uninterrupted Flows

Chapter 2: 1-D Kinematics

PHYSICS - CLUTCH CH 02: 1D MOTION (KINEMATICS)

Course Outline Introduction to Transportation Highway Users and their Performance Geometric Design Pavement Design

VISUAL PHYSICS ONLINE DYNAMICS TYPES OF FORCES FRICTION

12/06/2010. Chapter 2 Describing Motion: Kinematics in One Dimension. 2-1 Reference Frames and Displacement. 2-1 Reference Frames and Displacement

P5 Momentum Collision Calculations

Signalized Intersection Delay Models

Available online Journal of Scientific and Engineering Research, 2017, 4(4): Research Article

ANIL TUTORIALS. Motion IMPORTANT NOTES ANIL TUTORIALS,SECTOR-5,DEVENDRA NAGAR,HOUSE NO-D/156,RAIPUR,C.G,PH

CHAPTER 5 DELAY ESTIMATION FOR OVERSATURATED SIGNALIZED APPROACHES

Velocity Time Graphs 12.2

2.1 Traffic Stream Characteristics. Time Space Diagram and Measurement Procedures Variables of Interest

FUNDAMENTALS OF TRANSPORTATION ENGINEERING By Jon D. Fricker and Robert K. Whitford

The Determination of Vehicle Speeds from Delta-V in Two Vehicle Planar Collisions

Impact of Geometric Factors on the Capacity of Single-Lane Roundabouts

College Physics: A Strategic Approach, 3e (Knight) Chapter 2 Motion in One Dimension. 2.1 Conceptual Questions

Freeway rear-end collision risk for Italian freeways. An extreme value theory approach

What are Intelligent Transportation Systems? Major ITS Areas

The stopping distance of a car is the sum of the thinking distance and the braking distance.

Dynamics Kinetics of a particle Section 4: TJW Force-mass-acceleration: Example 1

PH 2213 : Chapter 05 Homework Solutions

IN THE SUPREME COURT OF BELIZE, A.D KIRK HALL BOWEN & BOWEN LTD.

Recent Researches in Engineering and Automatic Control

Measuring Momentum: Using distance moved after impact to estimate velocity

Page 1 / 15. Motion Unit Test. Name: Motion ONLY, no forces. Question 1 (1 point) Examine the graphs below:

Estimation of Measures of Effectiveness Based on Connected Vehicle Data

Created by T. Madas KINEMATIC GRAPHS. Created by T. Madas

FORCE AND MOTION SEPUP UNIT OVERVIEW

A Driver Interface for a Road Hazard Warning System: Development and Preliminary Evaluation

$QDO\]LQJ$UWHULDO6WUHHWVLQ1HDU&DSDFLW\ RU2YHUIORZ&RQGLWLRQV

H. T. Zwahlen, A. Russ, and T. Schnell 1

Look over: Chapter 2 Sections 1-9 Sample Problems 1, 2, 5, 7. Look over: Chapter 2 Sections 1-7 Examples 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11 PHYS 2211

Transcription:

University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Driving Assessment Conference 2007 Driving Assessment Conference Jul 10th, 12:00 AM Indirect Clinical Evidence of Driver Inattention as a Cause of Crashes Gary A. Davis University of Minnesota, Minneapolis Follow this and additional works at: http://ir.uiowa.edu/drivingassessment Davis, Gary A.. Indirect Clinical Evidence of Driver Inattention as a Cause of Crashes. In: Proceedings of the Fourth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design, July 9-12, 2007, Stevenson, Washington. Iowa City, IA: Public Policy Center, University of Iowa, 2007: 217-223. https://doi.org/10.17077/drivingassessment.1240 This Event is brought to you for free and open access by the Public Policy Center at Iowa Research Online. It has been accepted for inclusion in Driving Assessment Conference by an authorized administrator of Iowa Research Online. For more information, please contact lib-ir@uiowa.edu.

INDIRECT CLINICAL EVIDENCE OF DRIVER INATTENTION AS A CAUSE OF CRASHES Gary A. Davis University of Minnesota Minneapolis, MN USA E-mail: drtrips@umn.edu Summary: A recent report from the 100-Car Study identified driver inattention as a significant risk factor for involvement in crashes or near-crashes. If in fact inattention is a cause of crashes, it should be possible to find at least indirect evidence of it by reconstructing actual crashes. This paper describes reconstruction of two rear-ending crashes on an urban freeway, one left-turn cross-path crash on a suburban arterial, and one vehicle/pedestrian collision. Bayesian reconstruction methods were used to estimate driver reaction times, and these were then compared to reaction time measures from the literature. The working hypothesis was that atypically long reaction times on the part of the colliding drivers would provide indirect evidence for driver inattention. It turned out that an atypically long reaction time was shown by only one of the four colliding drivers, but that other indications of inattention were found in two other crashes. INTRODUCTION Driver inattention, and the degree to which it causes traffic crashes, is an active topic of debate. The recent report from the 100-Car Study (Klauer et al., 2006) offered evidence that inattention was a significant risk factor for involvement in crashes and near-crashes. In medical research, if an agent is the cause of a disease, it should be possible to use the agent to produce the disease in laboratory settings, find significant statistical associations between the presence of the agent and occurrence of the disease, and identify the agent in clinical investigations of disease cases. Similarly, if driver inattention is a cause of crashes, it should be possible to mimic its effects in simulator studies, identify it as a significant risk factor in observational studies, and find evidence for its presence in the reconstruction of crashes. This paper offers an initial contribution to this last line of evidence. Bayesian crash reconstruction methods will be used to estimate relevant driver variables, particular driver reaction times in actual crashes. Four crashes will be treated here: two freeway rear-ending crashes that occurred on an urban freeway, one left-turn cross-path crash that occurred on a four-lane, two-way arterial, and a collision between a vehicle and child pedestrian described in the report by McLean et al. (1994). An overview of the Bayesian approach to crash reconstruction can be found in Davis (2003). In essence, the reconstructionist is treated as a Bayesian agent, whose prior knowledge concerning the crashes is to be updated using available evidence. The reconstructionist s prior knowledge has two components. The first component is a structural equation model, which relates input variables such initial speeds, braking decelerations, and reaction times, to output variables, such as skidmark lengths, pedestrian throw distances, vehicle damage measurements, or post-impact trajectories. The second component consists of probability distributions expressing the reconstructionist s prior opinion concerning likely values for the model s input variables. The 217

objective is then to use Bayes Theorem to update these prior probability assessments in the light of evidence available from the crash scene, in order to produce posterior probability distributions for the input variables. Only very rarely will closed form expressions for these posterior distributions be available, but Markov Chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) methods can be used to compute approximations. CRASH CASES Freeway Rear-Ending Crashes Video recordings of two freeway rear-ending crashes provided the raw data for this first study. Details of the study location, and the data reduction and analysis can be found in Davis and Swenson (2006). Each recording captured the colliding vehicles, along with several other vehicles preceding the colliding pair. The computer program VideoPoint was used to extract the (x,y) screen coordinates of vehicles from a frame of the recorded video by clicking on a discernable point on the object of interest. Standard photogrammetry methods were then used to convert the screen coordinates to the corresponding real-world coordinates, producing measurements of each vehicle s trajectory prior to the crash. Each trajectory was then assumed to consist of three phases, an initial phase where the vehicle traveled at a constant speed, a braking phase where the vehicle decelerated at a constant rate, and a stopped phase were the vehicle remained stationary. That is, if x k (t) denotes the location of vehicle k at time t, x k (t) = v k t, t t 0k v k t - 0.5 a k (t-t 0k ) 2, t 0k < t t 0k +v k /a k v k t 0k + v k 2 /2a k, t > t 0k +v k /a k where t 0k is the time at which driver k began braking, v k is vehicle k s initial speed, and a k is vehicle k s braking deceleration. This trajectory model can be viewed as a nonlinear model for predicting a vehicle s location, parameterized by the initial speed v k, the braking initiation time t 0k, and the braking deceleration a k. By assuming non-informative prior probability distributions for these parameters, and then treating the measured trajectory data as error-prone measurements from a process governed by the trajectory equation, Bayes theorem can be used to compute posterior probability distributions for the trajectory model parameters. The Markov Chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) program WinBUGS (Spiegelhalter, et al., 2003) was used to compute Bayes estimates of the trajectory models for each of the vehicles in both crashes. Table 1 displays summaries of the posterior distributions for the trajectory model variables, and the estimated reaction times, for the first collision. If we take the posterior mean as the best point estimate of a variable s value, it appears that driver 1 was initially travelling at about 50 feet/sec, and about 28.2 seconds after the start of the video segment began braking to a stop with a deceleration of about 6.8 feet/sec 2. Driver 2 was initially traveling at about 46.7 feet/sec, and about 1.9 seconds after driver 1, driver 2 began braking, at a rate of about 6.5 feet/sec 2, and so forth. Each driver appears to have initiated braking after the preceding driver, and a collision occurred when vehicle 7 struck vehicle 6. 218

Table 1. Posterior Means and Standard Deviations (in parentheses) for Trajectory Variables, and Driver Reaction Times, from First Rear-Ending Collision Vehicle (k) v k (feet/sec) t 0k (seconds) a k (feet/sec 2 Reaction time ) (Seconds) 1 50.0 (0.8) 28.2 (0.1) 6.8 (0.1) 2 46.7 (0.3) 30.1 (0.1) 6.5 (0.1) 1.9 (0.14) 3 41.8 (0.4) 34.3 (0.2) 12.6 (1.0) 4.2 (0.16) 4 42.3 (0.3) 36.1 (0.1) 14.2 (0.5) 1.9 (0.17) 5 39.3 (0.2) 37.6 (0.1) 16.0 (0.9) 1.4 (0.10) 6 42.3 (0.6) 38.7 (0.1) 17.3 (1.6) 1.1 (0.03) 7 41.7 (0.4) 40.3 (0.1) 20.3 (1.1) 1.65 (0.15) A number of studies have attempted to estimate driver reaction times in emergency braking situations. Fambro et al. (1997), reviewing many of these, found average reaction times ranging between about 1.06 seconds and 1.5 seconds. They also conducted field tests of driver braking, and reported an average, for surprise braking situations, of about 1.1 seconds, and an estimated 95 th percentile of about 2 seconds. With the exception of driver 3, the estimated reaction times in Table 1 appear to be roughly consistent with what Fambro et al. reported. In particular, the estimated reaction time of the colliding driver 7, at about 1.7 seconds, was shorter than the estimated reaction times for three preceding, non-colliding drivers. It appears difficult then to maintain that an atypically long reaction time on the part of the colliding driver was a cause of this collision. As pointed out in Davis and Swenson (2006), one of the interesting features of this sequence is the long reaction time on the part of driver 3. Using the Monte Carlo estimates of the trajectory model parameters, it is possible to simulate what would have happened all other things being equal if driver 3 s reaction time had been shorter, say equal to about 2.0 seconds. In this hypothetical scenario, it turns out that the minimum deceleration needed for driver 7 to stop before colliding with vehicle 6 falls to about 12.2 fps 2. Since this is substantially less than driver 7 s estimated actual deceleration (20.3 fps 2 ), what we have is that, had driver 3 s reaction time been less, the collision between vehicles 6 and 7 probably would not have occurred. The reason for this, as pointed out in Davis and Swenson (2006), is that the drivers in this stopping wave appear to have had reaction times that were longer than their corresponding following headways. When this occurs there is a tendency for each driver to need to decelerate at a higher rate than that of the vehicle ahead, until ultimately the required safe successful deceleration exceeds what a driver can do. Thus, although driver 3 was not involved in a crash, a plausible case can be made that his or her long reaction time (and so, possibly, inattention) was a cause of the collision between vehicles 6 and 7. Table 2 shows a similar set of estimates for a different rear-ending collision, in this case vehicle 6 collided with vehicle 5, and shortly after that vehicle 7 collided with vehicle 6. Looking at the estimated reaction time for driver 7, we see that this was approximately 3.2 seconds, which is arguably longer than what an alert driver should have needed. Counterfactual simulation where this reaction time is reduced to 2.0 seconds shows that, other things equal, had driver 7 reacted faster the collision between vehicles 6 and 7 would, with high probability, not have occurred. In 219

this collision the pattern of successively increasing decelerations seen in the first case is less obvious, and this second collision is more consistent with the hypothesis that distraction by the colliding driver was a cause of the collision. Table 2. Posterior Means and Standard Deviations (in parentheses) for Trajectory Variables, and Driver Reaction Times, from Second Rear-Ending Collision Vehicle (k) v k (feet/sec) t 0k (seconds) a k (feet/sec 2 Reaction time ) (Seconds) 1 57.6 (0.7) 13.9 (0.11) 10.1 (0.18) 2 47.9 (0.55) 14.9 (0.12) 8.4 (0.14) 1.0 (0.16) 3 57.1 (0.4) 17.4 (0.12) 11.4 (0.39) 2.5 (0.16) 4 56.7 (0.2) 18.2 (0.07) 12.4 (0.28) 0.9 (0.14) 5 59.0 (0.3) 19.7 (0.06) 12.3 (0.20) 1.4 (0.89) 6 65.7 (1.5) 20.2 (0.19) 11.7 (0.26) 0.6 (0.20) 7 54.6 (0.2) 23.5 (0.07) 27.4 (1.8) 3.2 (0.21) A Left-Turn Cross-Path Crash The next case we will consider involved a collision between a left-turning vehicle and an opposing vehicle on a four-lane arterial. The opposing vehicle had the right-of-way, and the leftturning driver said he didn t see vehicle #2 when he began to turn. The opposing vehicle left an approximately 44-foot long skid mark prior to colliding, each vehicle s final position was documented in a scale drawing prepared by the Minnesota State Patrol (MSP), and probable estimates of each vehicle s orientation immediately prior to the collision were also documented. Using the MSP s estimates of vehicle weights and the vehicles approach and departure angles, a standard momentum conservation method (Fricke 1990) was embedded within a Bayesian accident reconstruction model, similar to that described in (Davis 2003). This produced estimates of each vehicle s speed immediately before the collision, and a standard skidding model then yielded an estimate of the opposing vehicle s speed at the start of the skidmark. By approximating the path of the left-turning vehicle with a circular arc joining that vehicle s position at the moment of collision to the centerline of its departure lane, together with a plausible final position in the destination lane, it was possible to estimate that the turning vehicle traveled approximately 52 feet between initiation of the turn and the collision. Dividing this distance by the estimate of the turning vehicle s pre-collision speed then gave an estimated time elapsing between turn initiation and collision, and subtracting the time the opposing vehicle spent braking prior to collision from this elapsed time gave an estimate of the opposing driver s reaction time. Adding the distance traveled during the reaction time to the point where braking began gave an estimate of the location of the opposing vehicle when the turning vehicle began to turn, and dividing this by the opposing vehicle s initial speed gave an estimate of the time gap accepted by the left-turning driver. Bayes estimates of relevant variables were computed using WinBUGS, and summaries of these results are displayed in Table 3. These estimates were computed assuming an uncertainty in the measured angles of ± 5 o, an uncertainty in the vehicles weights and the turning vehicle s initial distance of ± 5%, and the measurement error for skidding distances having a coefficient of variation equal to 0.1. 220

Table 3. Summary of Posterior Distributions for Variables in Left-Turn Collision Model Variable Opposing Vehicle s Initial Speed (mph) Turning Vehicle s Initial Speed (mph) Opposing Vehicle s Initial Distance (feet) Opposing Driver s Reaction Time (seconds) Accepted Gap (seconds) Posterior Mean Posterior Standard Deviation Posterior 2.5%-ile Posterior 97.5%-ile 51.4 4.5 43.1 60.2 14.6 2.1 10.8 19.2 172 19.7 138 215 1.4 0.35 0.8 2.2 2.3 0.36 1.7 3.1 The posterior mean for the opposing driver s reaction time was about 1.4 seconds, which is consistent with the typical reaction times described earlier, and so it is difficult to maintain that inattention on the part of the opposing driver was a factor. On the other hand, the estimated accepted gap was about 2.3 seconds, and we can ask whether or not this was shorter than what drivers typically accept. The minimum accepted gaps used by the 2000 HCM for left turns is 4.1 seconds, and the minimum accepted gap in a field study of gaps accepted by left-turning drivers on an urban arterial was 3.8 seconds (Davis 2007). Staplin (1995) reported average minimum acceptable gaps, for an opposing vehicle speed of 60 mph, in the range of 5-6 seconds. The short accepted gap is consistent with what the opposing driver reported, so a plausible case can be made that driver inattention contributed to this crash, but that this is indicated by the gap accepted by a turning driver, not the reaction time of the opposing driver. A Vehicle/Pedestrian Collision The final collision we will consider is case 89-H002, described in the report by McLean et al. (1994). Here, a 5-year old boy apparently ran from behind a parked car into a two-lane road, stopped briefly in the road, and then attempted to run across the road. He was struck by a vehicle traveling in the far lane. The available evidence consisted of a scale drawing of the crash scene, with a measured skid mark length, identification of the point of collision, and measured throw distance for the pedestrian. The prior distribution for the braking deceleration was taken to be uniform between 0.55 and 0.9 g, the prior distribution for the pedestrian s running speed was taken to be uniform between 1.0 and 4.5 meters/sec, and the prior distribution for the time the pedestrian spent waiting in the road was taken to be uniform between 1 and 3 seconds. The driver s reaction time was then estimated as the difference between the time the pedestrian appeared from behind the parked car and the time when braking began. Table 4 summarizes the Bayes estimates for this collision. 221

Variable Table 4. Estimation Summary for Selected Pedestrian Collision Variables Initial Speed (km/h) Impact Speed (km/h) Reaction Time (sec) Ped Running Time (sec) Braking time (sec) Posterior Mean Posterior Stand. Dev. Posterior 2.5 percentile Posterior 97.5 Percentile 74.0 6.8 61.3 87.5 38.9 2.9 33.3 44.5 2.4 1.0 0.8 4.6 1.7 0.8 0.9 3.7 1.4 0.2 1.0 1.7 If we consider the pedestrian s first appearance as the time when the driver s reaction phase began, the mean estimated reaction time of about 2.4 seconds suggests a slightly, but not atypically, long reaction. This estimate is influenced, however, by our uncertainty concerning how long the pedestrian stood in the road before attempting to run across. If we compare the estimated time elapsing between braking and collision (about 1.4 seconds) to the estimated time the pedestrian spent running before collision (about 1.7 seconds) it appears that deceleration probably began shortly after the pedestrian starting running. This in turn suggests that the driver was probably attending to the pedestrian, and that it is difficult to maintain that inattention was a factor. CONCLUSION As indicted in the introduction, if inattention is a cause of road crashes, one ought to be able to find indirect evidence of its presence in at least some actual crashes. One obvious candidate for an indirect measure of inattention would be reaction times that appear to be atypically long. Of the four crashes considered here only one, the second freeway rear-end collision, showed clear evidence of an atypically long reaction time on the part of the colliding driver. In two other cases, however, inattention appeared to be present in other forms. This suggests that simple comparisons of reaction times for colliding and non-colliding drivers will not illuminate the full nature of the inattention issue. REFERENCES Davis, G., and Swenson, T. (2006). Collective responsibility for freeway rear-ending accidents? An application of probabilistic causal models. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 38, 728-736. Davis,G. (2007). On the plausibility of using simulation to model left-turn cross-path crashes. Paper 07-1019, Proceedings of the 86 th Annual Meeting of the Transportation Research Board, Washington, DC. 222

Fambro, D., Koppa, R., Picha, D., and Fitzpatrick, K. (1997). Determination of Stopping Sight Distance. NCHRP Report 400. Washington, DC: Transportation Research Board. Fricke, L. (1990). Traffic Accident Reconstruction. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Traffic Institute. Klauer, S., Dingus, T., Neale, V., Sudweeks, J., and Ramsey, D. (2006). The Impact of Driver Inattention on Near-Crash/Crash Risk: An Analysis Using the 100-Car Naturalistic Driving Study Data. Report DOT HS 810-594. Washington, DC: NHTSA. McLean, J., Anderson, R., Farmer, L., Lee, B., and Brooks, C. (1994). Vehicle Travel Speeds and the Incidence of Fatal Pedestrian Crashes. University of Adelaide: Road Accident Research Unit. Spiegelhalter, D., Thomas, A., Best, N., and Lunn, D. (2003). WinBUGS User Manual, Version 1.4. Cambridge, UK: MRC Biostatistics Unit, Cambridge University. Staplin, L. (1995). Simulator and field measures of driver age differences in left turn gap judgments. Transportation Research Record, 1485, 49-55. TRB. (2000). Highway Capacity Manual. Washington, DC: Transportation Research Board. 223