Recruitment in starved nests: the role of direct and indirect interactions between scouts and nestmates in the ant Lasius niger

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Recruitment in starved nests: the role of direct and indirect interactions between scouts and nestmates in the ant Lasius niger"

Transcription

1 Insect. Soc. (2011) 58: DOI /s Insectes Sociaux RESEARCH ARTICLE Recruitment in starved nests: the role of direct and indirect interactions between scouts and nestmates in the ant Lasius niger A.-C. Mailleux A. Buffin C. Detrain J.-L. Deneubourg Received: 16 November 2010 / Revised: 24 May 2011 / Accepted: 1 June 2011 / Published online: 21 June 2011 Ó International Union for the Study of Social Insects (IUSSI) 2011 Abstract In social insects, the foraging activity usually increases with the length of food deprivation. In Lasius niger, a mass-recruiting ant species, the foraging adjustment to the level of food deprivation is regulated by the scout that fed at the food source and by the response of the nestmates to signals performed by the scout inside the nest. In this study, we look at the role of these direct interactions (antennations or trophallaxes) and indirect interactions (pheromonal emission) and how they are influenced by the level of food deprivation. At the beginning of recruitment, the relative number of nestmates leaving the nest to forage increases with the level of deprivation. The nestmates propensity to exit the nest is not influenced by a previous trophallactic and/or antennal contact with a scout. Our results strongly suggest that the exit of nestmates is triggered by a chemical signal emitted by a scout. Deprivation lowers the response threshold of nestmates to this chemical signal resulting in a more important exit from the nest. Surprisingly, 27% of starved nestmates that receive food from the scout relay the information by depositing a chemical signal before having discovered and drunk the food source. Both phenomena boost the recruitment process. Though successful foragers returning to the nest have a significant role in the recruitment to the food source, we observed that the response of the nestmates inside the nest also greatly influence regulation of the foraging activity. A.-C. Mailleux (&) Unit of Ecology and Biogeography, Catholic University of Louvain, Croix du Sud 4-5 (Carnoy), 1348 Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium anne-catherine.mailleux@uclouvain.be A. Buffin C. Detrain J.-L. Deneubourg Service d Ecologie Sociale, Université libre de Bruxelles, CP231, Avenue F. D. Roosevelt, 50, 1050 Brussels, Belgium Keywords Ants Food transfer Trophallaxis Starvation Lasius niger Introduction Animals constantly face trade-off between their need for energy and the risk they take when foraging. Their ecological success depends on their ability to adjust their foraging strategies to the level of food deprivation. In societies, this problem is even more complex as the society s needs have to be met, not just the needs of one individual. Individuals thus have to exchange information and regulate their activity to the needs of the other members of the group/society (e.g. Gotceitas and Godin, 2004; Theraulaz et al., 1991; Cassill and Tschinkel, 1999a, b; Smiseth and Moore, 2004). In eusocial societies, the length of food deprivation is one of most important parameters affecting collective foraging behaviour, this has been observed in ants (Cosens and Toussaint, 1986; Roces and Hölldobler, 1996; Schafer et al., 2006; Sendova-Franks et al., 2010; Szlep-Fessel, 1970), honeybees (Schulz et al., 1998, 2002; Toth et al., 2005) and bumble bees (Cartar and Dill, 1990). For instance, in the honeybee Apis mellifera, after a short food deprivation period, the number of foragers searching for food does not markedly increase, with few foragers being engaged in food exploitation once a source is discovered (Toth et al., 2005). After a longer food deprivation period, colonies exhibit a higher number of foragers, a steeper recruitment of foragers towards exploited food sources, as well as higher amounts of food retrieved to the colony (Toth et al., 2005). The regulation of the recruitment in function of the level of food deprivation has been studied in species tending aphids such as Solenopsis invicta (Howard and Tschinkel, 1980), Pheidole pallidula (Hölldobler, 1971, 1985; Traniello, 1977;

2 560 A.-C. Mailleux et al. Baroni-Urbani et al. 1988; Roces and Nuñez, 1993; De Biseau and Pasteels, 1994; Stuart and Moffett, 1994; Liefke et al., 2001) and Lasius niger (Devigne and Detrain, 2002; Mailleux et al., 2003, 2006, 2010; Sakata, 1995; Völkl et al., 1999). These species of ants are good biological models to study the behavioural flexibility of colonies that face fluctuations in food supply. Indeed, honeydew produced by aphids is an important source of carbohydrates (Auclair, 1963; Hölldobler and Wilson, 1990; Kaplan and Eubanks, 2005) and its production undergoes important temporal variations throughout the year (Sakata, 1995; Sudd and Sudd, 1985; Völkl et al., 1999). In the beginning of spring, honeydew resources are limited since aphid s colonies are scarce. As the year goes by, some aphid colonies suddenly disappear (Devigne and Detrain, 2002), whereas others spread out and produce huge quantities of honeydew. Moreover, the weather (such as rain) can stop honeydew crop for a rather long time. Due to such fluctuations in honeydew availability, ant s species tending aphids can occasionally undergo long food deprivation time (Howard and Tschinkel, 1980). After a prolonged deprivation, these aphid-tending ants intensify their food retrieving activities (Howard and Tschinkel, 1980; Cosens and Toussaint, 1986; Josens and Roces, 2000; Mailleux et al., 2006; Wallis, 1962). These researches were mainly focused on the strategy of foraging followed by starved animals outside the nest (Stephens and Krebs, 1986) while the underlying behavioural mechanisms of communication and interactions inside the nest were less with some noticeable exceptions such as Solenopsis invicta. Inside the nest, foragers of S. invicta coming back from a food source use six discrete behaviours to inform nestmates of the location and quality of a food site (Cassill, 2003). They increase walking tempo, lay incoming trails, waggle their heads and push their way through the cluster of ants inside the entrance, stroke nestmates with their antennae, advertise with a brief food display and lead groups of nestmates to the food site by laying outgoing trails. In response, nestmates assess the food sample with taste buds on the tips of their antennae. The motivation of the recruit depends on the quality of food advertised by the foragers, its employment status (nurse versus unemployed) and its level of hunger. Therefore, in the S. invicta, recruitment is an emergent event based on supply and demand decisions that depends on direct contact inside the nest rather than on a pheromone trail or at the food site. The influence of deprivation on foraging activity is linked to the increased trophallactic activity of the recruiters (Howard and Tschinkel, 1980). In L. niger, the regulation of the recruitment behaviour in the nest, in function of the food deprivation is not well known and deserves further investigations. In L. niger, the first foragers finding a liquid food source outside the nest drink and come back to the nest (Beckers et al., 1993; Mailleux et al., 2003, 2006) while laying a trail from the food source to the nest (Mailleux et al., 2003, 2006). The ants coming from starved nests bring larger quantities of food back to the nest. Remarkably, food deprivation does not alter the quantity of recruitment pheromone laid on the substrate by L. niger foragers on their homeward trip. This means that the increased global recruitment after a period of food deprivation does not result from an increased intensity of individual trail marking by foragers (Mailleux et al., 2006). Contrary to S. invicta, after long deprivation periods, higher mobilisation is essentially based on the increased motivation of the recruits to leave the nest and forage (Mailleux et al., 2010). This leaves the question open on how this motivation of the recruits is increased. This paper is the continuation of previous studies (Mailleux et al., 2003, 2006, 2010). We followed some foragers that found the food source, drank from it and came back to the nest whilst laying a recruitment trail inside the nest. We observed their behaviour when they were confronted with nestmates in colonies subjected to different levels of food deprivation. One can imagine that interindividual direct interactions such as antennations or trophallaxes and/or indirect interactions such as pheromonal emission may differ depending on deprivation and may influence recruits motivation (Cassill and Tschinkel, 1999a, b; Mc Cabe et al., 2006; Farina and Grüter, 2009). This should result from a more rapid trophallactic activity, an increased stimulation mediated by antennal contact and/or by an increased emission of pheromones, all those hypotheses are not mutually exclusive. In this paper, we measured and compared, for different levels of food deprivation, how the trophallactic and antennal contacts between L. niger foragers and nestmates inside nests influence nest departure. These observations were made at the very beginning of recruitment, a crucial phase in a process following autocatalytic dynamics. Methods Mature queenless colonies (n = 3) of Lasius niger were collected from the slopes of earth banks in Brussels. They contained around 1,100 workers (range 800 1,400 workers) and were reared in plaster nests ( cm). As a first step in understanding the colony s behaviour to diminish the risks of inter-colony heterogeneities due to heterogeneous development of the brood, our colonies contained no larvae or brood. Each colony was subdivided into three connected sections ( cm) covered by a glass plate and placed in plastic arenas ( cm), the borders of which were coated with Fluon Ò. Fluon is a colloidal solution of Teflon Ò that creates a very smooth

3 Recruitment in starved nests: the role of direct and indirect interactions 561 surface that prevents ants from escaping from the arena. These nests were closed, except for the entrance (1 cm wide) located in the middle of the longer front side of the nest. The nests were regularly moistened and kept at a temperature of 22 ± 3 C with a 12 h photoperiod. The ants were fed thrice a week with brown sugar solution (0.6 M), cockroaches (Periplaneta americana) and maggots (Calliphora erythrocephala). The influence of the length of food deprivation on the behaviour of foragers and nestmates was studied at the beginning of food recruitment. We focused our observations on the first 20 min of food exploitation since this period greatly influences further collective behaviours that may rapidly snowball. We analysed the response of ants to 3 ll droplets of sucrose solution (0.6 M), which exceeded crop capacity (Mailleux et al., 2003). The concentration and volume of these experimental droplets were similar to those of the honeydew droplets produced by aphids (Auclair, 1963). During one experiment, the nests were connected by a cardboard bridge (length 20 cm, width 0.5 cm) for 1 h to a small foraging area (6 9 6 cm) that the ants were able to freely explore. After the exploration phase, a food source was placed in the centre of the foraging area. The first forager that discovered the food droplet returned to the nest. It was called Scout (S, see below) and we quantified its behaviour inside the nest. Once this scout had contacted some nestmates and left the nest, it was gently removed and the foraging area was also removed. In doing so, we prevented other ants from reaching the food and limited our study to the behaviour of only one scout and its contact with its nestmates. For each experiment, we video recorded the nest for 20 min (magnification 29), starting when the observed ant entered the nest. Overall, we observed 28, 25 and 26 scouts after 1, 4 and 8 days of food deprivation (called 1 dd,4 dd and 8 dd in the text, respectively). Each food deprivation level is imposed on each colony in a random order. After a food deprivation period, the colonies were fed ad libitum during a resting period of 7 days before another food deprivation period. The maximum food deprivation period (8 days) did not induce abnormal mortality (Lenoir, 1979). We studied all the possible interactions between scouts and nestmates: antennal, trophallactic contacts and/or pheromonal emission. A trophallactic contact (Wheeler, 1910) is the transfer of fluids between members of a society through mouth-to-mouth feeding. We considered different behavioural groups, as detailed in Table 1. Nestmates were defined according to the type of contact (antennal or trophallactic) they made with the scout. Some nestmates made several antennal contacts and/or trophallactic contacts and/or a mixture of both. The nestmates that made one or more trophallactic contacts with the scout, regardless of the antennal contacts they had with the scout, were classified a tc (for trophallactic contact). Table 1 defines behavioural groups we studied and the abbreviation we used to refer to them. The other nestmates that only made antennal contact with the scouts were classified as ac (antennal contact). As the results concerning the Ne, workers that were close to the entrance in control condition i.e. when no scout entered the nest were already published (Mailleux et al., 2010), we did not detail them here but we used them for comparison. The following parameters were measured: 1. The proportion that left the nest during a period of 20 min after the entrance of the scout. This was calculated for each food deprivation period for each ant Table 1 Description of the different behavioural groups S Scout Individual that explored a new foraging area, discovered food and returned to the nest while laying a recruitment trail. Scouts were tested only once. After their visit to the nest, they were kindly removed from the nest ac tc NC Ne Nestmate having an antennal contact with S Nestmate having an trophallactic contact with S Nestmate having no contact with S (scout having access to food) Nestmate having no contact with S (no food) Nestmate having an antennal contact with the scout inside the nest. Nestmates having antennal contact with an ac worker were called a? ac (a for antennal, C for contact) Nestmate having a trophallactic contact with the scout inside the nest and received sugar solution regurgitated by the returning fed scout. Nestmates having a secondary trophallactic contact with the tc worker were called t? tc.) Nestmates having an antennal contact with the tc worker were called (a? tc). Nestmate that stood motionless at 1 cm (±0.3 cm) from the entrance. It was not in direct contact with the scout although it could potentially perceive pheromones emitted by the scout. In these conditions, the departure of the NC from the nest was considered to be spontaneous and/or triggered by the emission of chemical signals emitted by the scout (Mailleux et al., 2010). We did not take account all ants close to the scout but chose one worker NC per scout. Nestmate chosen with similar criteria as NC (individuals situated around 1 cm from the entrance, motionless), but observed during the exploration phase when no food was offered and no chemical trail was laid. They were considered as control ants i.e. ants that left the nest spontaneously without being stimulated either by a contact or by a pheromone.

4 562 A.-C. Mailleux et al. category. For example, in all experiments, we counted the number of ac observed after 1 day of deprivation that left the nest within the 20 min period and divided this by the total number of ac observed after 1 day of deprivation. This was calculated for all behavioural groups (S, ac, tc, a? ac, t? tc, a? tc, NC, Ne) (Table 1). 2. The time to leave the nest. For the scouts (S), the time to leave the nest started once the last contact ended and lasted until the scout left the nest. For tc and ac, the time to leave the nest started at the end of the last contact with the scout and ended when the nestmates left the nest. For NC, the time to leave the nest began when a scout reached the vicinity of the NC nestmate (around 1 cm) and ended when the NC left the nest. This was calculated for all experiments and for each behavioural group of ants (S, ac, tc, a? ac, t? tc, a? tc, NC). 3. The duration and number of antennal or trophallactic contacts. For S, the total contact time summed the time one scout spent in antennal and/or trophallactic contact with other nestmates. The number of contacts was the total number of contacts one scout had before leaving the nest. For tc, the total contact time and the number of contacts were measured during the contacts between tc and all of the other nestmates. The total contact time was the sum of the time spent in contacts (antennal and/or trophallactic) by one tc with the other nestmates. Similarly, for ac, we measured the same two parameters, although since ac did not receive sugar solution from the scout, all acs only had antennal contact with the other nestmates. The parameters (total contact time, number of contacts, time to leave the nest) were measured only for ants that left the nest within 20 min after the entrance of the scout. Indeed, preliminary observations showed that ants that did not leave the nest within 20 min joined an aggregate of ants situated deep within the nest and stopped moving. Hence, we considered that these ants were not involved in the initiation of foraging and we did not measure their time to leave the nest nor the contacts they had with their nestmates. 4. The trail-laying behaviour of the scout. When laying trail pheromones a worker bends its gaster down towards the substrate (the normal position being nearly horizontal), interrupts its walk for a fraction of a second and backs up to amplify the movement of its gaster. This behaviour was taken as the criteria for trail laying (Beckers et al., 1993). As ants were observed in a low-angle shot, it was not possible to precisely quantify the trail-laying behaviour (Mailleux et al., 2003), but it was possible to qualify the ants as trail-layers or non-trail layers. The behavioural parameters of L. niger foragers and nestmates subjected to different levels of food deprivation were compared using GraphPad Instat version 3.05 for Win95/NT (GraphPad Software, 1998, Inc., San Diego, California, USA). Given that most data were not normally distributed, we used non-parametric statistics to compare behavioural parameters as a function of food deprivation period. All behavioural parameters were compared in function of the colony and showed no colony effects. We used the Kruskal Wallis test with Dunn s post hoc test to compare the time parameters (time to leave the nest, total contact time) and the numbers of contacts (trophallactic or antennal) as a function of the deprivation period. We used chi-square tests to compare the proportion of ants leaving the nest versus the one not leaving the nest. Results Scout (S) Regardless of the length of the food deprivation period, all scouts returned to the nest, unloaded their sugar freight and left the nest within 5 min. When food deprivation was prolonged, the time to leave the nest significantly decreased (Table 2). When food deprivation was prolonged, the total number of contacts between by scouts and nestmates significantly decreased (from five to two antennal contacts) while the number of trophallactic contacts remained unchanged (Table 3). Indeed, no matter the food deprivation period, a scout performed about two trophallaxis and spent about 33 s in each of them; 1 dd : 28 ± 20 s (median ± 25th percentile, t rophallaxes = 52); 4 dd : 39 ± 27 s (n t = 45); 8 dd : 35 ± 25 s (n t = 43) (Kruskal Wallis test, KW = 4.72, ). We observed that 91% of the scouts laid at least one chemical spot of trail pheromone near the nest entrance when they returned from the source (after 1 dd ; 85% (n = 28) after 4 dd ; 91% (n = 25) after 8 dd, 96% (n = 26), v 2 test: v = 2.2, p = 0.33). Ants that had antennal contact with a scout (ac) and ants that had antennal contact with ac (a? ac) The proportion of ac that left the nest within the 20 min observation period significantly increased from 0.2 to 0.55 as food deprivation increased (Fig. 1, v 2 test: v = 37.1, p \ 0.001). Apart from this, the other behavioural para-

5 Recruitment in starved nests: the role of direct and indirect interactions 563 Table 2 Comparison between the time to leave the nest and the total time of contact for the different ant groups after three food deprivation periods i.e. the scouts (S), nestmates having antennal contact with the scouts (ac) and nestmates having contact with these ac (a? ac), nestmates having trophallactic contact with the scout (tc), nestmates having trophallactic contact with tc (t? tc) or antennal contact with tc (a? tc) S ac tc a? ac a? tc t? tc Time to leave the nest (in s) Total time of contact (in s) 1 dd 125 ± 88 [28] 58 ± 27 [26] 314 ± 120 [11] 52 ± 12 [21] 42 ± 31 [9] 361 ± 73[2] 4 dd 94 ± 77 [25] a 26 ± 18 [25] 334 ± 188 [27] 45 ± 12 [23] 48 ± 27 [13] 187 ± 36[8] 8 dd 81 ± 61 [26] a 105 ± 34 [12] 368 ± 115 [25] 47 ± 21 [38] 45 ± 18 [29] 179 ± 102[8] KW = 9.03, p \ 0.01 KW = 4.69, KW = 4.81, KW = 0.91, KW = 0.82, KW = 0.09, 1 dd 80 ± 53 [28] 1 ± 1 [26] 130 ± 42 [11] 1 ± 1 [21] 3 ± 1 [9] 121 ± 11 [2] 4 dd 90 ± 63 [25] 1 ± 1 [25] 154 ± 41 [24] 1 ± 1 [23] 1 ± 1 [13] 47 ± 20 [8] 8 dd 61 ± 46 [26] 1 ± 1 [12] 80 ± 45 [25] 1 ± 1 [38] 1 ± 1 [29] 35 ± 30 [8] KW = 4.27 KW = 0.25 KW = 4.92 KW = 5.57 KW = 5.20 KW = 0.24 Median and 25% percentile are given for all parameters. Numbers in brackets indicate sample size. All parameters were compared using the Kruskal Wallis test. Values of p and the results of the statistical tests (: not significant) are given in the last row for each parameter. When a group of data was considered as statistically different in function of the deprivation period, we made a post hoc Dunn test at the 0.05 level of significance; In this case, groups sharing the same letter were not statistically different. The parameters were measured only for ants that left the nest within 20 min after the entrance of the scout Table 3 Comparison between the number of antennation and trophallaxis for the different ant groups after three food deprivation periods i.e. the scouts (S), nestmates having antennal contact with the scouts (ac), nestmates having trophallactic contact with the scout (tc) S ac tc Number of antennal contacts 1 dd 5 ± 3 [28] 1 ± 0 [26] 9 ± 7 [15] 4 dd 3 ± 2 [25] a 1 ± 0 [25] 11 ± 4 [24] 8 dd 2 ± 1 [26] a 3 ± 1 [12] 9 ± 8 [25] KW = 14.38, p \ KW = 0.16, KW = 0.36, Number of trophallaxes 1 dd 2 ± 2 [28] 0 ± 0 [26] 1 ± 1 [15] 4 dd 2 ± 1 [25] 0 ± 0 [24] 2 ± 1 [24] 8 dd 2 ± 1 [26] 0 ± 0 [12] 2 ± 2 [25] KW = 2.66, KW = 1.42, KW = 4.40, For S, these parameters concerned the contacts between S and C. For ac (or for tc), these parameters concerned the contacts between ac (or tc) and all the other nestmates that ac (or tc) met in the nest. Median and 25th percentile are given for all parameters. Numbers in brackets indicate sample size. All parameters were compared using the Kruskal Wallis test. Values of p and the results of the statistical tests (n.s: not significant) are given in the last row for each parameter. When a group of data was considered as statistically different in function of the deprivation period, we made a post hoc Dunn test at the 0.05 level of significance; In this case, groups sharing the same letter were not statistically different. The parameters were measured only for ants that left the nest within 20 min after the entrance of the scout meters (time to leave the nest, number and duration of contacts with other nestmates) did not differ with food deprivation levels (Tables 2 and 3). No matter how long the food deprivation period was, the acs that left the nest did so within 44 ± 25 s (n = 63) (Table 2) and, before leaving, these ac had antennal contacts with around two nestmates (a? ac individuals) for a total duration of around 1 s (n = 63, Table 2). The proportion of these a? ac leaving the nest significantly increased with food deprivation (Fig. 1, v 2 test: v = 19.95, p \ 0.001). Before leaving, a? ac ants (ants that had antennal contact with nestmates that had previously come in contact with the scout) for a total time of 1 s ± 1s (n = 81). This time was similar to the contact time between S and ac (Kruskal Wallis test, KW = 6.72, ). Ants that had trophallactic contact with a scout (tc), ants that had antennal contact with tc (a? tc) and that had trophallactic contact with tc (t? tc) The proportion of tc ants that left the nest after having exchanged food with scouts significantly increased from 0.2 to 0.6 with food deprivation (Fig. 1, v 2 test: v = 21.1, p \ 0.001). However, the percentage of those tc ants that engaged themselves into a secondary trophallaxis with other nestmates did not increase with food deprivation

6 564 A.-C. Mailleux et al. Proportions that left the nest after the entrance of the scout a 129 b 35 bc 22 a 56 be bd cdef 39 bg 37 chi 66 fij 121 afh a 168 (Table 3). The total time spent doing trophallaxis was not influenced by food deprivation (Table 2). Among the starved tc ants, 25% (n = 24) and 32% (n = 25) (after 4 dd and 8 dd, respectively) left the nest after laying chemical marks near the nest entrance. This percentage of ants that laid a trace was not statistically significant for 4 dd and 8 dd (Fisher s test, D = 0.07, p [ 0.05). This behaviour was not observed after 1 dd (n = 28) and it was not observed among ac nestmates (n = 63). The proportion of t? tc ants that received a secondary trophallaxis and left the nest increased with food deprivation (v 2 test: v = 10.99, p \ 0.001). These t? tc individuals left the nest in a similar proportion to tc ants having received food directly from the scout at least after 1 dd and 4 dd (v 2 test: v = 0.03 and 1.71, respectively, ). However, the proportion of departing t? tc was lower than that of tc after 8 dd (v = 5.89, p \ 0.05). The proportion of a? tc ants that left the nest after antennal contact with tc increased with the length of food deprivation (v 2 test: v = 31.1, p \ 0.001). Before leaving, a? tc had antennal contacts for a total time of 1 ± 1s (n = 63). In fact, concerning the time to leave the nest, none of the ants that had antennal contact (ac, a? ac, a? tc) b 35 acj 9 acdegj 20 ac tc NC a ac a tc t tc Behavioural groups 1 deprivation day 4 deprivation days 8 deprivation days Fig. 1 Proportions of ants that left the nest within the first 20 min in function of the food deprivation level (1, 4 and 8 food deprivation days). The behavioural groups were all composed of nestmates: (ac) nestmates having antennal contact with the scouts (a? ac) nestmates having contact with ac (tc) nestmates having trophallactic contact with the scout (t? tc) nestmates having trophallactic contact with tc or (a? tc) nestmates having antennal contact with tc and (NC) were the ones having no contact with the scout. The numbers on top of the bars were the number of ants in each group. Proportions were compared with v 2 test: groups with the same letter were not significantly different. All proportions after 1 food deprivation day differed from the proportion after 4 and 8 food deprivations days except in the group a? tc acj 27 were statistically different (Kruskal Wallis test, KW = ). Ants that had no contact with a scout (NC) When food deprivation was prolonged, the proportion of non-contacted ants (NC) that left the nest increased statistically from 0.38 to 0.72 (Fig. 1, v 2 test: v = 9.39, p \ 0.01). The time to leave the nest (s) decreased significantly with food deprivation (1 dd : 42 ± 35, n = 15; 4 dd : 12 ± 3, n = 24; 8 dd : 10 ± 1, n = 25, KW = 10.64, p \ 0.005). The NCs were closer to the nest entrance and did not receive food from the scouts. These ants did not spend time in contacts. This resulted in a shorter time to leave the nest than the other behavioural groups. It must be noted that the proportion of ants that left the nest was similarly influenced by length of food deprivation for NC, ac and tc after 4 dd and 8 dd. Several NC departed from the nest without having had contact with the scout or any other nestmates. Was the exit of these NC ants spontaneous or triggered by the emission of chemical signals by the scout? We compared the proportions of NC and Ne that left the nest. Ne were considered a control group as they were chosen with similar criteria as NC (Table 1) but observed during the exploration phase when no food was offered and no trail laid. No matter what the food deprivation period was, the proportion of NC that left the nest was higher than the proportion of Ne (these last results were presented in Mailleux et al., 2010, v 2 test: after 1 dd, v = 4.8, p = 0.03; 4 dd, v = 8.9, p \ 0.005; 8 dd, v = 9.3, p \ 0.005). Comparison between ants that had antennal contact with the scout (ac) or trophallactic contact with the scout (tc) When the food deprivation period was prolonged, the proportion of ants that left the nest increased for both groups of nestmates ac and tc: 1 dd : 0.20 (n = 185); 4 dd : 0.52 (n = 82); 8 dd : 0.58 (n = 64) (v 2 test: v = 43.43, p \ 0.001) (Fig. 1). For equal food deprivation periods, the proportion that left the nest was similar for tc and ac suggesting that food intake did not facilitate nor prevent the exit of recruited nestmates. The time to leave the nest, the duration of contact and the number of contacts were always higher for tc workers than for ac as tc spent time making trophallaxes with some t? tc to unload their crop. When the food deprivation period was prolonged, scouts had fewer antennal contacts with ac and these nestmates departed more frequently. Therefore, the number of exiting ac per scout did not change with food deprivation: 1 dd : 1.0 ± 0.0, n = 28; 4 dd : 1.0 ± 0.0, n = 25; 8 dd : 1.0 ± 0.3, n = 26, KW = 1.89, ).

7 Recruitment in starved nests: the role of direct and indirect interactions 565 Comparison between ants that had no contact with the scout (NC), ants that had a? ac, a? tc, t? tc) and the control group (Ne) No matter how long the food deprivation period was, the proportion of a? ac that left the nest was similar to the Ne group (the control group) (v = 0.73, 0.14, 0.75 after 1 dd,4 d, and 8 dd, respectively, for all). Similarly, the proportion of t? tc that left the nest was similar to that of the Ne group (the control group) (v = 0.17, 0.47, 0.41 after 1 dd,4 d, and 8 dd, respectively, for all). This suggests that the exit of a? ac and t? tc was spontaneous as it occurred at similar rates than spontaneous departure of ants in control conditions (Ne). Discussion This study of social interactions between ant foragers and nestmates has shed light on the mechanisms that increase recruitment in colonies that are highly deprived of food. It is known that L. niger scouts that discovered a food source deposited a chemical signal that stimulated the departure of nestmates located near the nest entrance (Beckers et al., 1993; Mailleux et al., 2006, 2010). Here, we show that the proportions of nestmates that left the nest increase similarly with the level of food deprivation, independently of any direct contact with a scout. Thus, the signal delivered by the scout that triggers the exit of recruits must be chemical and is probably the same pheromone trail as the one laid when scouts come back to the nest. When the colony is food deprived, this signal is more effective although not stronger (Mailleux et al., 2010). Our results strongly suggest that food deprivation lowers the response threshold of the nestmates to this signal resulting in a more important exit from the nest (de Biseau and Pasteels, 2000; Mailleux et al., 2006, 2010). It is noticeable that nestmates that received food from scouts laid a chemical mark on the nest substrate. Indeed, 27% of food deprived nestmates that received food through trophallaxis increased the arousal state of workers by depositing chemical marks before leaving the nest. This phenomenon has never been evidenced before. It is commonly thought that an ant must get in contact with the source to lay a trail. This unexpected behaviour is not observed after short food deprivation (1 day) and it was not observed for nestmates that only had antennal contacts with scouts. This phenomenon may contribute to the amplification process of recruitment when the colony is food deprived. Here, we observe that starved scouts take less time to leave the nest. This time-saving is not responsible for steeper recruitment after long food deprivation. Indeed, a starved scout drinks for a longer time at the food source (Mailleux et al., 2006). This increased drinking time counterbalances the decrease in the time taken to leave the nest observed in the present study. As a result, the duration of foraging cycles (i.e. the time between two visits to the food source) is similar whatever the food deprivation period: it lasts around 337 s after 1 dd ; 327 after 4 dd and 312 after 8 dd. Therefore, the influence of food deprivation level on the collective response is not mediated by differences in the time to upload food at the source and to share this load with its nestmates. Previous studies have evidenced the major role of trophallaxes in the regulation of foraging in social insects (e.g. in fire ants Cassill and Tschinkel, 1999b; Cassill, 2003). In honeybees and ants, trophallaxes govern the dynamics of forager s departure from the nest: a forager has to unload the food it ingested at the source before leaving for another trip (Anderson and Ratnieks, 1999; Buffin et al., 2009; Hart and Ratnieks, 2001). The availability of the receivers acts as negative feedback: if no recruit accepts the recruiter s load, the latter remains inside the nest and the exploitation of the food source will diminish and finally stop. Here, we show that L. niger scouts must also unload their freight before leaving the nest but that the number of trophallaxes remains unchanged whatever the level of food deprivation. However, food deprivation facilitates the unloading of food by the scout towards nestmates. Indeed, in L. niger, after a short food deprivation period, nestmates rarely accept trophallaxis, which forces scouts to contact a greater number of nestmates before unloading. After long food deprivation periods, scouts more easily find motivated nestmates to accept their sugar load (Mailleux et al., 2010). Trophallaxes seem to delay the departure of the L. niger. Ants that received sugar solution (directly from the scout or indirectly during a secondary trophallaxis) take more than 3 min to leave the nest. Therefore, food transfer between ants increases the dynamics of the first steps of the recruitment process in such a mass foraging ant. Our study of workers behaviour inside the nest confirms the role of chemical cues in the regulation of the foraging activity but also highlights the importance of nestmates motivation. Indeed, the foraging activity of the colony depends not only on the recruiting signals but also on the state of the individuals that perceive it (here below a certain threshold of hunger). Not only does the behaviour of the recruiting scouts change according to the food source (Wilson, 1962; Nonacs and Dill, 1990, De Marco, 2006) but the response of potential recruits will depend on their own needs. Worker s need at the individual level reflects the colony s need namely in carbohydrate, used as fuel, or in proteins, necessary for brood development (Seeley, 1989; Cassill and Tschinkel, 1999a; Portha et al., 2002; Dussutour and Simpson, 2009; Cook et al., 2010). This highlights the

8 566 A.-C. Mailleux et al. central role of nestmates inside the nest in the fine tuning of food exploitation to best fit energy costs and benefits related to foraging activity. Acknowledgments A. Buffin is a F.R.I.A. researcher (Formation pour la Recherche dans l Industrie et dans l Agriculture). C. Detrain and J.L. Deneubourg are research associates from the Belgian National Fund for Scientific Research and were financially supported by the Fund for Joint Basic Research (Grant ). A.C. Mailleux is financially supported by the Institut d encouragement de la Recherche Scientifique et de l Innovation de Bruxelles. This is publication BRC158 of the Biodiversity Research Centre at Université Catholique de Louvain. References Anderson C. and Ratnieks F.L.W Task partitioning in insect societies. I. Effect of colony size on queueing delay and colony ergonomic efficiency. Am. Nat. 154: Auclair J.L Aphid feeding and nutrition. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 8: Beckers R., Deneubourg J.-L. and Goss S Modulation of traillaying in the ant Lasius niger (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) and its role in the collective selection of a food source. J. Insect Behav. 6: Baroni-Urbani C., Buser M.W. and Schilliger E Substrate vibration during recruitment in ant social organization. Insect. Soc. 35: Buffin A., Denis D., Van Simaeys G., Goldman S. and Deneubourg J.- L Feeding and stocking up: radio-labelled food reveals exchange patterns in ants. Plos One 4: e5919 Cartar R.V. and Dill L.M Why are bumblebees risk sensitive foragers? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 26: Cassill D.L Rules of supply and demand regulate recruitment to food in the fire ant, Solenopsis invicta. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 54: Cassill D.L. and Tschinkel W.R. 1999a. Information flow during social feeding in ant societies. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 29: Cassill D.L. and Tschinkel W.R. 1999b. Regulation of diet in the fire ant, Solenopsis invicta. J. Insect Behav. 12: Cook S.C., Eubanks M.D., Gold R.E. and Behmer S.T Colonylevel macronutrient regulation in ants: mechanisms, hoarding and associated costs. Anim. Behav. 79: Cosens D. and Toussaint N The dynamic nature of the activities of the wood ant Formica aquilonia foraging to static food resource within a laboratory habitat. Physiol. Entomol. 11: Devigne C. and Detrain C Collective exploration and area making in the ant Lasius niger. Insect. Soc. 49: De Biseau J.C. and Pasteels J.M Regulated food recruitment through individual behavior of scouts in the ant, Myrmica sabuleti (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). J. Insect Behav. 7: De Biseau J.C. and Pasteels J.M Response thresholds to recruitment signals and the regulation of foraging intensity in the ant Myrmica sabuleti (Hymenoptera, Formicidae). Behav. Proc. 48: De Marco R.J How bees tune their dancing according to their colony s nectar influx: re-examining the role of the foodreceivers eagerness. J. Exp. Biol. 209: Dussutour A. and Simpson S.J Communal nutrition in ants. Curr. Biol. 19: Farina W.M. and Grüter C Trophallaxis a mechanism of information transfer. In: Food Exploitation by Social Insects: Ecological, Behavioral, and Theoretical Approaches (Hrncir M. and Jarau S., Eds). Boca Raton, CRC Press. pp Gotceitas V. and Godin J.-G Foraging under the risk of predation in juvenile Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar L.): effects of social status and hunger. Anim. Behav. 67: Hart A.G. and Ratnieks F.L.W Task partitioning, division of labour and nest compartmentalisation collectively isolate hazardous waste in the leafcutting ant Atta cephalotes. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 49: Hölldobler B Recruitment behavior in Camponotus socius (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Z. Vergl. Physiol. 75: -142 Hölldobler B Liquid food transmission and antennation signals in ponerine ants. Isr. J. Entomol. 14: Hölldobler B. and Wilson E.O The Ants. Harvard Univ. Press Cambridge, MA. 732 pp Howard D.F. and Tschinkel W.R The effect of colony size and deprivation on food flow in the fire ant Solenopsis invicta (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 7: Josens R.B. and Roces F Foraging in the ant Camponotus mus: nectar-intake and crop filling depend on colony deprivation. J. Insect Physiol. 46: Kaplan I. and Eubanks M Aphids alter the community-wide impact of fire ants. Ecology 86: Lenoir A Le comportement alimentaire et la division du travail chez la fourmi Lasius niger. Bull. Biol. Fr. Belg. 113: Liefke C., Hölldober B. and Maschwitz U Recruitment behavior in the ant genus Polyrhachis (Hymenoptera, Formicidae). J. Insect Behav. 14: Mc Cabe S., Farina W.M. and Josens R.B Antennation of nectar-receivers encodes colony needs and food-source profitability in the ant Camponotus mus. Insect. Soc. 53: Mailleux A.-C., Detrain C. and Deneubourg J.-L Regulation of ants foraging to resource productivity. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 270: Mailleux A.-C., Detrain C. and Deneubourg J.-L Deprivation drives a threshold triggering communication. J. Exp. Biol. 209: Mailleux A.-C., Detrain C. and Deneubourg J.-L Impact of starvation on Lasius niger exploration. Ethology 116: Nonacs P. and Dill L.M Mortality risk vs food quality trade-offs in a common currency: ant patch preferences. Ecology 71: Portha S., Deneubourg J.-L. and Detrain C Self-organized asymmetries in ant foraging: a functional response to food type and colony needs. Behav. Ecol. 13: Roces F. and Nuñez J.A Information about food quality influences load-size selection in recruited leaf-cutting ants. Anim. Behav. 45: Roces F. and Hölldobler B Use of stridulation in foraging leaf cutting ants: Mechanical support during cutting or short range recruitment signal? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 39: Sakata H Density-dependant predation of the ant Lasius niger (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) on two attended aphids Lachnus tropicallis and Myzocallis kuricola (Homoptera: Aphididae). Res. Popul. Ecol. 37: Schafer R.J., Holmes S. and Gordon D.M Forager activation and food availability in harvester ants. Anim. Behav. 71: Schulz D.J., Huang Z.Y. and Robinson G.E Effects of colony food shortage on behavioural development in honeybees. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 42: Schulz D.J., Vermiglio M.J., Huang Z.Y. and Robinson G.E Effects of colony food shortage on social action in honeybees. Insect. Soc. 49: Seeley T.D Social foraging in honey bees: how nectar foragers assess their colony s nutritional status. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 24: Sendova-Franks A.B., Hayward R.K., Wulf B., Klimek T., James R., Planque R., Britton N.F. and Franks N.R Emergency

9 Recruitment in starved nests: the role of direct and indirect interactions 567 networking: famine relief in ant colonies. Anim. Behav. 79: Smiseth P.T. and Moore A.J Signaling of hunger when offspring forage by both begging and self-feeding. Anim. Behav. 67: Stephens D.W. and Krebs J.R Foraging Theory. Princeton University Press, New Jersey, USA. 247 pp Stuart R.J. and Moffett M.W Recruitment communication and pheromone trails in the Neotropical ants, Leptothorax (Nesomyrmex) spininodis and L (N.). echinatinodis. Experientia 50: Szlep-Fessel R The regulatory mechanism in mass foraging and the recruitment of soldiers Pheidole. Insect. Soc. 17: Sudd J.H. and Sudd M.E Seasonal changes in the response of wood-ants (Formica lugubris) to sucrose baits. Ecol. Entomol. 10: Traniello J.F Resource assessment, recruitment behavior,and organization of cooperative prey retrieval in the ant Formica schaufussi (Hymenoptera). J. Insect Behav. 11: 1-22 Theraulaz G., Gervet J. and Semenoff Tian-Chanski S Social regulation of foraging activities in Polistes dominulus Christ: a systemic approach to behavioural organization. Behaviour 116: Toth A.L., Kantarovitch S., Meisel A.F. and Robinson G.E Nutritional status influences socially regulated foraging ontogeny in honey bees. J. Exp. Biol. 208: Völkl W., Woodring J., Fisher M., Lorenz M.W. and Hoffmann K.H Ant-aphid mutualisms: the impact of honeydew production and honeydew sugar composition on ant preferences. Oecologia 118: Wallis D.I The relation between hunger, activity and worker function in an ant colony. Proc. Zool. Soc. Lond. B 139: Wheeler W.M The Ants. Their Structure, Development and Behaviour. New York: Columbia University Press. 663 pp Wilson E.O Chemical communication among workers of the fire ant Solenopsis saevissima (Fr. Smith) 1. The organization of mass-foraging. Anim. Behav. 10:

How does starvation affect spatial organization within nests in Lasius niger?

How does starvation affect spatial organization within nests in Lasius niger? Insect. Soc. (2011) 58:219 225 DOI 10.1007/s00040-010-0139-5 Insectes Sociaux RESEARCH ARTICLE How does starvation affect spatial organization within nests in Lasius niger? A.-C. Mailleux G. Sempo S. Depickère

More information

Complexity of Environment and Parsimony of Decision Rules in Insect Societies

Complexity of Environment and Parsimony of Decision Rules in Insect Societies Reference: Biol. Bull. 202: 268 274. (June 2002) Complexity of Environment and Parsimony of Decision Rules in Insect Societies CLAIRE DETRAIN* AND JEAN-LOUIS DENEUBOURG Unit of Social Ecology (CP 160/12),

More information

Food transport in ants: Do Lasius niger foragers maximize their individual load?

Food transport in ants: Do Lasius niger foragers maximize their individual load? C. R. Biologies 332 (2009) 500 506 http://france.elsevier.com/direct/crass3/ Ethology / Éthologie Food transport in ants: Do Lasius niger foragers maximize their individual load? Anne-Catherine Mailleux,

More information

Decision making in ant foragers (Lasius niger) facing conflicting private and social information

Decision making in ant foragers (Lasius niger) facing conflicting private and social information Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2011) 65:141 148 DOI 10.1007/s00265-010-1020-2 ORIGINAL PAPER Decision making in ant foragers (Lasius niger) facing conflicting private and social information Christoph Grüter & Tomer

More information

HOW DO ANTS AND SOCIAL CATERPILLARS

HOW DO ANTS AND SOCIAL CATERPILLARS HOW DO ANTS AND SOCIAL CATERPILLARS COLLECTIVELY MAKE DECISIONS? 1 Introduction In social insects, collective decisions occur when a colony is faced with several opportunities, for example food sources

More information

SC741 W12: Division of Labor Part I: Fixed- and Variable- Threshold Algorithms

SC741 W12: Division of Labor Part I: Fixed- and Variable- Threshold Algorithms SC741 W12: Division of Labor Part I: Fixed- and Variable- Threshold Algorithms Outline Division of labor in natural systems Ants Bees, wasps Models and mechanisms Fixed-threshold mechanisms Variable-threshold

More information

Task Performance of Midden Workers of Atta sexdens rubropilosa Forel (Hymenoptera: Formicidae)

Task Performance of Midden Workers of Atta sexdens rubropilosa Forel (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) J Insect Behav (2013) 26:873 880 DOI 10.1007/s10905-013-9403-7 Task Performance of Midden Workers of Atta sexdens rubropilosa Forel (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) Fabrícia G. Lacerda & Terezinha M. C. Della

More information

Effects of colony-level attributes on larval feeding in the fire ant, Solenopsis invicta

Effects of colony-level attributes on larval feeding in the fire ant, Solenopsis invicta Insectes soc. 46 (1999) 261 266 0020-1812/99/030261-06 $ 1.50+0.20/0 Birkhäuser Verlag, Basel, 1999 Insectes Sociaux Research article Effects of colony-level attributes on larval feeding in the fire ant,

More information

Effects of Colony Composition and Food Type on Nutrient Distribution in Colonies of Monomorium orientale (Hymenoptera: Formicidae)

Effects of Colony Composition and Food Type on Nutrient Distribution in Colonies of Monomorium orientale (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) HOUSEHOLD AND STRUCTURAL INSECTS Effects of Colony Composition and Food Type on Nutrient Distribution in Colonies of Monomorium orientale (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) POOI-YEN LOKE AND CHOW-YANG LEE 1 Urban

More information

Eusocial species. Eusociality. Phylogeny showing only eusociality Eusocial insects. Eusociality: Cooperation to the extreme

Eusocial species. Eusociality. Phylogeny showing only eusociality Eusocial insects. Eusociality: Cooperation to the extreme Eusociality: Cooperation to the extreme Groups form colonies with reproductive and worker castes. Eusociality has evolved most often in insects: Ants Eusocial species Honeybees Termites Wasps Phylogeny

More information

Optimal Traffic Organisation in Ants under Crowded Conditions.

Optimal Traffic Organisation in Ants under Crowded Conditions. Optimal Traffic Organisation in Ants under Crowded Conditions. Audrey Dussutour *, Vincent Fourcassié *, Dirk Helbing #, Jean-Louis Deneubourg * Centre de Recherches sur la Cognition Animale, UMR CNRS

More information

DEPENDENCE OF NECROPHORIC RESPONSE TO OLEIC ACID ON SOCIAL CONTEXT IN THE ANT, Pogonornyrmex badius

DEPENDENCE OF NECROPHORIC RESPONSE TO OLEIC ACID ON SOCIAL CONTEXT IN THE ANT, Pogonornyrmex badius Journal of Chemical Ecology, Vol., No. 1, 183 DEPENDENCE OF NECROPHORIC RESPONSE TO OLEIC ACID ON SOCIAL CONTEXT IN THE ANT, Pogonornyrmex badius DEBORAH M. GORDON Department of Zoology, Duke University

More information

Practical 5 SOCIAL BEHAVIOUR OF HONEY BEES

Practical 5 SOCIAL BEHAVIOUR OF HONEY BEES Practical 5 SOCIAL BEHAVIOUR OF HONEY BEES Aim: To understand communication system of a honey bee colony. Introduction Among different insect orders, only 8 have been recognized by insect taxonomists which

More information

Latency time and absence of group effect: two examples

Latency time and absence of group effect: two examples Latency time and absence of group effect: two examples Jean-Louis Deneubourg 1, Eric Bonabeau 2, Guy Theraulaz 3 (1) Unit of Theoretical Behavioral Ecology, Service de Chimie-Physique, CP 231, Université

More information

Interspecific ant competition over novel aphid resources and changes in plant chemistry. due to ant-aphid mutualisms on milkweed plants

Interspecific ant competition over novel aphid resources and changes in plant chemistry. due to ant-aphid mutualisms on milkweed plants Liesl Oeller 7/27/14 Ecology Summer 2014 Interspecific ant competition over novel aphid resources and changes in plant chemistry due to ant-aphid mutualisms on milkweed plants Abstract Ants and aphids

More information

Collective Decision-Making in Honey Bee Foraging Dynamics

Collective Decision-Making in Honey Bee Foraging Dynamics Collective Decision-Making in Honey Bee Foraging Dynamics Valery Tereshko and Andreas Loengarov School of Computing, University of Paisley, Paisley PA1 2BE, Scotland October 13, 2005 Abstract We consider

More information

The Effects of Water, Season, and Colony Composition on Foraging Preferences of Pheidole ceres [Hymenoptera: Formicidae]

The Effects of Water, Season, and Colony Composition on Foraging Preferences of Pheidole ceres [Hymenoptera: Formicidae] Journal of Insect Behavior, Vol. 18, No. 6, November 2005 ( C 2005) DOI: 10.1007/s10905-005-8740-6 The Effects of Water, Season, and Colony Composition on Foraging Preferences of Pheidole ceres [Hymenoptera:

More information

Division of Labour and Task Allocation

Division of Labour and Task Allocation Division of Labour and Task Allocation Introduction Many species of insects have division of labour Task allocation at colony level linked to elasticity of individuals Model is based upon response thresholds

More information

Introduction to Swarm Robotics

Introduction to Swarm Robotics COMP 4766 Introduction to Autonomous Robotics Introduction to Swarm Robotics By Andrew Vardy April 1, 2014 Outline 1 Initial Definitions 2 Examples of SI in Biology 3 Self-Organization 4 Stigmergy 5 Swarm

More information

Outline. 1 Initial Definitions. 2 Examples of SI in Biology. 3 Self-Organization. 4 Stigmergy. 5 Swarm Robotics

Outline. 1 Initial Definitions. 2 Examples of SI in Biology. 3 Self-Organization. 4 Stigmergy. 5 Swarm Robotics Outline COMP 4766 Introduction to Autonomous Robotics 1 Initial Definitions Introduction to Swarm Robotics 2 Examples of SI in Biology 3 Self-Organization By Andrew Vardy 4 Stigmergy April 1, 2014 5 Swarm

More information

Effect of Trail Bifurcation Asymmetry and Pheromone Presence or Absence on Trail Choice by Lasius niger Ants

Effect of Trail Bifurcation Asymmetry and Pheromone Presence or Absence on Trail Choice by Lasius niger Ants international journal of behavioural biology ethology Ethology Effect of Trail Bifurcation Asymmetry and Pheromone Presence or Absence on Trail Choice by Lasius niger Ants Antonia Forster*, Tomer J. Czaczkes,

More information

The fundamental ecological issue of determining the distribution

The fundamental ecological issue of determining the distribution Collegial decision making based on social amplification leads to optimal group formation Jean-Marc Amé*, José Halloy*, Colette Rivault, Claire Detrain*, and Jean Louis Deneubourg* *Service d Ecologie Sociale

More information

Bee Behavior. Summary of an article by. Stephen Taber III from Beekeeping in the United States

Bee Behavior. Summary of an article by. Stephen Taber III from Beekeeping in the United States Bee Behavior Summary of an article by Stephen Taber III from Beekeeping in the United States Bees Sense Organs: Vision Each compound eye is spherical in shape and comprised of some 6,300 cone-shaped facets

More information

RESEARCH ARTICLE Experience, corpulence and decision making in ant foraging

RESEARCH ARTICLE Experience, corpulence and decision making in ant foraging 2653 The Journal of Experimental Biology 215, 2653-2659 2012. Published by The Company of Biologists Ltd doi:10.1242/jeb.071076 RESEARCH ARTICLE Experience, corpulence and decision making in ant foraging

More information

Honey Bee Biology Workshop. The Queen. The Drone

Honey Bee Biology Workshop. The Queen. The Drone Honey Bee Biology Workshop 1. The bee colony 2. Behavioral sequence of workers 3. Worker-queen differentiation 4. Honey bee dances 5. Chemical communications The Queen Longevity: 1-3 years 1. The mother

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

Bee Colony Activities Throughout The Year

Bee Colony Activities Throughout The Year Bee Colony Activities Throughout The Year Written by Khalil Hamdan Apeldoorn The Netherlands A honeybee gathering nectar from a flower. Photo source: forestwander.com Bee collecting pollen. Photo source:

More information

Pheromones by Ellen Miller November 2015

Pheromones by Ellen Miller November 2015 Pheromones by Ellen Miller November 2015 Greek word pherein to carry Greek word hormone to stimulate A released chemical that triggers a social response in members of the same species The chemical is transmitted

More information

Do Ant Brood Pheromones Exist?

Do Ant Brood Pheromones Exist? FORUM Do Ant Brood Pheromones Exist? LAURENCE MOREL AND ROBERT K. V ANDER MEER Insects Affecting Man and Animals Laboratory, USDA-ARS, Gainesville, Florida 32604 FORUM: Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 81(5): 705-710

More information

28 3 Insects Slide 1 of 44

28 3 Insects Slide 1 of 44 1 of 44 Class Insecta contains more species than any other group of animals. 2 of 44 What Is an Insect? What Is an Insect? Insects have a body divided into three parts head, thorax, and abdomen. Three

More information

Swarm-bots and Swarmanoid: Two experiments in embodied swarm intelligence

Swarm-bots and Swarmanoid: Two experiments in embodied swarm intelligence Swarm-bots and Swarmanoid: Two experiments in embodied swarm intelligence Marco Dorigo FNRS Research Director IRIDIA Université Libre de Bruxelles IAT - 17.9.2009 - Milano, Italy What is swarm intelligence?

More information

Nest excavation in ants: group size effects on the size and structure of tunneling networks

Nest excavation in ants: group size effects on the size and structure of tunneling networks Naturwissenschaften (2004) 91:602 606 DOI 10.1007/s00114-004-0577-x SHORT COMMUNICATION JØrôme Buhl Jacques Gautrais Jean-Louis Deneubourg Guy Theraulaz Nest excavation in ants: group size effects on the

More information

Environmental signals

Environmental signals Environmental signals Why are environmental signals rare? Pp 632-635 Resource recruitment signals Costs and benefits Vertebrates and social insects Predator detection signals Types Patterns of usage Intertrophic

More information

Self-Organization and Collective Decision Making in Animal and Human Societies

Self-Organization and Collective Decision Making in Animal and Human Societies Self-Organization and Collective Decision Making... Frank Schweitzer SYSS Dresden 31 March 26 1 / 23 Self-Organization and Collective Decision Making in Animal and Human Societies Frank Schweitzer fschweitzer@ethz.ch

More information

Organization of work via the "common stomach" in social insects

Organization of work via the common stomach in social insects Organization of work via the "common stomach" in social insects István Karsai*, Thomas Schmickl** *Dept. Biological Sciences East Tennessee State University **Dept. Zoology Karl Franzens University Graz

More information

Modeling the Adaptive Role of Negative Signaling in Honey Bee Intraspecific Competition

Modeling the Adaptive Role of Negative Signaling in Honey Bee Intraspecific Competition J Insect Behav (21) 23:459 471 DOI 1.17/s195-1-9229-5 Modeling the Adaptive Role of Negative Signaling in Honey Bee Intraspecific Competition Brian R. Johnson & James C. Nieh Revised: 5 August 21 / Accepted:

More information

Emergence of increased division of labor as a function of group size

Emergence of increased division of labor as a function of group size Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2007) 62:289 298 DOI 10.1007/s00265-007-0464-5 ORIGINAL PAPER Emergence of increased division of labor as a function of group size Raphaël Jeanson & Jennifer H. Fewell & Root Gorelick

More information

Fire ants, Solenopsis invicta, dry and store insect pieces for later use

Fire ants, Solenopsis invicta, dry and store insect pieces for later use Fire ants, Solenopsis invicta, dry and store insect pieces for later use Glivery G. Gayahan a and Walter R. Tschinkel b Department of Biological Science, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL 32306-4370

More information

SYMPOSIUM Student Journal of Science & Math. Volume 2 Issue 1

SYMPOSIUM Student Journal of Science & Math. Volume 2 Issue 1 SYMPOSIUM Student Journal of Science & Math Volume 2 Issue 1 biology 117 B82.731 OBSERVATIONAL LEARNING IN EUSOCIAL INSECTS Background A RESEARCH PROPOSAL by Avity Norman Ants (order Hymenoptera, family

More information

Research Article Nest Digging by Leaf-Cutting Ants: Effect of Group Size and Functional Structures

Research Article Nest Digging by Leaf-Cutting Ants: Effect of Group Size and Functional Structures Psyche Volume 2012, Article ID 426719, 4 pages doi:10.1155/2012/426719 Research Article Nest Digging by Leaf-Cutting Ants: Effect of Group Size and Functional Structures Roberto da Silva Camargo, Ricardo

More information

The Little Miss Muffet Effect: Quantifying the Effect of Predation Risk on Foraging Patch Choice by Houseflies (Musca domestica)

The Little Miss Muffet Effect: Quantifying the Effect of Predation Risk on Foraging Patch Choice by Houseflies (Musca domestica) Journal of Insect Behavior, Vol. 18, No. 6, November 2005 ( C 2005) DOI: 10.1007/s10905-005-8744-2 The Little Miss Muffet Effect: Quantifying the Effect of Predation Risk on Foraging Patch Choice by Houseflies

More information

The Wisdom of the Acorn: Social Foraging in Temnothorax Ants. Zachary Shaffer

The Wisdom of the Acorn: Social Foraging in Temnothorax Ants. Zachary Shaffer The Wisdom of the Acorn: Social Foraging in Temnothorax Ants by Zachary Shaffer A Dissertation Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy Approved April 2014

More information

Colony nutritional status modulates worker responses to foraging recruitment pheromone in the bumblebee Bombus terrestris

Colony nutritional status modulates worker responses to foraging recruitment pheromone in the bumblebee Bombus terrestris Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2008) 62:1919 1926 DOI 10.1007/s00265-008-0623-3 ORIGINAL PAPER Colony nutritional status modulates worker responses to foraging recruitment pheromone in the bumblebee Bombus terrestris

More information

Synergy in social communication

Synergy in social communication CHAPTER 11 Synergy in social communication David J. T. Sumpter and Åke Brännström 11.1 Introduction Synergy is where a group of individuals co-ordinate to achieve a task that would not be possible were

More information

Social Insects. Social Insects. Subsocial. Social Insects 4/9/15. Insect Ecology

Social Insects. Social Insects. Subsocial. Social Insects 4/9/15. Insect Ecology Social Insects Social Insects Insect Ecology Sociality evolved multiple times in insects Much of Earth s fauna consists of social insects They play major roles in entire ecosystems Proliferation of ants

More information

Social Insects. Insect Ecology

Social Insects. Insect Ecology Social Insects Insect Ecology Social Insects Sociality evolved multiple times in insects Much of Earth s fauna consists of social insects They play major roles in entire ecosystems Proliferation of ants

More information

Foraging in honeybees when does it pay to dance?

Foraging in honeybees when does it pay to dance? Behavioral Ecology Advance Access published December 6, 2007 Behavioral Ecology doi:10.1093/beheco/arm117 Foraging in honeybees when does it pay to dance? Madeleine Beekman a,b and Jie Bin Lew a a Behaviour

More information

Hunger in Red Imported Fire Ants and Their Behavioral Response to Two Liquid Bait Products

Hunger in Red Imported Fire Ants and Their Behavioral Response to Two Liquid Bait Products HOUSEHOLD AND STRUCTURAL INSECTS Hunger in Red Imported Fire Ants and Their Behavioral Response to Two Liquid Bait Products KATHRYN S. O BRIEN AND LINDA M. HOOPER-BÙI Department of Entomology, 404 Life

More information

Variation in the transition from inside to outside work in the red harvester ant Pogonomyrmex barbatus

Variation in the transition from inside to outside work in the red harvester ant Pogonomyrmex barbatus Insect. Soc. 52 (2005) 212 217 0020-1812/05/030212-06 DOI 10.1007/s00040-004-0796-3 Birkhäuser Verlag, Basel, 2005 Insectes Sociaux Research article Variation in the transition from inside to outside work

More information

Queen worker caste ratio depends on colony size in the pharaoh ant (Monomorium pharaonis)

Queen worker caste ratio depends on colony size in the pharaoh ant (Monomorium pharaonis) Insect. Soc. DOI 10.1007/s00040-010-0126-x Insectes Sociaux RESEARCH ARTICLE Queen worker caste ratio depends on colony size in the pharaoh ant (Monomorium pharaonis) A. M. Schmidt T. A. Linksvayer J.

More information

Capacitor Placement for Economical Electrical Systems using Ant Colony Search Algorithm

Capacitor Placement for Economical Electrical Systems using Ant Colony Search Algorithm Capacitor Placement for Economical Electrical Systems using Ant Colony Search Algorithm Bharat Solanki Abstract The optimal capacitor placement problem involves determination of the location, number, type

More information

Intracolonial nepotism during colony fissioning in honey bees?

Intracolonial nepotism during colony fissioning in honey bees? Intracolonial nepotism during colony fissioning in honey bees? Juliana Rangel Co-authors: Heather Mattila, Thomas Seeley Department of Neurobiology and Behavior Cornell University Apimondia Conference,

More information

SUPERCOOLING POINTS OF RED IMPORTED FIRE ANTS, SOLENOPSIS INVICTA (HYMENOPTERA: FORMICIDAE) FROM LUBBOCK, TEXAS'

SUPERCOOLING POINTS OF RED IMPORTED FIRE ANTS, SOLENOPSIS INVICTA (HYMENOPTERA: FORMICIDAE) FROM LUBBOCK, TEXAS' Vol. 98, No. 4, September & October 1987 153 SUPERCOOLING POINTS OF RED IMPORTED FIRE ANTS, SOLENOPSIS INVICTA (HYMENOPTERA: FORMICIDAE) FROM LUBBOCK, TEXAS' Stephen W. ~ a b e r 2James, C. ~ o k e n d

More information

Self-Organization in Social Insects

Self-Organization in Social Insects Self-Organization in Social Insects Kazuo Uyehara 5/04/2008 Self-organization is a process in which complex patterns or behaviors are produced without direction from an outside or supervising source. This

More information

Evolving Agent Swarms for Clustering and Sorting

Evolving Agent Swarms for Clustering and Sorting Evolving Agent Swarms for Clustering and Sorting Vegard Hartmann Complex Adaptive Organically-Inspired Systems Group (CAOS) Dept of Computer Science The Norwegian University of Science and Technology (NTNU)

More information

Sex allocation in the ant Camponotus (Colobopsis) nipponicus (Wheeler): II. The effect of resource availability on sex-ratio variability

Sex allocation in the ant Camponotus (Colobopsis) nipponicus (Wheeler): II. The effect of resource availability on sex-ratio variability Insect. Soc. DOI 10.1007/s00040-013-0297-3 Insectes Sociaux RESEARCH ARTICLE Sex allocation in the ant Camponotus (Colobopsis) nipponicus (Wheeler): II. The effect of resource availability on sex-ratio

More information

Meta-heuristic ant colony optimization technique to forecast the amount of summer monsoon rainfall: skill comparison with Markov chain model

Meta-heuristic ant colony optimization technique to forecast the amount of summer monsoon rainfall: skill comparison with Markov chain model Chapter -7 Meta-heuristic ant colony optimization technique to forecast the amount of summer monsoon rainfall: skill comparison with Markov chain model 7.1 Summary Forecasting summer monsoon rainfall with

More information

12. Social insects. Is it better to be social? Is it better to be social? What is social? Some costs of being social

12. Social insects. Is it better to be social? Is it better to be social? What is social? Some costs of being social Is it better to be social? 12. Social insects Cost and benefit viewpoint Social behavior is not always adaptive (costs exceed benefits) What are some costs of being social? What are some benefits of being

More information

Insect Success. Insects are one of the most successful groups of living organisms on earth

Insect Success. Insects are one of the most successful groups of living organisms on earth Insect Success Insects are one of the most successful groups of living organisms on earth Why Insects are so successful Insects comprise about 95% of all known animal species. Actually it is insects instead

More information

Bee Communica,on A Hive Mind

Bee Communica,on A Hive Mind Bee Communica,on A Hive Mind Hive Mind Hive Mind One for all and all for one Kill self to protect the hive Fatal S,ng WHY? Kin Selec)on: So closely related The hive occupants: One queen a fer,le female

More information

open flower infructescence stem inflorescence stem cross section bracts rosette

open flower infructescence stem inflorescence stem cross section bracts rosette open flower inflorescence stem cross section bracts rosette Figure S1. Photomontage of Vriesea friburgensis with inflorescence (left) and infructescence (right) (normally only one stem per rosette). Stems

More information

Information transfer during recruitment in the ant Lasius niger L. (Hymenoptera: Formicidae)

Information transfer during recruitment in the ant Lasius niger L. (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2004) 55:242 250 DOI 10.1007/s00265-003-0704-2 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Julien Le Breton V. FourcassiØ Information transfer during recruitment in the ant Lasius niger L. (Hymenotera: Formicidae)

More information

In-hive pollen transfer between bees enhances cross-pollination of plants

In-hive pollen transfer between bees enhances cross-pollination of plants In-hive pollen transfer between bees enhances cross-pollination of plants J. Paalhaar, W.J. Boot, J.J.M. van der Steen* & J.N.M. Calis Laboratory of Entomology, Wageningen University, PO Box 803, 6700

More information

Natal versus breeding dispersal: Evolution in a model system

Natal versus breeding dispersal: Evolution in a model system Evolutionary Ecology Research, 1999, 1: 911 921 Natal versus breeding dispersal: Evolution in a model system Karin Johst 1 * and Roland Brandl 2 1 Centre for Environmental Research Leipzig-Halle Ltd, Department

More information

Foraging. This week in Animal Behaviour. How do animals choose? Do animals choose their food?

Foraging. This week in Animal Behaviour. How do animals choose? Do animals choose their food? This week in Animal Behaviour Lecture 22: Cooperation Lecture 23: Foraging Lecture 24: TBA Text chapters 10-11 LAB: Project 2 proposal seminars Midterm 2 on Tuesday Nov. 8 Foraging 1. Models of foraging

More information

Marie-Claire Cammaerts 1, Zoheir Rachidi 1 & Geoffrey Gosset 1

Marie-Claire Cammaerts 1, Zoheir Rachidi 1 & Geoffrey Gosset 1 International Journal of Biology; Vol. 6, No. 3; 2014 ISSN 1916-9671 E-ISSN 1916-968X Published by Canadian Center of Science and Education Physiological and Ethological Effects of Caffeine, Theophylline,

More information

the primary pollinator for more than 50 fruit and vegetable crops, which form the nutritional

the primary pollinator for more than 50 fruit and vegetable crops, which form the nutritional de Ridder 1 Title: A Life Lesson, A Tiny Teacher: Honeybee Politics Author: Kendal de Ridder Abstract This paper aims to examine the intricate communication methods of honeybees; beginning with the initial

More information

Submitted to Biology Letters. Patterns of split sex ratio in ants have multiple evolutionary causes based on different within-colony conflicts

Submitted to Biology Letters. Patterns of split sex ratio in ants have multiple evolutionary causes based on different within-colony conflicts Patterns of split sex ratio in ants have multiple evolutionary causes based on different within-colony conflicts Journal: Biology Letters Manuscript ID: draft Article Type: Research Date Submitted by the

More information

Journal of Insect Physiology

Journal of Insect Physiology Journal of Insect Physiology 54 (2008) 1349 1355 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Insect Physiology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jinsphys Effect of social and environmental

More information

Peter Dutton. 28 August Halesworth& District

Peter Dutton. 28 August Halesworth& District Peter Dutton 28 August 2014 Halesworth& District Bees Hymenoptera - 150,000+ species Sawflies, Wasps, Bees, Ants, Bees - 20,000+ species Most solitary, a few social Honey Bee - The only one which produces

More information

Research Article Ants Can Expect the Time of an Event on Basis of Previous Experiences

Research Article Ants Can Expect the Time of an Event on Basis of Previous Experiences International Scholarly Research Notices Volume 2016, Article ID 9473128, 9 pages http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2016/9473128 Research Article Ants Can Expect the Time of an Event on Basis of Previous Experiences

More information

10 Trophallaxis. A Mechanism of Information Transfer. Walter M. Farina and Christoph Grüter. Contents. Introduction

10 Trophallaxis. A Mechanism of Information Transfer. Walter M. Farina and Christoph Grüter. Contents. Introduction 10 Trophallaxis A Mechanism of Information Transfer Walter M. Farina and Christoph Grüter Contents Introduction... 173 Returning to the Nest after Foraging... 175 The Offering Behavior of Active Foragers...

More information

Communication in ants

Communication in ants R570 Primer Communication in ants Duncan E. Jackson 1 and Francis L.W. Ratnieks 2 Male stumptail macaques from Thailand. Despite a clear dominance hierarchy stumptail macaques are tolerant and reconcile

More information

Sensitive Ant Model for Combinatorial Optimization

Sensitive Ant Model for Combinatorial Optimization Sensitive Ant Model for Combinatorial Optimization CAMELIA CHIRA cchira@cs.ubbcluj.ro D. DUMITRESCU ddumitr@cs.ubbcluj.ro CAMELIA-MIHAELA PINTEA cmpintea@cs.ubbcluj.ro Abstract: A combinatorial optimization

More information

Outline. Ant Colony Optimization. Outline. Swarm Intelligence DM812 METAHEURISTICS. 1. Ant Colony Optimization Context Inspiration from Nature

Outline. Ant Colony Optimization. Outline. Swarm Intelligence DM812 METAHEURISTICS. 1. Ant Colony Optimization Context Inspiration from Nature DM812 METAHEURISTICS Outline Lecture 8 http://www.aco-metaheuristic.org/ 1. 2. 3. Marco Chiarandini Department of Mathematics and Computer Science University of Southern Denmark, Odense, Denmark

More information

Available online at ScienceDirect. Procedia Computer Science 20 (2013 ) 90 95

Available online at  ScienceDirect. Procedia Computer Science 20 (2013 ) 90 95 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia Computer Science 20 (2013 ) 90 95 Complex Adaptive Systems, Publication 3 Cihan H. Dagli, Editor in Chief Conference Organized by Missouri

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

Colony-size effects on task organization in the harvester ant Pogonomyrmex californicus

Colony-size effects on task organization in the harvester ant Pogonomyrmex californicus Insect. Soc. (2013) 60:191 201 DOI 10.1007/s00040-013-0282-x Insectes Sociaux RESEARCH ARTICLE Colony-size effects on task organization in the harvester ant Pogonomyrmex californicus C. T. Holbrook T.

More information

When do honey bee guards reject their former nestmates after swarming?

When do honey bee guards reject their former nestmates after swarming? Insectes soc. 49 (2002) 56 61 0020-1812/02/010056-06 $ 1.50+0.20/0 Birkhäuser Verlag, Basel, 2002 Insectes Sociaux Research article When do honey bee guards reject their former nestmates after swarming?

More information

The honeybee waggle dance: can we follow the steps?

The honeybee waggle dance: can we follow the steps? Opinion The honeybee waggle dance: can we follow the steps? Christoph Grüter 1 and Walter M. Farina 2 1 Laboratory of Apiculture and Social Insects, Department of Biological and Environmental Science,

More information

Heterogeneity and Value-Sensitive Collective Decision-Making

Heterogeneity and Value-Sensitive Collective Decision-Making Heterogeneity and Value-Sensitive Collective Decision-Making Naomi Ehrich Leonard Mechanical & Aerospace Engineering, Princeton University Vaibhav Srivastava, MAE Dept., Princeton University Alessio Franci,

More information

What makes a honeybee scout?

What makes a honeybee scout? Behav Ecol Sociobiol (7) 6:985 995 DOI.7/s65-6-33-9 ORIGINAL PAPER What makes a honeybee scout? Madeleine Beekman & Amy L. Gilchrist & Michael Duncan & David J. T. Sumpter Received: 6 May 6 / Revised:

More information

The Use of Edges in Visual Navigation by the Ant Leptothorax albipennis

The Use of Edges in Visual Navigation by the Ant Leptothorax albipennis Ethology 107, 1125Ð1136 (2001) Ó 2001 Blackwell Wissenschafts-Verlag, Berlin ISSN 0179±1613 Centre for Mathematical Biology, and Department of Biology and Biochemistry, University of Bath The Use of Edges

More information

Detecting Nestmate Recognition Patterns in the Fission-Performing Ant Aphaenogaster senilis: A Comparison of Different Indices

Detecting Nestmate Recognition Patterns in the Fission-Performing Ant Aphaenogaster senilis: A Comparison of Different Indices Detecting Nestmate Recognition Patterns in the Fission-Performing Ant Aphaenogaster senilis: A Comparison of Different Indices Katsuya Ichinose, 1,2,4 Xím Cerdá, 1,3 Christidè s Jean-Philippe, 1 and Alain

More information

Model of Droplet Dynamics in the Argentine Ant Linepithema Humile (Mayr)

Model of Droplet Dynamics in the Argentine Ant Linepithema Humile (Mayr) Bulletin of Mathematical Biology (2001) 63, 1079 1093 doi:10.1006/bulm.2001.0260 Available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Model of Droplet Dynamics in the Argentine Ant Linepithema Humile (Mayr)

More information

Upskilling community leaders for Australian Pollinator Week

Upskilling community leaders for Australian Pollinator Week Upskilling community leaders for Australian Pollinator Week Classroom or other educational activities DISCLAIMER Hello and thank you for being part of this project. This presentation is designed to be

More information

Floral scents affect the distribution of hive bees around dancers

Floral scents affect the distribution of hive bees around dancers Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2007) 61:1589 1597 DOI 10.1007/s00265-007-0391-5 ORIGINAL PAPER Floral scents affect the distribution of hive bees around dancers Paula C. Díaz & Christoph Grüter & Walter M. Farina

More information

Evolution of Self-Organized Systems

Evolution of Self-Organized Systems Reference: Biol. Bull. 202: 256 261. (June 2002) Evolution of Self-Organized Systems BLAINE J. COLE Department of Biology and Biochemistry, University of Houston, Houston, Texas 77204-5001 Abstract. In

More information

Abstract. Introduction

Abstract. Introduction NEW METHOD FOR RAISING QUEENS Hossein Yeganehrad Caspian Apiaries P.O. Box 16058 617, New Westminster, British Columbia, Canada, V3M 6W6 radbees@hotmail.com Paper 138, Oral Presentation XXXVIIIth Apimondia

More information

4 Questions relating to Behavior

4 Questions relating to Behavior Chapter 51: Animal Behavior 1. Stimulus & Response 2. Learned Behavior 3. Connecting Behavior to Survival & Reproduction 4 Questions relating to Behavior The Dutch behavioral scientist Niko Tinbergen proposed

More information

Foundations of animal behaviour. Niaux caves, France. Roz Dakin. roslyndakin.com/biol321. Lecture notes posted here

Foundations of animal behaviour. Niaux caves, France. Roz Dakin. roslyndakin.com/biol321. Lecture notes posted here Foundations of animal behaviour Niaux caves, France Roz Dakin 2rd@queensu.ca Montgomerie Lab Room 4325 Biosciences (moving to 3520) 10-15,000 years ago people travelled 5 km deep into tunnels... We ve

More information

The Bee, Part I: Dance of the Bees Gioietta Kuo

The Bee, Part I: Dance of the Bees Gioietta Kuo January 9, 2018 Busy Bees by Sharon Sperry Flickr CC BY-NC-ND 2.0 The Bee, Part I: Dance of the Bees Gioietta Kuo Bees are not ordinary wildlife. They have a special relationship with human society. For

More information

Emergent Teamwork. Craig Reynolds. Cognitive Animation Workshop June 4-5, 2008 Yosemite

Emergent Teamwork. Craig Reynolds. Cognitive Animation Workshop June 4-5, 2008 Yosemite Emergent Teamwork Craig Reynolds Cognitive Animation Workshop June 4-5, 2008 Yosemite 1 This talk Whirlwind tour of collective behavior in nature Some earlier simulation models Recent work on agent-based

More information

Ecology Notes CHANGING POPULATIONS

Ecology Notes CHANGING POPULATIONS Ecology Notes TEK 8.11 (B) Investigate how organisms and populations in an ecosystem depend on and may compete for biotic and abiotic factors such as quantity of light, water, range of temperatures, or

More information

Worker connectivity: a review of the design of worker communication systems and their effects on task performance in insect societies

Worker connectivity: a review of the design of worker communication systems and their effects on task performance in insect societies Insect. Soc. 54 (2007) 203 210 0020-1812/07/030203-8 DOI 10.1007/s00040-007-0945-6 Birkhäuser Verlag, Basel, 2007 Insectes Sociaux Review article Worker connectivity: a review of the design of worker communication

More information

An ant colony algorithm for multiple sequence alignment in bioinformatics

An ant colony algorithm for multiple sequence alignment in bioinformatics An ant colony algorithm for multiple sequence alignment in bioinformatics Jonathan Moss and Colin G. Johnson Computing Laboratory University of Kent at Canterbury Canterbury, Kent, CT2 7NF, England. C.G.Johnson@ukc.ac.uk

More information

The Phase-Ordering Kinetics of Cemetery Organization in Ants

The Phase-Ordering Kinetics of Cemetery Organization in Ants The Phase-Ordering Kinetics of Cemetery Organization in Ants Eric Bonabeau Guy Theraulaz Vincent Fourcassié Jean-Louis Deneubourg SFI WORKING PAPER: 1998-01-008 SFI Working Papers contain accounts of scientific

More information

Assessment Schedule 2013 Biology: Demonstrate understanding of the responses of plants and animals to their external environment (91603)

Assessment Schedule 2013 Biology: Demonstrate understanding of the responses of plants and animals to their external environment (91603) NCEA Level 3 Biology (91603) 2013 page 1 of 6 Assessment Schedule 2013 Biology: Demonstrate understanding of the responses of plants and animals to their external environment (91603) Assessment Criteria

More information

Nutritional Asymmetries Are Related to Division of Labor in a Queenless Ant

Nutritional Asymmetries Are Related to Division of Labor in a Queenless Ant Nutritional Asymmetries Are Related to Division of Labor in a Queenless Ant Chris R. Smith 1 *, Andrew V. Suarez 2, Neil D. Tsutsui 3, Sarah E. Wittman 4, Benjamin Edmonds 5, Alex Freauff 5, Chadwick V.

More information

Regents Review Assignment #8-A08 Living Environment: Comet Part A Questions

Regents Review Assignment #8-A08 Living Environment: Comet Part A Questions Part A Questions 1. A student notices that fruit flies with the curlywing trait develop straight wings if kept at a temperature of 16 C, but develop curly wings if kept at 25 C. The best explanation for

More information