Review of Robert A. Paul Mixed Messages: Cultural and Genetic Inheritance in the Constitution of Human Society. Herbert Gintis

Similar documents
1 Gene-Culture Coevolution

Sociobiological Approaches. What We Will Cover in This Section. Overview. Overview Evolutionary Theory Sociobiology Summary

Two critical areas of change as we diverged from our evolutionary relatives: 1. Changes to peripheral mechanisms

The Evolution of Mind and Morality: 19th-21st Centuries. Winter, 2007

Chapter 1: Introduction

Primate Diversity & Human Evolution (Outline)

1 low Humans Evolved

Human Adaptation - ad aptos: good fit between trait and environment

Evolution & Natural Selection

[in press, July, 2006: In W. A. Darity (Ed.), International Encyclopedia of Social Sciences (2 nd ed.). New. Genetic Determinism

3/30/2012. Two Contrasting but Complementary Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Behavior:

ABSTRACT: Dissolving the evolutionary puzzle of human cooperation.

Standards A complete list of the standards covered by this lesson is included in the Appendix at the end of the lesson.

The Evolution of Speech

Neanderthal vs Cro-Magnon 1of10 found at

FLASH CARDS. Kalat s Book Chapter 1 Alphabetical

Anthro 101: Human Biological Evolution. Lecture 11: Cooperation, Intelligence, Communication, Culture, & Human Behavior. Prof.

Lesson Topic Learning Goals

:: Slide 1 :: :: Slide 2 :: :: Slide 3 :: :: Slide 3 continued :: :: Slide 4 :: :: Slide 5 ::

Bio 1M: The evolution of apes. 1 Example. 2 Patterns of evolution. Similarities and differences. History

At Penn Museum Conference: Summary of Richerson

APES C4L2 HOW DOES THE EARTH S LIFE CHANGE OVER TIME? Textbook pages 85 88

'University of Michigan

Evolution by Natural Selection. Though it is considered a theory, there are few facts more widely accepted in all of science!

Evolution Problem Drill 10: Human Evolution

NGSS Example Bundles. 1 of 15

MS-LS3-1 Heredity: Inheritance and Variation of Traits

arxiv: v1 [q-bio.pe] 9 Sep 2016

Cooperation Achieved by Migration and Evolution in a Multilevel Selection Context

Level 3 Biology, 2014

Origin of an idea about origins

How related are organisms?

Who Were Neanderthals and Did They Interbreed with Modern Humans?

ADAPTATIONS. Characteristics that give an organism a better chance of survival.

Introduction. THE FORMATION OF THE SOLAR SYSTEM - Theories Old and New Imperial College Press

Understanding Natural Selection

MATCHING STRUCTURE AND THE EVOLUTION OF COOPERATION IN THE PRISONER S DILEMMA

On the Origin of Altruism. desire that compels humans to help is called altruism by psychologists. Through this feeling,

Yes. 1 ENGL 191 * Writing Workshop II. 2 ENGL 191 * Writing Workshop II. Yes

6 HOW DID OUR ANCESTORS EVOLVE?

Characteristics of Animals

Chapter 16: Evolutionary Theory

Mammalogy Lecture 15 - Social Behavior II: Evolution

Lesson 1 Syllabus Reference

The Evolving Brain. Communication Intelligence Technologies Social Complexity Cultural Complexity

Introduction to Biology

Evolution of cooperation. Martin Nowak, Harvard University

Introduction to Biology

15.3 Darwin Presents his Case. Biology Mr. Hines

UNIT 4: EVOLUTION Chapter 12: The History of Life. I. The Fossil Record (12.1) A. Fossils can form in several ways

Biological Anthropology

Cooperation. Main points for today. How can altruism evolve? Group living vs. cooperation. Sociality-nocooperation. and cooperationno-sociality

GRADE 6 SCIENCE REVISED 2014

Anthropology 562: Introduction to Evolutionary Anthropology

Bones of Contention (Lubenow) PPT

Biological Anthropology Sample Exam 2 MULTIPLE CHOICE

4. Identify one bird that would most likely compete for food with the large tree finch. Support your answer. [1]

Theory a well supported testable explanation of phenomenon occurring in the natural world.

Computer Simulations on Evolution BiologyLabs On-line. Laboratory 1 for Section B. Laboratory 2 for Section A

Biological Anthropology

Selfishness and Cooperation: Challenge for Social Life

The Theory of Evolution Chapter 6

Chapter 16: Reconstructing and Using Phylogenies

The Origin of the Social Impulse: E.O. Wilson s Recent and Controversial Rejection of Kin Selection in Historical Context

The History of Life on Earth

Studying Life. Lesson Overview. Lesson Overview. 1.3 Studying Life

Evolution and Epigenetics. Seminar: Social, Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience Speaker: Wolf-R. Brockhaus

GENERAL INFORMATION. Number of One credit (1)

On the evolution of reciprocal cooperation

Extensive evidence indicates that life on Earth began more than 3 billion years ago.

How Altruism Can Prevail Under Natural Selection

MS-LS4-1 Biological Evolution: Unity and Diversity

EVOLUTION. - Selection, Survival, and Drift

Heredity and Evolution

The theory of evolution continues to be refined as scientists learn new information.

(Richard Dawkins) 20 Desmond Morris

The Human Animal. Molecular Evidence. Early Homo Evolution

The Human Animal. BIO 1300: The Human Animal

B L U E V A L L E Y D I S T R I C T C U R R I C U L U M Science 7 th grade


Assessment Schedule 2016 Biology: Demonstrate understanding of trends in human evolution (91606)

Growth & Development. Characteristics of Living Things. What is development? Movement. What is a cell?

Memes & Changing language

Evolution. Some quotes to begin:

Casey Leonard. Multiregional model vs. Out of Africa theory SLCC

Lesson 10 Study Guide

Ray, Evolutionary Psychology Instructor Site: Test Bank Questions Ch. 2, p. 1. Chapter 2

Homework. Guided Reading Recent Hominids (#22-31) Need ear buds/headphones for Monday!!

Two Views of Adaptation

Kingdom Animalia. Zoology the study of animals

HOMO SAPIENS EXHIBITION

Course Name: Biology Level: A Points: 5 Teacher Name: Claire E. Boudreau

Life Science Curriculum Sixth Grade

Environmental Influences on Adaptation

The Origin of New Species

Group activities: Making animal model of human behaviors e.g. Wine preference model in mice

GRADE EIGHT Theme: Cause and Effect

Evolution and Darwin

NGSS UNIT OVERVIEW EVOLUTION

Evolution and Natural Selection (16-18)

Transcription:

Review of Robert A. Paul Mixed Messages: Cultural and Genetic Inheritance in the Constitution of Human Society Herbert Gintis December 12, 2016 Robert A. Paul s recent book is an extremely broad-ranging and informative investigation of cultural dynamics in small-scale societies. Paul is a cultural anthropologist with a broad range of sociological knowledge concerning such societies. His theoretical framework is that of dual inheritance theory, which he applies it to a vast number of issues in different societies. His analysis is very successful scientifically and yet remains enjoyable throughout. He is an excellent writer and the book fits together well. Dual inheritance theory, as I explain below, arose as a theory of the interaction between genes and culture in the evolution of our species the only known species to which the notion of long-term cultural evolution applies. Paul s interpretation of this theory is, however, purely cultural. This interpretation is idiosyncratic, but defensible because he is dealing with societies in time periods too short for extensive genetic evolution to have taken place. Paul s version of dual inheritance theory posits that there are two competing/conflicting systems of human sociality. The first is kinship/family, which we share with innumerable other species. The second is tribal, which does not suppress family, but promotes cooperation, amity, and regard independent of kinship genealogy. He suggests that human cultural symbolic systems induce individuals to stress their cooperation and friendship with non-family, which accounts for our success as a species. Paul has thought deeply about these matters, and his treatment is well worth thinking about. The reason this interpretation of dual inheritance theory is plausible is that humans share with innumerable sexually-reproducing species the tendency to favor family members over genealogically distant individuals. It follows that kin/family preference is genetic in a way that tribal preferences 1

are not (applying, as they do, only to our species). Paul stresses repeatedly that kinship/family and tribal cultural systems develop almost independently, and are often in conflict. The fitness-reducing nature of some cultural institutions was stressed by Robert B. Edgerton decades ago in his in his Sick Societies: Challenging the Myth of Primitive Harmony (New York, Press, 1992), and curiously Paul does not reference this great book at all. I suspect that Paul prefers the term dual inheritance to gene-culture coevolution because the latter suggests harmony, not conflict, between the two systems. Paul s treatment of human evolutionary theory is, however, not completely balanced. First, he refers to the masters of inclusive fitness theory only in the historical section of the book, and he misstates repeated the notion that individuals can spread their genes only by personal biological reproduction. This is not a big problem, but it is incorrect and very annoying. Moreover, it conflicts with one of Paul s major building blocks, the kinship/family nexus. As was shown long ago by William see, for instance, his collection of articles Narrow Roads of Gene Land, Oxford University Press, 1998 individuals can enhance their fitness both by raising more offspring or by helping relatives to raise their offspring, precisely because relatives share more genes that randomly selected groups of individuals. Indeed, the main reason humans differentially help relatives is not that we share genes for kin-helping with other species, but because Hamilton s notion of inclusive fitness applies just as much to humans as to other species. Genes that promote cooperation independent of kinship, the key to our success as a species, can also spread, according to standard population biology see, for instance my recent book with Samuel Bowles, A Cooperative Species: Human Reciprocity and its Evolution (Princeton: Princeton University Press, 2011). But the mechanisms are rather subtle and apply only to highly social species, as has been stressed by Bert Holldobler and Edward O. Wilson see their inspiring work The Superorganism: The Beauty, Elegance, and Strangeness of Insect Societies (New York: Norton, 2008). A second imbalance in his treatment is that he makes no case whatever for the gene-side of dual inheritance theory, or what I prefer to call gene-culture coevolution, a process which is of fundamentally important interest in making humans what they are, independent from the clash of kinship/family versus tribal values. Gene-culture coevolution explores the evolutionary dynamic that applies to any species for which epigenetic information takes the form of culture that accumulates reliably and long-term across generations. That applies strongly only to Homo sapiens. The main point is that culture includes techniques and social norms that determine which genes will be evolutionarily rewarded in a given society. Therefore human genes are the product of human culture as much as the reverse. This theory was developed by Lumsden and Wilson (1981), Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman (1981), and Boyd and Richerson (1985), and has become a major branch of 2

evolutionary theory. Indeed, it is supplies the basic framework of my recent book Individuality and Entanglement: The Moral and Material Bases of Human Social Life, Princeton University Press, 2016. I give, as an extended example of the intimate links between genes and culture in humans, the evolution of the physiology of communication. This example is important because it involves the development of key human genetic capacities that are the product of social structure as opposed to simple individual adaptation. The physiology of communication in humans is an emergent property of human social evolution (Gintis 2011). The example is especially relevant to this journal, which explores the evolutionary roots of the humanities. How are genes and human culture similar? Both genes and human culture consist of information passed across generations and subject to mutation and selection. How have humans managed to evolve their extraordinary capacity for verbal commumnication? Persuasive and informative speakers in the hunter-gatherer period of our existence were rewarded with higher quality mates and increased reproductive opportunities. Their offspring inherited their cognitive and physiological communicative powers. This is gene-culture coevolution of human communication, operating over tens and hundreds of thousands of years. The evolution of the physiology of speech and facial communication presents a theoretical challenge. It is easy to explain, if everyone else is gabbing away and you can only grunt and pant, why you might be handicapped in finding a spouse and teaching your children. But how could the use of complex phonics begin? When everyone is grunting and panting, what is the fitness benefit of being able to make more complex varieties of sounds? What is the fitness benefit of being able to interpret complex sounds? The answers are far from obvious and go far beyond simple individual fitness, or even inclusive fitness, maximization. For this reason, the evolution of the physiology of speech is a dramatically complex example of gene-culture coevolution. A most common error in the literature is to consider language as a purely mental phenomenon that can be explained simply as a byproduct of brain size and intelligence. In fact, the ability to communicate through facial sign and speech has required major changes in human physiology. These could only have come about because individuals with superior communication capacities were rewarded with more and healthier children. Why might this have occurred? The increased social importance of communication in human society rewarded genetic changes that facilitate speech. Regions in the motor cortex expanded in early humans to facilitate speech production. Concurrently, nerves and muscles to the mouth, larynx, and tongue became more numerous to handle the complexities of speech (Jurmain, Nelson, Kilgore and Travathan 1997). Parts of the cerebral cortex, Broca s and Wernicke s areas, which do not exist or are relatively small 3

in other primates, are large in humans and permit grammatical speech and comprehension (Binder, Frost, Hammeke, Cox, Rao and Prieto 1997, Belin, Zatorre, Lafaille, Ahad and Pike 2000). Adult modern humans have a larynx low in the throat, a position that allows the throat to serve as a resonating chamber capable of a great number of sounds (Relethford 2007). The first hominids that have skeletal structures supporting this laryngeal placement are the Homo heidelbergensis, who lived from 800,000 to 100,000 years ago. In addition, the production of consonants requires a short oral cavity, whereas our nearest primate relatives have much too long an oral cavity for this purpose. The position of the hyoid bone, which is a point of attachment for a tongue muscle, developed in Homo sapiens in a manner permitting highly precise and flexible tongue movements. Another indication that the tongue has evolved hominids to facilitate speech is the size of the hypoglossal canal, an aperture that permits the hypoglossal nerve to reach the tongue muscles. This aperture is much larger in Neanderthals and humans than in early hominids and nonhuman primates (Dunbar 2005). Human facial nerves and musculature have also evolved to facilitate communication. This musculature is present in all vertebrates, but except in mammals it serves feeding and respiratory functions alone (Burrows 2008). In mammals, this mimetic musculature attaches to the skin of the face, thus permitting the facial communication of such emotions as fear, surprise, disgust, and anger. In most mammals, however, a few wide sheetlike muscles are involved, rendering fine information differentiation impossible, whereas in primates, this musculature divides into many independent muscles with distinct points of attachment to the epidermis, thus permitting higher bandwidth facial communication. Humans have the most highly developed facial musculature by far of any primate species, with a degree of involvement of lips and eyes that is not present in any other species. In short, humans have evolved a highly specialized and very costly complex of physiological characteristics that both presuppose and facilitate sophisticated aural and visual communication, whereas communication in other primates, lacking as they are in cumulative culture, goes little beyond simple calling and gesturing capacities. This example is quite a dramatic and concrete illustration of the intimate interaction of genes and culture in the evolution of our species. REFERENCES Belin, P., R. J. Zatorre, P. Lafaille, P. Ahad, and B. Pike, Voice-selective Areas in Human Auditory Cortex, Nature 403 (2000):309 312. Binder, J. R., J. A. Frost, T. A. Hammeke, R. W. Cox, S. M. Rao, and T. Prieto, 4

Human Brain Language Areas Identified by Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging, Journal of Neuroscience 17 (1997):353 362. Boyd, Robert and Peter J. Richerson, Culture and the Evolutionary Process (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1985). Burrows, Anne M., The Facial Expression Musculature in Primates and Its Evolutionary Significance, BioEssays 30,3 (2008):212 225. Cavalli-Sforza, Luigi Luca and Marcus W. Feldman, Cultural Transmission and Evolution (Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1981). Dunbar, Robin M., The Human Story (New York: Faber & Faber, 2005). Gintis, Herbert, Gene-culture Coevolution and the Nature of Human Sociality, Proceedings of the Royal Society B 366 (2011):878 888. Jurmain, Robert, Harry Nelson, Lynn Kilgore, and Wenda Travathan, Introduction to Physical Anthropology (Cincinnati: Wadsworth Publishing Company, 1997). Lumsden, Charles J. and Edward O. Wilson, Genes, Mind, and Culture: The Coevolutionary Process (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1981). Relethford, John H., The Human Species: An Introduction to Biological Anthropology (New York: McGraw-Hill, 2007). cnpapersnhuman EvolutionnHyperCognition.tex December 12, 2016 5