Cognitive modeling with conceptual spaces

Similar documents
Mappings For Cognitive Semantic Interoperability

Spatial Relations for Semantic Similarity Measurement

Alexander Klippel and Chris Weaver. GeoVISTA Center, Department of Geography The Pennsylvania State University, PA, USA

A General Framework for Conflation

Cognitive semantics and cognitive theories of representation: Session 6: Conceptual spaces

GEO-INFORMATION (LAKE DATA) SERVICE BASED ON ONTOLOGY

Preferred Mental Models in Qualitative Spatial Reasoning: A Cognitive Assessment of Allen s Calculus

Convex Hull-Based Metric Refinements for Topological Spatial Relations

Environmental Cognition and Perception I

Dynamic Fuzzy Sets. Introduction

Geospatial Semantics. Yingjie Hu. Geospatial Semantics

Semantic Similarity Models for Geo-spatial Information System

Lecture 1: Geospatial Data Models

Key Words: geospatial ontologies, formal concept analysis, semantic integration, multi-scale, multi-context.

A semantic and language-based model of landscape scenes

Metrics and topologies for geographic space

Intelligent GIS: Automatic generation of qualitative spatial information

A Cognitive Map for an Artificial Agent

Spatial Intelligence. Angela Schwering

Adamatzky, A. (2001). Computing in Nonlinear Media and Automata Collectives.

Hybrid Distance Based Measures for Geospatial Domain

Places as Media of Containment

Cognitive Engineering for Geographic Information Science

Towards Process-Based Ontology for Representing Dynamic Geospatial Phenomena

Spatial Knowledge Acquisition from Addresses

Human-Centered Fidelity Metrics for Virtual Environment Simulations

Kai-Florian Richter Stephan Winter. Landmarks. GIScience for Intelligent Services

From Research Objects to Research Networks: Combining Spatial and Semantic Search

Taxonomies of Building Objects towards Topographic and Thematic Geo-Ontologies

September 16, 2004 The NEURON Book: Chapter 2

PSTs mental models about role and distribution of ozone layer and ozone layer depletion

4CitySemantics. GIS-Semantic Tool for Urban Intervention Areas

The Influence of Scale, Context and Spatial Preposition in Linguistic Topology

A conservation law for map space in portrayal and generalisation

[Figure 1 about here]

Derivation and implementation of a semantic GIS data model informed by principles of cognition

Information-theoretic and Set-theoretic Similarity

How much of symbolic manipulation is just symbol pushing?

Spatial Cognition: From Rat-Research to Multifunctional Spatial Assistance Systems

Towards Usable Topological Operators at GIS User Interfaces

ALGEBRAIC THINKING AND FORMALIZED MATHEMATICS FORMAL REASONING AND THE CONTEXTUAL

Quantum Probability in Cognition. Ryan Weiss 11/28/2018

A classification of spatio-temporal entities based on their location in space-time

A spatial literacy initiative for undergraduate education at UCSB

CHAPTER 12. Geo-Ontologies. Frederico Fonseca and Gilberto Câmara Introduction Ontology and ontologies

A reference landform ontology for automated delineation of depression landforms from DEMs

SPATIAL NARRATIVES FROM MOBILE GIS - the case of H.C.Andersen in Copenhagen

Syntactic Patterns of Spatial Relations in Text

GeoVISTA Center, Department of Geography, The Pennsylvania State University, PA, USA

M inds On GIS: Encouraging Spatial Thinking in an Introductory GIS Lab Sharla Lair

THE ROLE OF COMPUTER BASED TECHNOLOGY IN DEVELOPING UNDERSTANDING OF THE CONCEPT OF SAMPLING DISTRIBUTION

Using C-OWL for the Alignment and Merging of Medical Ontologies

MULTIDIMENSIONAL REPRESENTATION OF GEOGRAPHIC FEATURES. E. Lynn Usery U.S. Geological Survey United States of America

Paper presented at the 9th AGILE Conference on Geographic Information Science, Visegrád, Hungary,

FACTOR ANALYSIS AND MULTIDIMENSIONAL SCALING

MAINTAINING DRAGGING AND THE PIVOT INVARIANT IN PROCESSES OF CONJECTURE GENERATION

The effects of different verbal route instructions on spatial orientation

PROBABILISTIC MATCHING OF MAP OBJECTS IN MULTI-SCALE SPACE

Topologic and Metric Decision Criteria for Wayfinding in the Real World and the WWW

Dynamical Embodiments of Computation in Cognitive Processes James P. Crutcheld Physics Department, University of California, Berkeley, CA a

Predicting Neighbor Goodness in Collaborative Filtering

Similarities and differences between outdoor and indoor space from the perspective of navigation

Semantic Annotation of Geographic Information

Value Constraints In ORM

Ontologies and Spatial Decision Support

Variable/Equality Concept

Modeling Temporal Uncertainty of Events: a. Descriptive Perspective

How context influences the segmentation of movement trajectories an experimental approach for environmental and behavioral context

Ontology Summit 2016: SI Track: SI in the GeoScience Session 1: How is SI Viewed in the GeoSciences"

The endpoint hypothesis: A topological-cognitive assessment of geographic scale movement patterns

AP Human Geography Syllabus

Understanding (dis)similarity measures

Outline. Limits of Bayesian classification Bayesian concept learning Probabilistic models for unsupervised and semi-supervised category learning

Department of Geography University of California, Santa Barbara U.S.A.

via Topos Theory Olivia Caramello University of Cambridge The unification of Mathematics via Topos Theory Olivia Caramello

Relating Land Use to the Landscape Character:

Causal Models Guide Analogical Inference

Using spatial-temporal signatures to infer human activities from personal trajectories on location-enabled mobile devices

Landmarks selection in street map design

Student Visualization and Conceptual Understanding of Atomic Orbitals Using a. Virtual Environment

Visualization Schemes for Spatial Processes

A Computable Language of Architecture

The Logic of Geometric Proof

WS/FCS Unit Planning Organizer

Institute for Geoinformatics University of Münster Germany

CONVEX MEANINGS AND EVOLUTIONARY STABILITY

Geographers Perspectives on the World

A Region-based Direction Heuristic for Solving Underspecified Spatio-temporal Planning Problems

Environmental psychology:

MODAL LOGICS OF SPACE

Semantics of Landscape:

The Concept of Geographic Relevance

Structure learning in human causal induction

GIS and Spatial Statistics: One World View or Two? Michael F. Goodchild University of California Santa Barbara

Course Introduction II

Warmup. geography compass rose culture longitude

This is the published version of a paper presented at International Natural Language Generation (INLG).

Simple Explicit Animation (SEA) Approach in Teaching Organic Chemistry

Maximum Entropy Generative Models for Similarity-based Learning

Transcription:

Cognitive modeling with conceptual spaces Martin Raubal Department of Geography, University of California at Santa Barbara 5713 Ellison Hall, Santa Barbara, CA 93106, U.S.A. raubal@geog.ucsb.edu Abstract. This paper presents a formal approach to cognitive modeling by utilizing conceptual spaces. Conceptual spaces were introduced by Peter Gärdenfors as geometric and topologic representations that mediate between the symbolic and subconceptual cognitive levels. They can be used for knowledge representation and to explain cognitive reasoning. The goal here is to discuss both the advantages of this approach, and limitations regarding its potential as a universal form of cognitive representation that also serves as a representational model for human spatial reasoning. Keywords: Conceptual spaces, concepts, representation, cognitive reasoning. 1 Introduction The work presented here tackles two main topics of the workshop, i.e., formal approaches to cognitive modeling and human-machine interaction in geospatial information systems. Communication between systems and their users is made possible through the mutual understanding of terms and concepts. If we want geospatial services and tools to give better answers to user questions it is necessary to bridge and eventually resolve the discrepancy between user concepts and system concepts. Concepts can be formally represented in conceptual vector spaces sets of quality dimensions within geometrical structures (Gärdenfors, 2000; Raubal, 2004). These spaces allow for representing instances and types of concepts, measuring semantic distances between them, and modeling different contexts by assigning weights to their dimensions. Furthermore, one can semantically compare the system and user spaces by applying projections and transformations (Raubal, 2005). This way it is possible for the system to adapt the semantics of its concepts to the user s semantics, which eventually leads to improved human-computer interaction. From a cognitive modeling perspective, formal conceptual spaces offer a possible mental model as a representation of the mental world, which is a representation of the real world and concerned with the inner workings and processes within the brain and nervous system (Palmer, 1978). This results in various questions concerning the cognitive plausibility and adequacy of conceptual space representations, their possible limitations with regard to explaining mental (spatial) information processing, and their potential to be integrated with other cognitive models.

2 Martin Raubal 2 Formal conceptual spaces The notion of conceptual space was introduced as a framework for representing information at the conceptual level (Gärdenfors, 2000). Conceptual spaces can be utilized for knowledge representation and sharing, and support the paradigm that concepts are dynamical systems (Barsalou, 2003). Sowa (2006) argued that conceptual spaces are a promising geometrical model for representing abstract concepts as well as physical images. A conceptual space is a set of quality dimensions with a geometrical or topological structure for one or more domains. Domains are represented through sets of integral dimensions, which are distinguishable from all other dimensions. For example, the color domain is formed through the dimensions hue, saturation, and brightness. Concepts cover multiple domains and are modeled as n-dimensional regions. Every object or member of the corresponding category is represented as a point in the conceptual space. This allows for expressing the similarity between two objects as the spatial distance between their points. Recent work has focused on representing actions and functional properties in conceptual spaces (Gärdenfors, 2007). In (Raubal, 2004), a methodology to formalize conceptual spaces as vector spaces was presented. Formally, a conceptual vector space is defined as C n = {(c 1, c 2,, c n ) c i C} where the c i are the quality dimensions. A quality dimension can also represent a whole domain and in this case c j = D n = {(d 1, d 2,, d n ) d k D}. Vector spaces have a metric and therefore allow for the calculation of distances between points in the space. This can also be utilized for measuring distances between concepts, either based on their approximation by prototypical points or prototypical regions (Schwering & Raubal, 2005a). The calculation of these semantic distances requires that all quality dimensions of the space are represented in the same relative unit of measurement. Assuming a normal distribution, this is ensured by calculating the z-scores for the individual values (Devore & Peck, 2001). For specifying different contexts one can assign weights to the quality dimensions of a conceptual vector space. This is essential for the representation of concepts as dynamical systems, because the salience of dimensions may change over time. C n is then defined as {(w 1 c 1, w 2 c 2,, w n c n ) c i C, w j W} where W is the set of real numbers. 3 Representing cognitive processes with conceptual spaces It has been argued that due to their modeled quality dimensions, conceptual spaces can be utilized to represent one of the major aspects of semantics and reasoning, i.e., similarity (Gärdenfors, 2004; Goldstone & Son, 2005). Indeed, it is hard to imagine situations in our daily lives that do not in some way include reasoning that involves similarity, e.g., when searching for particular entities, grouping entities into categories, or to be more concrete, simply trying to change a light bulb (Barsalou, 1983). Furthermore, conceptual spaces account for prototype effects by allowing for the representation of prototypes as n-dimensional regions in the space. This is based on Rosch s structural theory of centrality, where prototypical members had been found to correspond to the means of attributes that have a metric (Rosch, 1978).

Cognitive modeling with conceptual spaces 3 Instead of approximating prototypes and concepts by points (i.e., vectors) in the space, it is possible to represent them by regions, which allows for integrating shape and size within the similarity measure (Schwering & Raubal, 2005a). This also accounts for the fact that people s similarity judgments are asymmetric (A. Tversky, 1977). Conceptual spaces are inherently spatial in that they are modeled by a geometrical or topological structure. In order to be used as an adequate model for spatial reasoning though, it is necessary to explicitly represent spatial relations, in particular topologic and metric relations. When determining semantic similarity between geospatial concepts, spatial relations play a major role in the calculation process. All geospatial objects have a position in space with regard to some spatial reference system and consequently a spatial relation to each other. Spatial relations are therefore also central characteristics on the conceptual level (Donnelly & Bittner, 2005). Schwering & Raubal (2005b) presented an approach of integrating spatial relations into semantic similarity measurements between different geospatial concepts represented in conceptual spaces. It was demonstrated that such integration improves the quality of the measurements by enhancing the accuracy of their results. On the downside, representing spatial relations as quality dimensions in the conceptual space is a tedious process because each dimension does not only need to represent one concept and its values, but one concept and its values with regard to a second concept (when modeled as binary relations). Figure 1 shows an example from the domain of hydrology by representing a nearness relation. In this research, the simplifying assumption was made that quality dimensions of a conceptual space are independent. This is often not true and it is therefore necessary to investigate the covariances between dimensions, and to account for these in the conceptual space representations. nearness to flooding area river yes not specified river low nearness near very near Figure 1: Modeling relations as dimensions on Boolean and ordinal scale (from (Schwering & Raubal, 2005b). Conceptual spaces as cognitive models can also be used to explain processes of learning and forgetting, which correspond to the addition and subtraction of quality dimensions, and changes within the internal structure and scale of particular dimensions. 3 Open research questions Although conceptual spaces have proven to be promising candidates for cognitive models in terms of representing concepts and instances, and serving as an explanatory framework for cognitive processes, several open questions remain:

4 Martin Raubal 1. What is the potential of conceptual spaces for serving as a universal form of cognitive representation? Using geometric models is only one possible way of representing information at the conceptual level. Different views on the nature of conceptual representations in the human cognitive system exist, such as mental imagery (Kosslyn, 1994) or schematic perceptual images extracted from modes of experience (Barsalou et al., 1993). Could such images be represented in or combined with conceptual spaces? Would such combination be similar to a cognitive collage (B. Tversky, 1993)? What are the components of a hybrid cognitive model similar to the argument for unified theories (instead of theory) of cognition made by (Newell, 1990) that covers the whole ground? Human participants tests may help assess the validity of geometrical representations of concepts and point to limitations of conceptual spaces as a representational model. 2. When formally specifying conceptual spaces, the simplifying assumption is often made that the quality dimensions of the conceptual spaces are independent. As previously stated, this is not always true. It will be necessary to investigate the covariances between dimensions and to account for these in the representations of the conceptual spaces. Again, human participants tests are a way to identify the quality dimensions for a concept and to infer their dependencies, which would lead to nonorthogonal axes in the representation. 3. Several researchers have argued against a geometric approach for concept representation and similarity measurement for the reasons that the axioms of minimality, symmetry, and triangle inequality do not hold cognitively. With conceptual vector spaces it seems possible to account for these phenomena by assigning different weights depending on the context. In this way, dissimilarity of the same concept depending on the viewpoint and asymmetric semantic distances could be represented. The axiom of triangle inequality seems to be violated only when different contexts are mixed (e.g., geographical and political), such as in the example given by (A. Tversky, 1977). It seems though that accounting for context needs more than assigning weights. For example, context rules (Keßler, Raubal, & Janowicz, 2007) may be applied to conceptual spaces that allow for both addition and removal of dimensions, but also for a re-segmentation of dimensions. 4. In order to evaluate the potential of conceptual spaces as an approach to modeling mental spatial information processing for human spatial reasoning, more tests and simulations with geographic use cases are required see (Barkowsky, 2002) for an example within the domain of visual imagery. Such simulations will further demonstrate the advantages and limitations of conceptual spaces as a representational model for human spatial reasoning. References Barkowsky, T. (2002). Mental Representation and Processing of Geographic Knowledge (Vol. 2541). Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer. Barsalou, L. (1983). Ad hoc categories. Memory & Cognition, 11, 211-227. Barsalou, L. (2003). Situated simulation in the human conceptual system. Language and Cognitive Processes, 5(6), 513-562.

Cognitive modeling with conceptual spaces 5 Barsalou, L., Yeh, W., Luka, B., Olseth, K., Mix, K., & Wu, L. (1993). Concepts and meaning. In K. Beals, G. Cooke, K. Kathman, K. McCullough, S. Kita & D. Testen (Eds.), Papers from the parasession on conceptual representations (Vol. 29, pp. 23-61): University of Chicago: Chicago Linguistics Society. Devore, J., & Peck, R. (2001). Statistics - The Exploration and Analysis of Data (Fourth Edition). Pacific Grove, CA: Duxbury. Donnelly, M., & Bittner, T. (2005). Spatial Relations Between Classes of Individuals. In A. Cohn & D. Mark (Eds.), Spatial Information Theory - International Conference, COSIT 2005, Ellicottville, NY, USA (Vol. 3693, pp. 182-199). Berlin: Springer. Gärdenfors, P. (2000). Conceptual Spaces - The Geometry of Thought. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books, MIT Press. Gärdenfors, P. (2004). How to Make the Semantic Web More Semantic. In A. Varzi & L. Lieu (Eds.), Formal Ontology In Information Systems (pp. 17-34). Amsterdam: IOS Press. Gärdenfors, P. (2007). Representing actions and functional properties in conceptual spaces. In T. Ziemke, J. Zlatev & R. Frank (Eds.), Body, Language and Mind (Vol. 1: Embodiment, pp. 167-195). Berlin, Germany: Mouton de Gruyter. Goldstone, R., & Son, J. (2005). Similarity. In K. Holyoak & R. Morrison (Eds.), Cambridge Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Keßler, C., Raubal, M., & Janowicz, K. (2007). The Effect of Context on Semantic Similarity Measurement. In R. Meersman, Z. Tari & P. Herrero (Eds.), On The Move - OTM 2007 Workshops, Part II - 3rd International IFIP Workshop On Semantic Web & Web Semantics (SWWS 07), Vilamoura, Algarve, Portugal. (Vol. 4806, pp. 1274-1284). Berlin: Springer. Kosslyn, S. (1994). Image and brain - The resolution of the imagery debate. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Newell, A. (1990). Unified Theories of Cognition (paperback ed.). Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press. Palmer, S. (1978). Fundamental aspects of cognitive representation. In E. Rosch & B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 259-303). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Raubal, M. (2004). Formalizing Conceptual Spaces. In A. Varzi & L. Vieu (Eds.), Formal Ontology in Information Systems, Proceedings of the Third International Conference (FOIS 2004) (Vol. 114, pp. 153-164). Amsterdam, NL: IOS Press. Raubal, M. (2005). Mappings For Cognitive Semantic Interoperability. In F. Toppen & M. Painho (Eds.), AGILE 2005-8th Conference on Geographic Information Science (pp. 291-296). Lisboa, Portugal: Instituto Geografico Portugues (IGP). Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of Categorization. In E. Rosch & B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and Categorization (pp. 27-48). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Schwering, A., & Raubal, M. (2005a). Measuring Semantic Similarity between Geospatial Conceptual Regions. In A. Rodriguez, I. Cruz, M. Egenhofer & S. Levashkin (Eds.), GeoSpatial Semantics - First International Conference, GeoS 2005, Mexico City, Mexico, November 2005 (Vol. 3799, pp. 90-106). Berlin: Springer. Schwering, A., & Raubal, M. (2005b). Spatial Relations for Semantic Similarity Measurement. In J. Akoka, S. Liddle, I.-Y. Song, M. Bertolotto, I. Comyn-Wattiau, W.-J. vanden Heuvel, M. Kolp, J. Trujillo, C. Kop & H. Mayr (Eds.), Perspectives in Conceptual Modeling: ER 2005 Workshops CAOIS, BP-UML, CoMoGIS, ecomo, and QoIS, Klagenfurt, Austria, October 24-28, 2005. (Vol. 3770, pp. 259-269). Berlin: Springer. Sowa, J. (2006). Categorization in Cognitive Computer Science. In H. Cohen & C. Lefebvre (Eds.), Handbook of Categorization in Cognitive Science (pp. 141-163): Elsevier. Tversky, A. (1977). Features of Similarity. Psychological Review, 84(4), 327-352. Tversky, B. (1993). Cognitive Maps, Cognitive Collages, and Spatial Mental Model. In A. Frank & I. Campari (Eds.), Spatial Information Theory: Theoretical Basis for GIS (Vol. 716, pp. 14-24). Heidelberg-Berlin: Springer.