The role of habitat heterogeneity on signal evolution in predator prey. interactions, with implications for the evolution of crypsis and aposematism

Similar documents
Frequency-dependent selection with background

Coevolution of predators and prey

Non-independence in Statistical Tests for Discrete Cross-species Data

Behavior of the Day! Macroevolutionary Patterns of Behavior. Adaptive and Non-adaptive behavior. Adaptation vs Exaptation. Historical Hypotheses

-The study of the interactions between the different species in an area

Natal versus breeding dispersal: Evolution in a model system

Can the dietary conservatism of predators compensate for positive frequency dependent selection against rare, conspicuous prey?

1.) The traits that help an organism survive in a particular environment are selected in natural selection. Natural Selection

IV. Comparative Anatomy

Who developed the theory of acquired characteristics? a. Darwin b. Lamarck c. Charles Lyell d. Aristotle

Ecology - Defined. Introduction. scientific study. interaction of plants and animals and their interrelationships with the physical environment

organism Community Ecology population community ecosystem biosphere

Chapter 22: Descent with Modification

Unit 8: Ecology Guided Reading Questions (60 pts total)

Section 4 Professor Donald McFarlane

The concept of adaptive phenotypic polymorphism in evolutionary theory

PREY CONCEALMENT: VISUAL BACKGROUND COMPLEXITY AND PREY CONTRAST

1315 q 2005 The Royal Society

Principles of Ecology BL / ENVS 402 Exam II Name:

organism population community ecosystem biosphere Community Ecology AP Biology

organism population community ecosystem biosphere Community Ecology AP Biology

11.6. Patterns in Evolution. Evolution through natural selection is not random.

BIOS 5970: Plant-Herbivore Interactions Dr. Stephen Malcolm, Department of Biological Sciences

Assessing Evolutionary Reasoning of Introductory Biology Students

Testing adaptive hypotheses What is (an) adaptation? Testing adaptive hypotheses What is (an) adaptation?

Introduction interspecific interactions

Are individuals in a population of a species the same?

9 Interactions symbioses. Classical Population Biology

Outline. Ecology: Succession and Life Strategies. Interactions within communities of organisms. Key Concepts:

Chapter 44. Table of Contents. Section 1 Development of Behavior. Section 2 Types of Animal Behavior. Animal Behavior

Ecology Notes CHANGING POPULATIONS

Foundations of animal behaviour. Niaux caves, France. Roz Dakin. roslyndakin.com/biol321. Lecture notes posted here

I. Theories of Evolution Evolution: Adaptation: Jean Baptiste de Lamarck: a) Use & Disuse: b) Inheritance of Acquired Characteristics:

WEATHER, CLIMATE AND ADAPTATIONS OF ANIMALS TO CLIMATE

Adaptive Traits. Natural selection results in evolution of adaptations. Adaptation: trait that enhances an organism's survival and reproduction

Introduction to Navigation

4/17/17. Community Ecology populations interact? Community Ecology. Niche. Community all the organisms that live together in a place interactions

Environmental signals

6 TH. Most Species Compete with One Another for Certain Resources. Species Interact in Five Major Ways. Some Species Evolve Ways to Share Resources

Biology 2017 Mr. Johnson

Interspecific Interactions: Symbiosys

Amazing Adaptations!

Name Date Class. Patterns of Evolution

EVOLUTION. - Selection, Survival, and Drift

Evolutionary Forces. What changes populations (Ch. 17)

Half Hollow Hills High School AP Biology

Gene Pool The combined genetic material for all the members of a population. (all the genes in a population)

Adaptation. Survival of the Fittest

Super Selection. 7 th Grade. Concepts. Objectives. Outline. Duration Pre-Visit: 40 minutes Museum Visit: 60 minutes Post Visit: 50 minutes

Biology Principles of Ecology Oct. 20 and 27, 2011 Natural Selection on Gall Flies of Goldenrod. Introduction

Predators Punish Local Aposematic Morph in a Polymorphic Species. Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, Tulane University, New Orleans, LA

LEANE CLOTTER RYAN DEAN TRABER FISCHER WILLIAM FRY TARA HALTERMAN TENESHA HILL SARA LYONS MIKE MOYER BRITTNIE PECK RYAN SLAUBAUGH TAHIRA WHITE

Companion to Cells, Heredity & Classification Student Resources

Bio 1M: The evolution of apes. 1 Example. 2 Patterns of evolution. Similarities and differences. History

Chapter 15 Evolution

Structure and Function

Ecology- The study of the interractions of organisms with their physical environment and with each other and of the results of such interactions.

IV. Natural Selection

Biology Chapter 15 Evolution Notes

Morgan County School District Re-3 Revised Nov. 2007

MCDB 1111 corebio 2017 Midterm I

THE THEORY OF EVOLUTION

Why some measures of fluctuating asymmetry are so sensitive to measurement error

Ecology. Outline Principles of Ecology. Definition of ecology Hierarchy of relationships. Ecosystems & Energy Flow Populations & Exponential Growth

ANIMAL COMMUNICATION III. CONTEXT A. PARENT OFFSPRING BEGGING IN CHICKS

ESTIMATION OF CONSERVATISM OF CHARACTERS BY CONSTANCY WITHIN BIOLOGICAL POPULATIONS

Essential Questions. What factors are most significant in structuring a community?

Theory of Evolution. Mr. Rafferty 5-19

chatper 17 Multiple Choice Identify the choice that best completes the statement or answers the question.

Natural Selection Study Guide Answer Key

Community Structure. Community An assemblage of all the populations interacting in an area

Chapter 1 Biology: Exploring Life

Structural Adaptations

Evolution. Chapters 16 & 17

Daniel T. Blumstein and William E. Cooper, Jr.

3U Evolution Notes. Natural Selection: What is Evolution? -The idea that gene distribution changes over time -A change in the frequency of an allele

Darwin s theory of evolution by natural selection

Ch 22 Descent with Modification Darwin was influenced by the work of others during his time.

Chapter 4 Ecosystems and Living Organisms

Natural Selection. Charles Darwin & Alfred Russell Wallace

UNIT 4: EVOLUTION Chapter 10: Principles of Evolution. I. Early Ideas about Evolution (10.1) A. Early scientists proposed ideas about evolution

Predator behavior influences predator-prey population dynamics. Predator behavior influences predator-prey population dynamics

Chapter 53 Community Ecology

1. A group of individuals of the same species which live together in the same area at the same time is called a.

6. Which of the following is not a basic need of all animals a. food b. friends c. water d. protection from predators

Camouflage through behavior in moths: the role of background matching and disruptive coloration

Adaptation. Adaptation describes any trait that enhances an organisms fitness or increases its chance of survival.

Table of Contents. Pages 2-4. Pages 5-7. Pages Pages Pages Tropical Rainforest Reading Passage. Scaffolded Questions

The Mechanisms of Evolution

GAUTENG DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION SENIOR SECONDARY INTERVENTION PROGRAMME LIFE SCIENCES GRADE 12 SESSION 4 (LEARNER NOTES)

Factors Affecting Rate of Food Consumption

OPTIMAL FORAGING MODELS

Adaptations and Natural Selection. Adaptations and Natural Selection

PENNSYLVANIA. All living things need to interact in an ecosystem in order to survive.

ANIMAL COMMUNICATION

Chapter 16. Darwin s Theory Of Evolution

Lecture #4-1/25/02 Dr. Kopeny

e.g. population: 500, two alleles: Red (R) and White (r). Total: 1000 genes for flower color in the population

ECOSYSTEMS AND THEIR LIVING COMMUNITIES

Adaptations Questions

Transcription:

1 2 The role of habitat heterogeneity on signal evolution in predator prey interactions, with implications for the evolution of crypsis and aposematism 3 PrePrints 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 Marcio R. Pie Departamento de Zoologia, Universidade Federal do Paraná, C.P. 19020, 81531 980, Curitiba, PR, Brazil. * Corresponding author: pie@ufpr.br Abstract Little is known about mechanisms promoting the diversity of antipredation strategies found in nature. In this study I explore how habitat heterogeneity may have profound effects on predator prey communication. Habitat heterogeneity inevitably increases background noise in communication systems. Therefore, as habitat heterogeneity increases, an organism must produce more signal in order to convey the same amount of information (signal to noise ratio; S/N). Therefore, all else equal, strategies maximizing S/N (e.g. aposematism, sexually selected traits) should become more exacerbated as habitat heterogeneity increases, whereas strategies minimizing S/N (e.g. crypsis) should show the opposite trend. A test of this hypothesis is provided based on information on detection-avoidance strategies ofghanaian preying mantids. If this hypothesis is widely applicable, it can provide a parsimonious explanation for the origin of aposematism, given that the same organisms can become more cryptic or aposematic simply by shifting their habitats and the corresponding levels of background noise. Key wordswaldo effect, camouflage, antipredation, sexual selection, sensory drive.

26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 From the brightly coloured monarch butterfly to the exquisite crypsis found in stick insects, the bewildering diversity of antipredation strategies have not only intrigued naturalists for centuries, but also were a main theme during the development of the theory of evolution by natural selection. The primary function of antipredation strategies such as crypsis and aposematism are fairly obvious, yet degree of elaboration of these strategies vary considerably among species (Ruxton et al. 2004; Stevens and Merilaita 2011). For instance, strategies to evade detection from predators can vary from a general overall green coloration to a highly elaborate imitation of a leaf, including counterfeit veins and signs of herbivory (Ruxton et al. 2004). Although mechanisms such as differential predation pressures or anatomical constraints might play an important role in explaining variation in the degree of elaboration of antipredation strategies, in this study I explore a simple, parsimonious explanation that could nevertheless involve considerable explanatory power. I suggested elsewhere (Pie 2005) that the sensory drive model developed by Endler (1992; 1993) could be applied in the context of prey predator interactions. One of the implications of this model is that much of the variation in prey predator strategies may be described in terms of signal to noise ratios (S/N): organisms may evolve strategies that either to minimize S/N or to maximize it (Endler 1978; Endler 1993). For heuristic purposes, S/N can be decoupled into signal and noise components. Usually the organism has more control over the former, since environmental noise is independent of the presence of the organism. On the other hand, the noise component may vary according to differences in habitat heterogeneity: simpler environments provide less background noise than highly heterogeneous environments. If we assume that noise increases with habitat heterogeneity, for the same amount of signal, S/N should decrease with habitat heterogeneity (Fig. 1). Consequently, animals in highly 2

51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75 heterogeneous environments must invest more signal than animals in simpler environments to convey the same amount of information. Given that these assumptions are likely to be highly applicable in many contexts, the resulting trade off should have profound consequences for signal design and evolution. Let us first consider the influence of habitat heterogeneity on a strategy that maximizes S/N such as aposematism. A very simple environment (e.g. desert) provides little background noise. Thus even a small amount of signal is sufficient for an aposematic animal to be conspicuous. Alternatively, a complex environment such as a rainforest provides an enormous amount of noise; a visual signal of an aposematic animal may easily be missed among the variety of other signals and light environments. Therefore, to be as conspicuous as its desert counterpart, a forest animal should provide more signal to generate the same S/N. The same arguments could simply be reversed in relation to strategies that minimize S/N. For instance, deserts provide little noise, forcing cryptic organisms to use elaborate strategies to attain a low S/N ratio. This same level of S/N could be attained more easily by a forest animal, given the high level of background noise. An effective way of conveying this principle could be to refer to it as the Waldo effect, after the famous cartoon book by Martin Handford (1997). The drawing representing Waldo is the same in all situations (the same amount of signal), but his detection by the reader varies according to the background situation (varying amounts of noise, providing different S/N). It is noteworthy that heterogeneity depends on the sensory system: deserts may be highly homogeneous in relation to visual signals and light environments, but may be highly heterogeneous in wind currents, which can have a strong influence on the evolution of olfactory and/or sound cues (e.g. Legnagne et al. 1999. See also Ruxton 2009). Also, the nature of the heterogeneity in a given habitat 3

76 77 78 79 80 81 82 83 84 85 86 87 88 89 90 91 92 93 94 95 96 97 98 99 100 depends largely on its scale. If habitat heterogeneity is spatially coarse grained, S/N may be low or high depending upon which patch the animal signals in. Conversely, if the spatial heterogeneity is fine grained, the visual background is essentially homogeneous (Endler 1988). To the best of my knowledge, this simple model has never been tested explicitly. However, there is some empirical evidence supporting its predictions. Marchetti (1993) found a negative correlation between the presence of bright patches in the plumage of Phylloscopus warblers and brightness of their respective habitat. These bright patches have been shown to function in intraspecific territoriality (Marchetti 1993) and in prey flush foraging (Jablonski 1996; 1999). Either way, the brightness of the habitat is clearly associated with habitat heterogeneity (open/closed habitats). Also, there is experimental evidence for a role of background noise on prey detection (e.g. Dimitrova and Merilaita 2010; 2012). As a first approach to test this hypothesis I collated data on the relative frequency of different kinds of crypsis in different environments using published records of praying mantids. Robinson (1969; 1981) discriminates two kinds of crypsis: eucrypsis and special protective resemblance. The term eucrypsis denotes simple devices such as homochromy, countershading and disruptive coloration, whereas special protective resemblance involves more elaborate strategies such as flower, bark, leaf and stick mimicry. (Although there has been a strong controversy over the validity of different classifications of cryptic signals (see Robinson 1981 and additional papers on the same issue), for the current purposes I will assume that Robinson s classification is appropriate.) Given two habitats that differ in heterogeneity such as a savannah and a rainforest, we would expect that S/N minimizing strategies should be less elaborate in the rainforest and more elaborate in the savannah. In other words: eucrypsis should be more common in the complex 4

101 102 103 104 105 106 107 108 109 110 111 112 113 114 115 116 117 118 119 120 121 122 123 124 125 environment (forest), whereas special protective resemblance should be more common in the simpler environment (savannah). Using reviews in Edmunds (1972; 1976) I was able to examine information on 40 mantid species. Although not strictly a random sample, the original purposes of the author should not bias these results in any specific direction. As predicted, among the savannah mantids, 15 out of 18 cryptic species had special protective resemblance (83.3%), in contrast to 10 out of 22 species from the forest (45.4%; p=0.015, Fisher's exact test). Also, another interesting pattern arose from this analysis. In addition to crypsis, many mantid species have startle displays (a S/N maximizing strategy) as a backup strategy. Interestingly, although not systematically quantified, Edmunds s descriptions suggest that startle displays were generally more dramatic among forest species, frequently including sounds in addition to the visual signals. The framework explored in the present study has interesting implications for a long standing conundrum in evolutionary biology: the origin of aposematism. The evolution of this antipredation strategy has been difficult to explain because rare conspicuous mutants should experience a higher cost of discovery by predators relative to the cryptic majority, while at frequencies that would be exceedingly low to lead to predator aversion learning. A variety of mechanisms have been proposed to explain the early stages of aposematism, such as predator dietary conservatism (Thomas et al. 2003, Speed 2001), gregariousness (Mappes and Alatalo 1997), density dependent phenotypic plasticity (Sword 2002), kin selection (Malcolm 1986), and variation in selective pressures over space and time (Mappes et al. 2005). However, many of these hypotheses either lack the generality expected for such a taxonomically and ecologically widespread phenomenon, or simply push the explanation one step back (ex. why should predators innately avoid brightly coloured 5

126 127 128 129 130 131 132 133 134 135 136 137 138 139 140 141 142 143 144 145 146 147 148 149 prey?). On the other hand, the dramatic changes in conspicuousness of different species according to the background is highly familiar during field work, such as the sudden "disappearance" of clear winged ithomiine butterflies as soon as they leave a clearing and enter a patch of forest. The existence of such distinct levels of S/N in neighbouring habitats could lead to the establishment of an "enemy free space" (Jeffries and Lawton 1984), such that a species can be simultaneously cryptic in visually noisy background and conspicuous in a neighbouring simple background, thus circumventing many of the limitations of previous hypotheses. The conclusions drawn here should not be restricted to predator prey interactions. Analogous effects should be evident in other systems where S/N is minimized or maximized, such as sexual selection (e.g. Price 1996). Also, the use of more precise measures of signal and noise (Endler 1990) should provide quantitative tests of this hypothesis, which could be a prolific area for future research. Acknowledgements I thank A. Duran and C. S. Schneider for valuable comments on a previous version of this manuscript. This study was partially funded by a research grant from CNPq (304897/2012 4). References Dimitrova M, Merilaita S (2010) Prey concealment: visual background complexity and prey contrast distribution.behav Ecol 21: 176 181. Dimitrova M, Merilaita S (2012) Prey pattern regularity and background complexity affect detectability of background matching prey. Behav Ecol 23: 384 390. 6

150 151 152 153 154 155 156 157 158 159 160 161 162 163 164 165 166 167 168 169 170 171 172 173 174 Edmunds M (1972) Defensive behaviour in Ghanaian praying mantids. Zool J Linn Soc 51: 1 32. Edmunds M (1976) The defensive behaviour of Ghanaian preying mantids with a discussion of territoriality.zool J Linn Soc 58: 1 37. Endler, JA (1978) A predator s view of animal color patterns. Evol Ecol 11: 319 364. Endler, JA (1988) Frequency dependent predation, crypsis, and aposematic coloration. Phil Trans Royal Soc London B 319: 505 523. Endler JA (1990) On the measurement and classification of colour studies of animal colour patterns. Biol J Linn Soc 41: 316 352. Endler JA (1992) Signals, signal conditions and the direction of evolution. Am Nat 139: S125 S153. Endler JA (1993) Some general comments on the evolution and design of animal communication systems. Phil Trans Royal Soc 340: 215 225. Handford M (1997) Where's Waldo? The Wonder Book. Candlewick Press, Cambridge, MA. Jablonski P (1996) Dark habitats and bright birds, warblers may use wing patches to flush prey. Oikos 75: 350 354. Jablonski P (1999) A rare predator exploits prey escape behavior, the role of tail fanning and plumage contrast in foraging of the painted redstart (Myioborus pictus). Behav. Ecol. 10: 7 14. Jeffries MJ, Lawton JH (1984) Enemy free space and the structure of ecological communities. Biol J Linn Soc 23: 269 286. Legnane T, Aubin T, Lauga J, Jouventin P (1999) How do king penguin (Aptenodytes patagonicus) apply the mathematical theory of information to communicate in windy conditions? Proc R Soc London B 266: 163 28. 7

175 176 177 178 179 180 181 182 183 184 185 186 187 188 189 190 191 192 193 194 195 196 197 198 Malcolm SB (1986) Aposematism in a soft bodied insect: a case for kin selection. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18: 387 393. Mappes J, Alatalo, RV (1997) Effects of novelty and gregariousness in survival of aposematic prey. Behav Ecol 8: 174 177. Mappes J, Marples N, Endler JA (2005) The complex business of survival by aposematism. Trends Ecol Evol20, 598 603. Marchetti K (1993) Dark habitats and bright birds illustrate the role of the environment in species divergence. Nature 362: 149 152. Pie MR (2005) Signal evolution in prey recognition systems. Behav Proc 68: 47 50. Price T (1996) An association of habitat with color dimorphism in finches. Auk 113: 256 257. Robinson MH (1969) The defensive behaviour of some orthopteroid insects form Panama. Trans Royal Soc London 121: 281 303. Robinson MH (1981) A stick is a stick and not worth eating, on the definition of mimicry. Biol J Linn Soc 16: 15 20. Ruxton GD (2009) Non visual crypsis: a review of the empirical evidence for camouflage to senses other than vision. Phil Trans Royal Soc B 364: 549 557. Ruxton GD, Sherratt TN, Speed MP (2004) Avoiding Attack: The Evolutionary Ecology of Crypsis, Warning Signals and Mimicry. Oxford University Press, Oxford. Speed MP (2001) Can receiver psychology explain the evolution of aposematism? Anim Behav 61: 205 216. Stevens M, Merilaita S eds. (2011)Animal camouflage: mechanisms and function. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. 8

199 200 201 202 203 204 Sword, G.A. (2002) A role for phenotypic plasticity in the evolution of aposematism. Proc R Soc London B 269: 1639 1644. Thomas RJ, Marples NM, Cuthill IC, Takahashi M, Gibson EA (2003) Dietary conservatism may facilitate the initial evolution of aposematism. Oikos 101: 458 466. Yachi S, Higashi M (1998) The evolution of warning signals. Nature 394: 882 884. 205 9

206 207 208 PrePrints 209 210 211 Fig. 1. An illustration of the principle presented in the present study. The "X" in both 212 figures represents the same amount of signal, yet it is more easily detected in the 213 figure on the right given the lower level of background noise. 10