Supplemental Information. Noise and Correlations. in Parallel Perceptual Decision Making. Thomas U. Otto and Pascal Mamassian

Similar documents
Is the Human Visual System Invariant to Translation and Scale?

!) + log(t) # n i. The last two terms on the right hand side (RHS) are clearly independent of θ and can be

12/2/15. G Perception. Bayesian Decision Theory. Laurence T. Maloney. Perceptual Tasks. Testing hypotheses. Estimation

Brightness induction: Unequal spatial integration with increments and decrements

Mathematical Tools for Neuroscience (NEU 314) Princeton University, Spring 2016 Jonathan Pillow. Homework 8: Logistic Regression & Information Theory

Recipes for the Linear Analysis of EEG and applications

Contrast gain control: a bilinear model for chromatic selectivity

SV6: Polynomial Regression and Neural Networks

Temporal context calibrates interval timing

SIGNAL DETECTION BY HUMAN OBSERVERS" Prof. J. A. Swets P. D. Donahue Louise Iarussi

Supplementary Figure 1. Characterization of the single-photon quantum light source based on spontaneous parametric down-conversion (SPDC).

The influence of lightness, and the crispening effect on the perceived contrast of textured images

Context-Dependent Decay of Motor Memories during Skill Acquisition

The effect of speaking rate and vowel context on the perception of consonants. in babble noise

Surround effects on the shape of the temporal contrast-sensitivity function

Low-level correlations between object properties and viewpoint can cause viewpoint-dependent object recognition

BASIC VISUAL SCIENCE CORE

AC : STATISTICAL PROCESS CONTROL LABORATORY EXERCISES FOR ALL ENGINEERING DISCIPLINES

A DECISION-MAKING THEORY OF VISUAL DETECTION l

Hypothesis Testing in Action: t-tests

Humans use continuous visual feedback from the hand to control both the direction and distance of pointing movements

Acoustics 08 Paris 6013

Spatial pattern summation is phase-insensitive in the fovea but not in the periphery

Geophysical Research Letters. Supporting Information for

Supplemental Data: Appendix

Structured matrix factorizations. Example: Eigenfaces

Practical Statistics

Comparing Color and Leader Line Approaches for Highlighting in Geovisualization

Extending a biologically inspired model of choice: multialternatives, nonlinearity and value-based multidimensional choice

The homogeneous Poisson process

Adaptation to a 'spatial-frequency doubled' stimulus

Concerns of the Psychophysicist. Three methods for measuring perception. Yes/no method of constant stimuli. Detection / discrimination.

Lecture 5: ANOVA and Correlation

Single-Trial Neural Correlates. of Arm Movement Preparation. Neuron, Volume 71. Supplemental Information

Signal Modeling Techniques in Speech Recognition. Hassan A. Kingravi

Simultaneous lightness contrast with double increments

CS Homework 3. October 15, 2009

Review of the role of uncertainties in room acoustics

Internally generated preactivation of single neurons in human medial frontal cortex predicts volition

Robustness and Distribution Assumptions

Things you always wanted to know about statistics but were afraid to ask

Københavns Universitet. Testing the race inequality Gondan, Matthias; Heckel, A. Published in: Journal of Mathematical Psychology

The idiosyncratic nature of confidence

Supplementary Materials for

PATTERN RECOGNITION AND MACHINE LEARNING

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn.2283

An evaluation of different measures of color saturation

CS 195-5: Machine Learning Problem Set 1

LivePhoto Physics Activity 3. Velocity Change. Motion Detector. Sample

6 Extending a biologically inspired model of choice: multi-alternatives, nonlinearity, and value-based multidimensional choice

California Content Standard. Essentials for Algebra (lesson.exercise) of Test Items. Grade 6 Statistics, Data Analysis, & Probability.

Inferring Visuomotor Priors for Sensorimotor Learning

2. What are the tradeoffs among different measures of error (e.g. probability of false alarm, probability of miss, etc.)?

William H. Bauman III * NASA Applied Meteorology Unit / ENSCO, Inc. / Cape Canaveral Air Force Station, Florida

Outline. 1. Basics of gravitational wave transient signal searches. 2. Reconstruction of signal properties

Prepare for this experiment!

The Structure and Timescales of Heat Perception in Larval Zebrafish

Artificial Neural Networks D B M G. Data Base and Data Mining Group of Politecnico di Torino. Elena Baralis. Politecnico di Torino

Comparison of Modern Stochastic Optimization Algorithms

Brightness of Different Hues Is a Single Psychophysical Ratio Scale of Intensity

Concepts and Applications of Kriging. Eric Krause

Problem Set 2. MAS 622J/1.126J: Pattern Recognition and Analysis. Due: 5:00 p.m. on September 30

Pattern Recognition. Parameter Estimation of Probability Density Functions

Review for Exam 1. Erik G. Learned-Miller Department of Computer Science University of Massachusetts, Amherst Amherst, MA

NINE CHOICE SERIAL REACTION TIME TASK

Every animal is represented by a blue circle. Correlation was measured by Spearman s rank correlation coefficient (ρ).

Multimedia Interactive Simulations of Physical Experiments

Chem 1 Kinetics. Objectives. Concepts

Learning features by contrasting natural images with noise

Motion influences the Effects of Systematic Chromatic Changes

Strength-based and time-based supervised networks: Theory and comparisons. Denis Cousineau Vision, Integration, Cognition lab. Université de Montréal

Principal Component Analysis CS498

If we want to analyze experimental or simulated data we might encounter the following tasks:

1 ** The performance objectives highlighted in italics have been identified as core to an Algebra II course.

Psychology 282 Lecture #4 Outline Inferences in SLR

Comparison of receptive fields to polar and Cartesian stimuli computed with two kinds of models

Problem Set 2. MAS 622J/1.126J: Pattern Recognition and Analysis. Due: 5:00 p.m. on September 30

Sociology 6Z03 Review II

Mathematics: applications and interpretation SL

Released 2013 Achievement Test. Mathematics GRADE

Outlines: (June 11, 1996) Instructor:

NEVER IGNORE A COINCIDENCE

Quantum dynamics I. Peter Kvam. Michigan State University Max Planck Institute for Human Development

Functional form of motion priors in human motion perception

Fundamentals to Biostatistics. Prof. Chandan Chakraborty Associate Professor School of Medical Science & Technology IIT Kharagpur

Introduction to Bayesian Learning. Machine Learning Fall 2018

Midterm: CS 6375 Spring 2015 Solutions

Concepts and Applications of Kriging. Eric Krause Konstantin Krivoruchko

encoding and estimation bottleneck and limits to visual fidelity

Detection theory. H 0 : x[n] = w[n]

Two-Sample Inferential Statistics

Exercises. Chapter 1. of τ approx that produces the most accurate estimate for this firing pattern.

Land Navigation Table of Contents

Acoustic Quantities. LMS Test.Lab. Rev 12A

Galaxy Zoo. Materials Computer Internet connection

Transformation of stimulus correlations by the retina

Driven Harmonic Oscillator

Georgia Standards of Excellence Algebra I

Final Examination CS540-2: Introduction to Artificial Intelligence

AstroBITS: Open Cluster Project

Transcription:

Current Biology, Volume 22 Supplemental Information Noise and Correlations in Parallel Perceptual Decision Making Thomas U. Otto and Pascal Mamassian Supplemental Inventory Figure S1 (related to Figure 1) Figure S2 (related to Figure 2) Figure S3 (related to Figure 3) Figure S4 (related to Figure 4) Table S1 (a list of conditions with best-fitting recinormal distributions) Table S2 (a list of best-fitting model parameters) Supplemental Experimental Procedures Supplemental References

Figure S1. Predictions for Condition R MS Based on Probability Summation and the Corresponding Single Conditions S M and S S The framework is introduced in Figure 1, main text. (A) We systematically varied the correlation coefficient. The expected speed-up of latencies is largest assuming a maximal negative correlation. Critically, Boole s inequality (see S M +S S ) provides an upper bound that is not violated. The prediction assuming statistical independence is highlighted by a broken line ( =0; see Equation 1, main text). (B) We extended the probability summation model by allowing for additional noise when signals are processed simultaneously. With additional noise (in % relative to single conditions), violations of Boole s inequality occur for fastest latencies similar to empirical distributions (see Figure 2B, main text). Critically, the speed-up of fastest responses comes at the cost of a slow-down of slowest responses. Hence, latencies are overall more variable.

Figure S2. The Redundant Signal Effect with Color and Sound Signals Results are analogous to the experiment with motion and sound signals (see Figure 2, main text). (A) Mean latencies in the redundant condition R CS were faster compared to single conditions S C and S S (mean and SEM of 60 blocks with 50 latencies each). (B) Cumulative distributions (circles and shaded areas indicate group quantiles with SEM; best-fitting recinormal distributions are shown as solid lines). The shift of the R CS distribution to the left corresponds to the speed-up of mean latencies (see A). Numerous quantiles in condition R CS exceeded the theoretical bound provided by the sum of distributions in conditions S C and S S (arrow).

Figure S3. Interactions with Color and Sound Signals Results are analogous to the experiment with motion and sound signals (see Figure 3, main text). (A) Mean latencies depended on the condition that was presented on the previous trial. (B) For each group quantile of conditions S C and S S (see Figure S2), we determined the relative frequency of color signals presented on the previous trial. A value close to one (zero) indicates that most responses summarized by a quantile were preceded by a color (sound) signal. (C) The analysis of color frequencies showed a negative correlation between latencies in single conditions (Pearson's linear correlation coefficient, H =-0.76, p<0.0001; numerical values of H across experiments were by chance identical). (D) Based on the single conditions, we fitted the probability summation model with the correlation coefficient as a free parameter to the latency distribution in condition R CS ( P sum ). The empirical distribution deviated from P sum in that most quantiles were faster except for the slowest quantiles that were slower than predicted (see also E). The extended model with the correlation coefficient and the additional noise as free parameters provided a reasonable Fit to the empirical distribution (best-fitting parameters are summarized in Table S2). (E) We tested for deviations from P sum using the parameters of recinormal distributions fitted separately to the empirical and predicted quantiles of each block. While the mean µ was slightly elevated (one-sample t-test, p<0.0001), the standard deviation σ was largely increased (one-sample t-test, p<0.0001).

Figure S4. Predictions with Color and Sound Signals Results are analogous to the experiment with motion and sound signals (see Figure 4, main text). We presented conditions in separate blocks of trials (S C, S S, and R MS ). We included also a new condition in which targets were defined by a conjunction of color and sound signals (C CS ). (A) Mean latencies. While responses in condition R CS were sped-up, responses in condition C CS were slowed-down. Mean and SEM of 60 blocks. (B) Cumulative distributions. The extended model with the correlation coefficient and the additional noise as free parameters provided an excellent Fit for condition R CS (the best-fitting parameters are summarized in Table S2). Using identical parameters, the model nicely predicted the distribution in condition C CS. Each distribution is based on 3,000 responses.

Table S1. Best-Fitting Recinormal Distributions Experiment Condition µ (s -1 ) σ (s -1 ) RMSE Random S M 2.222 0.364 0.011 S S 2.354 0.482 0.003 R MS 2.640 0.355 0.005 S C 2.288 0.355 0.008 S S 2.373 0.481 0.003 R CS 2.710 0.341 0.008 Block by block S C 2.555 0.290 0.014 S M 2.396 0.329 0.010 S S 2.522 0.433 0.004 R MS 2.678 0.389 0.007 R CS 2.747 0.384 0.006 C MS 2.199 0.364 0.013 C CS 2.355 0.375 0.015

Table S2. Best-Fitting Model Parameters Experiment Condition Model* (s -1 ) RMSE Random R MS C -0.70-0.067 N - 0.137 (~32%) ** 0.058 C+N *** -0.59 0.097 (~23%) ** 0.019 R CS C -0.82-0.094 N - 0.182 (~43%) ** 0.074 C+N *** -0.72 0.130 (~31%) ** 0.022 Block by block R MS C 0.10-0.030 N - 0.042 (~11%) ** 0.021 C+N *** 0.21 0.059 (~15%) ** 0.006 R CS C 0.13-0.045 N - 0.059 (~16%) ** 0.029 C+N *** 0.29 0.083 (~23%) ** 0.005 C MS Prediction from R MS from R MS 0.034 C+N -0.04 0.074 (~19%) ** 0.010 C CS Prediction from R CS from R CS 0.022 C+N 0.37 0.055 (~15%) ** 0.010 * Correlation only (C), Noise only (N), Correlated and noise (C+N). ** Compared to the average standard deviation in corresponding single conditions (see Table S1). *** The extended model provided a significantly better fit compared to both restricted models (extra sum-of-squares F test, p<0.0001 in all cases).

Supplemental Experimental Procedures Experimental Setup Participants were tested individually in an isolated experimental room. Auditory stimuli were presented to both ears simultaneously through Sennheiser HD-280 Pro headphones. Visual stimuli were presented on a Sony GDM-C520 CRT monitor (100 Hz refresh rate). Viewing distance was 60 cm supported by a chin rest. A computer running MATLAB (The MathWorks) equipped with standard toolboxes [48, 49] controlled stimulus presentation and the collection of responses via the keyboard. Auditory Stimulation Auditory background noise was continuously presented in all conditions. Noise consisted of Gaussian noise (i.e., a sequence of normally distributed random numbers at a sample rate of 44.1 khz) which was filtered so that most of the power was between 262-330 Hz. The presentation level of the background noise was 53 db(a) as measured using an artificial ear adaptor. As sound signals, we presented 294 Hz tones (the note D) that were embedded in the noise. Tones were presented for 500 ms including sinusoidal ramp on- (10 ms) and offsets (100 ms). The presentation level of the tones was 53 db(a). Visual Stimulation Visual background noise was continuously presented in all conditions. The noise was composed of 200 white dots (2x2 pixels) on a dark grey background. Dots moved linearly in random directions with a speed of 1 deg/s. Dots were restricted to the area of a notional annulus with an inner/outer diameter of 0.5/5.0 deg around central fixation. On each refresh, dots falling outside the area and some of the remaining dots (with a probability of 1%) were randomly re-located within the area of the notional annulus. As visual signals, we changed the motion or the color of some dots. With motion signals, 50% of the dots changed from random motion to coherent rotation around fixation (0.14 cps). With color signals, 30% of the dots turned yellow. In both cases, signals were presented for 500 ms and dots returned to white and random motion, respectively. Task and Procedures Participants were instructed to detect target signals by pressing a button. We asked participants to respond as fast as possible, but to avoid false alarms and missed targets. We employed a partially selfpaced, continuous stimulation paradigm. Auditory and visual background noise was presented throughout a block. After a random interval of 1500-3500 ms (uniformly distributed), a target was presented. At the time of a response, a new random interval preceding the next target was triggered. We considered responses with latencies within 100-1500 ms after signal onset as valid. Responses falling outside this range were false alarms (~2% across all conditions) and missed targets (<1%), respectively. After an error, a feedback-screen, which indicated the error, interrupted the continuous stimulation for 1500 ms before a new random interval preceding the next target was triggered. In Experiment 1, conditions were randomly interleaved within a block. This included two single conditions (only the auditory or only the visual signal was presented) and one redundant condition (both signals were presented simultaneously; see Figure 1A,B). For each condition, we collected 55 valid responses per block. A block lasted about 9 min and was interrupted twice by a pause-screen to offer participants a short rest. In Experiment 2a, we tested the same three conditions as in Experiment 1 but we presented each condition in a separate block of trials. In Experiment 2b, targets were defined by a conjunction of two signals (both the auditory and the visual signal were presented simultaneously as in the redundant conditions; Figure 1F). Critically, the random interval of 1500-5000 ms preceding a target could contain non-target signals (only the auditory or only the visual signal was presented). Participants had to withhold a response on presentation of single signals. A block contained about twice as many non-targets than targets. In each block of Experiment 2, we collected 55 valid responses. A block lasted about 3-4 min. Participants were familiarized with all conditions in a short practice session before the experiment proper (about 5-10 min). Participants then performed 4 sessions lasting about 1 h each. To reduce fatigue effects, participants performed not more than 2 sessions on a day. Within a session, we presented a block of each condition once. Each block was introduced by a start-screen indicating the next condition by an animation of possible targets and non-targets. Participants initiated blocks by a button press. At the

end of a block, the start-screen for the next block appeared. We encouraged participants to use these occasions to take a rest. The order of conditions/blocks was randomized across observers and sessions. Latency Analysis All analysis and modeling was performed using MATLAB (The MathWorks). We used a reciprocal scale (1/latency) for the analysis of response latencies (except for the computation of simple mean latencies). Mean latencies of one condition measured with one participant but in different sessions could differ by several 10s of ms, which is possibly related to learning or fatigue that might occur across sessions or to other general effects. To avoid that such differences affect the shape (and particularly the variance) of the estimated group latency distributions [30], we considered each block as an independent sample instead of pooling latencies across sessions. To reduce the impact of anticipatory responses and lapses of attention, we performed an outlier correction on the basis of the 55 valid responses of each block and condition. We rejected trials with latencies deviating by more than 3 standard deviations from the mean on the reciprocal scale (about 0.8% of the trials). Then, to obtain equally sized data blocks, we selected the latest 50 trials of each block/condition (the first few trials were considered as training and not further analyzed). In total, we collected 60 equally sized data blocks summing up to 3,000 latencies per condition (39,000 responses were collected for the whole study). To obtain cumulative group distributions, we rank ordered the latencies of each block and averaged response latencies of each rank on the reciprocal scale (Vincent averaging [30]; e.g., the latency of the fastest group quantile is computed by the average latency of the fastest response within each block, and so forth for the remaining ranks). With this method, we obtained 50 group quantiles that were based on 60 responses each. To obtain continuous distribution functions, we fitted recinormal distributions (i.e., normal distributions on the reciprocal scale) to the group quantiles of each condition by minimizing the root mean squared error (RMSE; using the fminsearch routine of MATLAB). Estimates of the mean (µ) and the standard deviation (σ) of the group distribution were virtually identical to the averaged estimates of the individual blocks. Trial History Analysis For each response in Experiment 1, we recorded the condition that was presented on the previous trial. For the 60 responses of a given quantile, we computed the relative frequency of signals that were presented on the previous trial. For example, with motion and sound signals, the motion frequency is given by the number of motion signals divided by the total number of sound and motion signals on the previous trial. Note that the total number of signals could be larger than 60 because two signals were presented in redundant conditions. Model We predicted the latency distribution with redundant signals based on probability summation. The exact distribution is given by the minimum function of the latency distributions in the single conditions, which can be estimated using the maximum function of the corresponding drift rate distributions (Figure 1C-E). For two Gaussian random variables the exact maximum function is known [34]. Let (D A, D V ) denote a bivariate Gaussian random vector with means (µ A, µ V ), variances (σ A 2, σ V 2 ) and correlation coefficient. The probability density function of the maximum distribution is given by f(x) = f 1 ( x) + f 2 ( x), with 1 μ A (Equation S1) σ A σ A σ A 1 ρ σ V 1 ρ and 1 μ V σ V σ V σ V 1 ρ (Equation S2) σ A 1 ρ where ( ) and ( ) are the probability density function and the cumulative distribution function of the standard normal distribution, respectively. Thus, with the drift rates D A ~ N(µ A, σ A 2 ) and D V ~ N(µ V, σ V 2 ) as determined in the single conditions, the probability summation model had only one degree of freedom, i.e., the correlation coefficient.

To model the noise interaction in redundant conditions, the interaction noise is added to the standard deviation of the two drift rates as determined in the corresponding single conditions. Thus, if drift rates D A ~ N(µ A, σ A 2 ) and D V ~ N(µ V, σ V 2 ) were determined in single conditions with auditory and visual signals, we used the adapted drift rates D A ~ N(µ A, (σ A + ) 2 ) and D V ~ N(µ V, (σ V + ) 2 ) for model predictions of the redundant condition. Model predictions were then computed analogously to predictions based on probability summation using the adapted drift rates (see Equations S1 and S2). Thus, the extended model had two degrees of freedom, the correlation coefficient and the additional noise. We fitted the model to empirical distributions by minimizing the RMSE. Model fitting and prediction of latency distributions with conjunction signals was performed analogously using the minimum instead of the maximum function [34] of the corresponding drift rates. Supplemental References 48. Brainard, D.H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spat. Vis. 10, 433-436. 49. Pelli, D.G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spat. Vis. 10, 437-442.