BY MICHAEL P. SCHWARZ 1 2 AND MARK W. BLOWS 2

Similar documents
THE EVOLUTION AND ONTOGENY OF NESTMATE RECOGNITION IN SOCIAL WASPS

Natal nest discrimination in the paper wasp, Polistes dominulus

Context-dependent nestmate discrimination in the paper wasp, Polistes dominulus: a critical test of the optimal acceptance threshold model

Ontogeny of division of labor in a facultatively eusocial sweat bee Megalopta genalis

Kin recognition in social insects and other animals-a review of recent findings and a consideration of their relevance for the theory of kin selection

Intracolonial nepotism during colony fissioning in honey bees?

Imprinting and kin recognition

Local resource competition. Sex allocation Is the differential allocation of investment in sons vs. daughters to increase RS. Local mate competition

Polistes paper wasps. Why cooperate? Why cooperate? 12/3/2012. Paper wasp natural history. Cooperative breeding and communication

Reproduction in primitively eusocial wasps

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission.

Chapter 14 The Evolution of Social Behavior (1 st lecture)

Thursday, September 26, 13

9.916 Ingroups and Outgroups

Sociality outside the nest: helpers in pre-hibernating clusters of Polistes dominulus

Eusocial species. Eusociality. Phylogeny showing only eusociality Eusocial insects. Eusociality: Cooperation to the extreme

Animal Behaviour. Mark Elgar. Eusociality II

Nest Hydrocarbons as Cues for Philopatry in a Paper Wasp

Cooperation. Main points for today. How can altruism evolve? Group living vs. cooperation. Sociality-nocooperation. and cooperationno-sociality

The basics of kin selection theory

12. Social insects. Is it better to be social? Is it better to be social? What is social? Some costs of being social

Title. Author(s)Yagi, Norihiro; Hasegawa, Eisuke. CitationSociobiology, 58(1): Issue Date Doc URL. Type.

Nestmate recognition and the genetic relatedness of nests in the ant Formica pratensis

Questions About Social Behavior

TESTS OF REPRODUCTIVE-SKEW MODELS

Preferential phenotypic association linked with cooperation in paper wasps

Living in groups 1. What are three costs and three benefits of living in groups?

o the U. S. species were. Three o these 4 were recently molted DECIDUOUS WINGS IN CRICKETS: A NEW BASIS FOR WING DIMORPHISM BY T. J.

Association between caste and genotype in the termite Mastotermes darwiniensis Froggatt (Isoptera: Mastotermitidae)

Chapter 44. Table of Contents. Section 1 Development of Behavior. Section 2 Types of Animal Behavior. Animal Behavior

Kin recognition by phenotype matching in female Belding's ground squirrels

Transactional Skew and Assured Fitness Return Models Fail to Predict Patterns of Cooperation in Wasps

Wasp who would be queen: A comparative study of two primitively eusocial species

POLYMORPHISM IN 8TELOPOLYBIA AREA TA (HYMENOPTERA, VESPIDAE) BY ROBERT L. JEANNE AND ROBERT FAGEN

How altruism evolves: assortment and synergy

Evolution of Social Behavior: Kin Selection & Sociobiology. Goal: Why does altruism exist in nature?

Why such altruism? Why are these nymphs sacrificing themselves to protect other aphids?

o the Kansas River, near Eudora, Douglas Co.unty, Kansas. _Although Lawrence, Kansas OCCUPANCY BY HYLAEU8 OF SUBTERRANEAN

Newey, Philip Simon (2009) Colony mate recognition in the weaver ant Oecophylla smaragdina. PhD thesis, James Cook University.

Mammalogy Lecture 15 - Social Behavior II: Evolution

Impact of helpers on colony productivity in a primitively eusocial bee

VARIABILITY OF CHEMOSENSORY STIMULI WITHIN HONEYBEE (Apis mellifera) COLONIES: Differential Conditioning Assay for Discrimination Cues

What is altruism? Benefit another at a cost to yourself. Fitness is lost!

Mating frequency and genetic relatedness of workers in the hornet Vespa analis (Hymenoptera: Vespidae)

Flexible social organization and high incidence of drifting in

Natal versus breeding dispersal: Evolution in a model system

Taming of the skew: transactional models fail to predict reproductive partitioning in the paper wasp Polistes dominulus

4 Questions relating to Behavior

Queen mating frequencies and genetic relatedness between workers in the hornet Vespa ducalis (Hymenoptera: Vespidae)

Kin structure and queen execution in the Argentine ant Linepithema humile

Assessment Schedule 2016 Biology: Demonstrate understanding of the responses of plants and animals to their external environment (91603)

1987) suggesting kin selection may play an important role in establishment. (Richardson et al. 1986). Allozyme loci offer the advantage that

Chapter 13 Opener: Weaver ants form superbly cooperative societies. Chapter 9. The Evolution of Social Behavior

Submitted to Biology Letters. Patterns of split sex ratio in ants have multiple evolutionary causes based on different within-colony conflicts

THE CENTRAL CONCEPTS OF INCLUSIVE FITNESS 3000 word article in the Oxford University Press Encyclopedia of Evolution, January 2002.

Habitat fragmentation and evolution of dispersal. Jean-François Le Galliard CNRS, University of Paris 6, France

Body Size Shapes Caste Expression, and Cleptoparasitism Reduces Body Size in the Facultatively Eusocial Bees Megalopta (Hymenoptera: Halictidae)

A Test of the Mating Limitation Hypothesis for Caste Determination in Evylaeus albipes (Hymenoptera: Halictidae), a Primitively Eusocial Halictine Bee

Allozyme variation and sociogenetic structure of Polistes satan Bequaert 1940 colonies (Hymenoptera, Vespidae)

The role of male disease susceptibility in the evolution of haplodiploid insect societies

Origin and evolution of ei~sociality: A perspective from studying primitively eusociai wasps

Hiromi Nishida. 1. Introduction. 2. Materials and Methods

Monogamy within the Termite World: Mate Choice and Colonial Structure

BY A. BUSCHINGER, R.D. SCHUMANN

Social Insects. Social Insects. Subsocial. Social Insects 4/9/15. Insect Ecology

Social Insects. Insect Ecology

THE LIFE HISTORY OF POLISTES METRICUS SAY: A STUDY OF BEHAVIOR AND PARASITIC NATURAL ENEMIES AMANDA COLEEN HODGES

When do honey bee guards reject their former nestmates after swarming?

ALTRUISM OR JUST SHOWING OFF?

The early history of Hamiltonian-based research on kin recognition

The Biology of Social Insects

SC741 W12: Division of Labor Part I: Fixed- and Variable- Threshold Algorithms

Upskilling community leaders for Australian Pollinator Week

Insurance-based advantages for subordinate co-foundresses in a temperate paper wasp

Levels of Ecological Organization. Biotic and Abiotic Factors. Studying Ecology. Chapter 4 Population Ecology

Chapter 4 Population Ecology

Cooperation Behaviors selected that benefit a recipient. Either altruistic ( -,+) or mutualistic (+,+). Can occur across species.

BY DONALD P. JOUVENAZ, DANIEL P. WOJCIK, AND ROBERT K. VANDER MEER

A halictid bee with sympatric solitary and eusocial nests offers evidence for Hamilton s rule

A MEGASECOPTERON FROM UPPER CARBONIFEROUS BY F. M. CARPENTER. In I962 Professor F. Stockmans, of the Institut Royal des Sciences STRATA IN SPAIN

POPULATIONS and COMMUNITIES

Non-independence in Statistical Tests for Discrete Cross-species Data

The Evolution of Sex Chromosomes through the. Baldwin Effect

Due Date: Student Name: Candidate Number: Blog resource: Click4Biology:

Social interaction. Kin and Group selection. Social interaction. Social interaction. Social interaction. Social interaction

Queen Number and. Museum of Comparative Zoology, Harvard University, Cambridge, USA and Museum of Zoology, Palais de Rumine, Lausanne, Switzerland

Lipid stores, ovary development, and brain gene expression in Polistes metricus females

SOCIAL ANIMALS. -Spectacular -Photographed -Studied -Appreciated. The PINNACLE of evolution???

The Evolution of Eusociality in Bees

Policing and group cohesion when resources vary

Author's personal copy

fossilis in his Neuropteren aus dem lihographischen Schiefer in Bayern (1862). Except for the mere statement A JURASSIC NEUROPTERAN FROM THE LITHO-

The organization of individuals into cooperative social groups

Sexual Reproduction. Page by: OpenStax

Climate Change Vulnerability Assessment for Species

Structure of lecture notes

Alana Schick , ISCI 330 Apr. 12, The Evolution of Cooperation: Putting gtheory to the Test

Structure of lecture notes

Worker reproductive competition affects division of labor in a primitively social paperwasp (Polistes instabilis)

The evolution of begging: Signaling and sibling competition

Transcription:

KIN ASSOCIATION DURING NEST FOUNDING IN THE BEE EXONEURA BICOLOR: ACTIVE DISCRIMINATION, PHILOPATRY AND FAMILIAR LANDMARKS.* BY MICHAEL P. SCHWARZ 1 2 AND MARK W. BLOWS 2 INTRODUCTION Hamilton s Kin Selection Hypothesis requires, as a minimal condition, that altruistic individuals tend to direct aid towards other individuals sharing a greater genetic similarity than would be expected by random association. Interaction between kin can arise in two ways: (i) It can arise contextually without requiring any discrimination between individuals. For example, low dispersal rates of siblings and population subdivision can lead to interaction between kin at greater rates than would otherwise be expected. (ii) It can arise through recognition mechanisms such that individuals are able to assess their kinship or prior familiarity with other individuals and adopt behavioural strategies accordingly. Over the last decade there have been an increasing number of studies investigating the mechanisms for kin recognition in social organisms (Gamboa et al. 1986; Fletcher and Michener 1987; Carlin 1989). Whilst kin recognition in the highly eusocial ants and bees has been the subject of intensive investigations (see for eg. Veeresch et al. 1990), most studies on primitively social insects have investigated recognition in either of three contexts: (i) Recognition of nests by their occupants (eg. Foster and Gamboa 1989; Pfennig 1990; Wcislo 1990); (ii) discrimination between familiar and unfamiliar individuals by guards at the nest entrance (see Michener and Smith 1987 for citations); and (iii) behavioural interactions in novel environments, such as circle tubes (eg. Kukuk 1990), binary choice apparatus (Kukuk et al. 1977) and flight boxes (eg. Bornais et al. 1983). However, few studies of primitively social insects have explicitly investigated kin recognition away from natal nests and where interactions are not forced upon 1Department of Zoology, La Trobe University, Bundoora, Vic. 3083, Australia 2Department of Genetics and Human Variation, La Trobe University, Bundoora, Vic. 3083, Australia *Manuscript received 15 July 1991. 241

242 Psyche [Vol. 98 subject animals by the experimenter. Such contexts could be particularly useful for kin recognition studies, since there are likely to be few or no pre-existing recognition labels associated with the interaction arena, and there should be a lower likelihood of artefacts due to forced interactions. Exoneura bicolor is a univoltine bee exhibiting a solitary/ quasisocial/semisocial colony polymorphism (Schwarz 1986, 1987). The majority of newly built nests are cofounded by groups of up to 8 females, and electrophoretic analysis of these colonies (Schwarz 1987) has shown that relatedness between cofoundresses is moderately high (0.597 _+ 0.062; b _+ SE). In montane habitats, this bee nests in the dead fronds of tree-ferns, and this means that nests tend to occur in well-defined aggregations. The finding of moderately high values of relatedness within newly cofounded nests indicates effective kin association between dispersing females in this species. However, it is possible that such association arises without active assessment of kinship or prior familiarity; for example, it could arise if foundresses are strongly philopatric or if foundress dispersal is asynchronous among neighbouring colonies, so that association between previous nestmates arises incidentally. A potential role for philopatry as a factor contributing to relatedness between cofoundresses is suggested by a study by Blows and Schwarz (1991), who found significant coancestries for nesting aggregations in 3 out of 8 localities studied. Statistically significant coancestries varied from (R) 0.03 to 0.11. Since cofounding of new nests is limited to an approximately 2 week period in spring (Schwarz 1986), asynchronous dispersal between colonies is unlikely to contribute to relatedness among cofoundresses. In many Polistes species, new nests are often founded by two or more related gynes after overwintering. In these wasps, kin association may result in part from philopatry (Klahn 1979; Noonan 1981) as well as nestmate recognition (Post and Jeanne 1982; Bornais et al. 1983; but see Pratte 1982). In this study we explicitly examine both these possible factors during colony foundation in a primitively social allodapine bee, Exoneura bicolor. We present data confirming that cofounding by kin in a primitively social allodapine bee, Exoneura bicolor, is the result of active discrimination by females and that this discrimination occurs even in novel

1991] Schwarz & Blows 243 environments where contextual cues, such as natal nests and familiar landmarks, are absent. MATERIALS AND METHODS Intact colonies of E. bicolor were collected from Sherbrooke National Park in late April 1988. Whole nests were plugged with cotton wool and taken to La Trobe University where they were stored at 10C until released. Adult females were removed from nests by splitting the nests open longitudinally; colony members were transferred to petri dishes and given unique abdominal and thoracic marks using Testors Model Master TM enamel paint. A total of 151 females, taken from 26 multifemale nests were used in the experiment. The number of original inhabitants per nest ranged from 2 to 21 (mean 5.6; median 4). Marked females were released at a series of stakes bearing trap nests in the La Trobe University Zoology Wildlife Reserve. This reserve does not contain naturally occurring colonies of allodapine bees. Trap nests comprised 40 cm lengths of dead tree fern fronds (Cyathea australis) tied onto upright metal stakes (black painted star pickets), with 30 fronds per picket. Fronds were fixed in subhorizontal positions on the stakes. A total of 6 stakes were used, placed in a straight line with stakes at 2 m intervals. On 2 May 1988 bees were released en mass by placing all bees into a single container and gently tapping them onto the central stake. While we cannot rule out the possibility that females remained in nestmate clumps in this container, this seems unlikely given the number of females involved, the size of the container (small ice-cream container), and the considerable shaking that occurred when carrying the bees from the lab to the field release site. Bees were released at 1030 hr; ambient temperature was 21C and there was no wind. Montane populations of E. bicolor do not show foundress dispersal during autumn. Apart from colonies whose nests have been damaged, nearly all nest initiation is restricted to a short period of about 2 weeks in late spring (Schwarz 1986). Therefore, this experiment was carried out at a time when some colonies may need to find new nests, but foundress dispersal is otherwise rare. After release, bees were left for 2 weeks and collected on 16 May 1988 during a rainy period so that all colony members would be present. Newly constructed nests were plugged with cotton

244 Psyche [Vol. 98 wool and opened in the laboratory. All recovered females were scored for paint marks and then assayed electrophoretically at 2 presumptive loci, both staining as esterases. Electrophoresis and staining techniques are described in Blows and Schwarz (1991). Relatedness between nestmates was calculated using Goodnight and Queller s (1989) technique based on Grafen s (1985) relatedness coefficient, and can be interpreted as a measure of identity by descent. Relatedness coefficients and their estimates were calculated by jackknifing across colonies. All nest lengths were recorded. RESULTS In the two weeks prior to collection of the newly founded nests, females were observed flying around fern fronds, constructing new nests, or visiting newly established nests. Occupants of new nests were frequently observed sitting in the entrances of their nests with their antennae pointing outwards. Visitors to new nests frequently entered several nests in rapid succession, although most visits lasted for only a few seconds. Visits to occupied nests did not seem to be accompanied by aggression on behalf of either the visitors or the occupants. Of the 151 bees released, 77 were recovered from a total of 54 nests, comprising 20 multifemale nests and 34 single-female nests. A further three nests did not contain any bees. The number of occupied nests per stake and bees per nest are summarized in Table 1. The greatest number of nests were founded at the central stake on which the bees had originally been released, with progressively decreasing numbers in adjacent stakes. No nests were constructed at either of the terminal stakes. 17 nests contained two bees each, and 3 nests had three bees each. Markings on 37 of the recovered bees were lost, but where all bees in a new nest still had markings (five 2-female nests), all bees nesting together had originally been nestmates (although each pair had originated from different source nests). However, a number of bees nesting alone had original nestmates that were also nesting alone or with unmarked bees at the same stake. Of the 6 nests containing more than one female with intact marks, all marked females were from the same original nests, so that we have no evidence of colony mixing after dispersal.

1991] Schwarz & Blows 245 All females from the 20 new nests containing more than one female were electrophoresed and relatedness was estimated as 0.32 + 0.09 (b + SE). This estimate is lower than those reported by Schwarz (1987) but falls within the range of estimates in a more comprehensive study of intra-colony relatedness in natural populations (Blows and Schwarz 1991). Positions of solitary and multifemale nests relative to the release-point stake were compared using Chi-square tests. Whilst the distribution of single and multifemale nests did not differ significantly (Z 2 3.463, p > 0.05), the distribution of females in these two nest types differed slightly but significantly (Z 2 6.946, p < 0.05), with proportionately more females at the distal stakes nesting solitarily. Therefore there is some evidence that females dispersing shorter distances have a greater likelihood of cofounding, or that cofounding females disperse over shorter distances. Mean nest lengths for single and multifemale nests are given in Table 1. These data show that mean nest length increases synergistically with the number of cofoundresses, so that mean nest lengths per resident female are approximately 3 cm, 4 cm and 4.7 cm for single, two and three-female nests respectively. DISCUSSION Our results indicate that when original nests of Exoneura bicolor were destroyed, and when alternative nesting sites are available at a range of distances from the release site, those females recovered tended to disperse short distances. Furthermore, the majority of bees recovered in our experiment had joined relatives to form multi-female colonies. Table 1. Mean nest length and the distribution of single and multi-female nests founded among 6 experimental stakes containing trap nests. Stakes were placed in a straight line at 2 m intervals. Bees were originally released at stake 4. Stake 2 3 4 5 6 Nest Length Mean S.E. 1-female nests 0 5 6 14 9 0 2.97 0.33 2-female nests 0 0 4 10 3 0 8.03 0.79 3-female nests 0 0 0 14.17 2.05 Total nests 0 5 11 25 13 0

246 Psyche [Vol. 98 Three factors, which are not mutually exclusive, could explain the limited dispersal distances observed in our experiment: (i) females may prefer to nest in central rather than peripheral sites, perhaps to reduce exposure to predation; (ii) females may be minimizing searching effort by utilizing the first suitable nesting sites encountered; (iii) females may be reducing dispersal distances in order to increase the likelihood of locating previous nestmates. Given the large number of nesting sites per stake in our experiment (6 stakes with 60 frond-ends per stake) it is unlikely that either low dispersal rates or cofounding in our experiment are due to limited nesting sites. In natural populations nest densities may become very high, with up to four nests per frond; nesting densities in our experiment were comparatively low. Therefore, kin association occurred in a novel environment and, although dispersal distances were low, they are not sufficient to explain kin association during nest initiation. However, cooperatively nesting females tended to be closer to the release point, suggesting that females may have a greater likelihood of nesting with kin if they disperse shorter distances. A similar situation appears to occur in some Polistes wasps, where philopatry may bring related gynes together after overwintering, but is not sufficient to explain the extent of observed kin association (Bornais et al. 1983). The lower dispersal distances of cooperatively nesting females could also be explained if there is competition for central nesting sites (to minimize predation) and if cooperatively nesting females are better able to compete for these sites. We feel that this possibility is unlikely for several reasons. Firstly, nesting densities in our experiment were low compared to many natural aggregations, so that possible competition in our experiment is expected to be relatively minor. Secondly, we did not observe any agonistic interactions, either between resident females and visitors, or between residents within stakes. Thirdly, if central areas are preferred because of reduced predator pressure, excluding other females from these areas would tend to increase the exposure of central nesting females to any predators that do enter these areas. Therefore, our findings suggest that females of Exoneura bicolor assort themselves into kin groups (or groups of previous nestmates) on the basis of cues that can be carried into a novel

1991] Schwarz & Blows 247 environment, even when nesting sites are not limiting and a moderately large number of kin groups are present. Whilst we have not strictly shown that such cues are utilized during natural periods of founding, it seems unlikely that the ability to use these cues would not be utilized at times when kin association among large groups of dispersers is frequent. In colonies of E. bicolor, reproductivity per female increases with the number of nestmates (Schwarz 1988a) indicating benefits for cooperative nesting, but communal progressive rearing in allodapine bees creates a high potential for nestmate parasitism. These two factors may create strong selective advantages for cooperative nest founding moderated by kin or nestmate recognition (Schwarz 1988b). Our finding of synergistic increases in nest length with the number of nestmates could indicate some immediate advantages for cooperative nesting, even in the absence of brood rearing. Nests in E. bicolor are often quite long (>0.5m) (Schwarz 1986) and it is possible that longer nests may enhance defence against parasites or predators such as ants. However, there may be an alternative explanation for the short nest lengths of single females. Schwarz and O Keefe (1991) have shown that when kin are not available, most solitary nesting females in autumn eventually join non-relatives to form multi-female colonies. It is possible that solitary females delay investment in nest construction until they are either joined by other females or defect to other colonies. In conclusion, our results suggest that the high relatedness between females in newly cofounded nests in natural populations are the result of active discrimination by females. Such discrimination could be based on relatedness or prior familiarity but cannot be explained by philopatry or asynchronous dispersal. The experimental methods described here allow kin recognition to be investigated without inducing forced interactions. Our techniques could also be elaborated to investigate recognition in more detail, whilst still avoiding unrealistic scenarios. For example, our protocol could be expanded to test for within-colony discrimination between matrilines by using larger source colonies (which would contain several matrilines) and comparing intra-colony relatedness before and after cofounding experiments.

248 Psyche [Vol. 98 SUMMARY Previous work has demonstrated moderately high intra-colony relatedness among cofoundresses in newly built nests of the allodapine bee Exoneura bicolor. In this paper we test whether this kin association can be explained by spatially limited or asynchronous dispersal, or whether it requires active kin or nestmate recognition. Bees from 21 colonies were released in a novel environment containing abundant potential nesting sites at various distances from the release point. Most bees recovered had formed multi-female nests. Relatedness among cofoundresses was significant, and we found no evidence of colony mixing after release. Dispersal distances tended to be small (<4 m), and cooperatively nesting females tended to be closer to the release site than solitary females. Kin association during cofounding in this bee requires active recognition of either kinship or previous familiarity, but this could be facilitated by limited dispersal distances. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS We would like to thank K. J. O Keefe for help with fieldwork, G. Paras and the La Trobe University Wildlife Reserve for the use of their facilities, and D. C. Queller and K. F. Goodnight for providing us with their programme for estimating relatedness using electrophoretic data. We also thank an anonymous reviewer for helpful comments on an earlier draft of this manuscript. Bees were collected under permit no. 878/54 (Dept. of Conservation, Forests and Land). This work was supported by the National Research Fellowship Scheme and the Department of Zoology, La Trobe University. LITERATURE CITED BLOWS, M. W. AND SCHWARZ, M. P. 1991. Spatial distribution of a primitively social bee: Does population genetic structure facilitate altruism? Evolution 45: 680-693. BORNAIS, K. M., LARCH, C. M., GAMBOA, G. J. AND DAILY, R. B. 1983. Nestmate discrimination among laboratory overwintered foundresses of the paper wasp Polistes fuscatus. Canadian Entom. 115: 655-658. FLETCHER, D. J. C. AND MICHENER, C. D. 1987. Kin recognition in animals. John Wiley & Sons, Chichester.

19911 Schwarz & Blows 249 FOSTER, R. L. AND GAMBOA, G. J. 1989. Nest entrance marking with colony specific odours by the bumble bee Bombus occidentalis. Ethology 81: 273-278. CARLIN, N. F. 1989. Discrimination between and with colonies of social insects: Two null hypotheses. Nether. Jour. Zool. 39: 86-100. GAMBOA, G. J., REEVE, H. K. AND PFENNIG, D. W. The evolution and ontogeny of nestmate recognition in social wasps. Ann. Rev. Entomol. 31: 431-454. GRAFEN, A. 1985. A geometric view of relatedness, pp. 28-89 in DAWKINS, R. and RID- ELY, M. (eds.) Oxford surveys in evolutionary biology Vol. 2. Oxford University Press, N.Y. KLAHN, J. E. 1979. Philopatric and non-philopatric foundress associations in the social wasp Polistes fuscatus. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 5: 417-424. KUKUK, P. F. 1990. Trophallaxis in a communal halictine bee Lasioglossum (Chilalictus) erythrurum. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 87: 5402-5404. KUKUK, P. F., BREED, M. D., SOBTI, A. AND BELL, W. J. 1977. The contributions of kinship and conditioning to nest recognition and colony member recognition in a primitively social bee, Lasioglossum zephyrum. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2:319-327. MICHENER, C. D. AND SMITH, B. H. 1987. Kin recognition in primitively social insects, in FLETCHER, D. J. C. and MICHENER, C. D. (eds.) Kin recognition in animals. John Wiley & Sons, Chichester. NOONAN, K. M. 1981. Individual strategies of inclusive-fitness-maximizing in Polistes fuscatus foundresses, pp. 18-44, in ALEXANDER, R. D. and TINKLE, D. W. (eds.) Natural Selection and social behaviour: research and theory. Chiron Press, New York. PFENNIG, D. W. 1990. Nestmate discrimination among workers from neighbouring colonies of social wasps Polistes exclamans. Can. J. Zool. 68:268-271. POST, D. C. AND JEANNE, R. L. 1982. Recognition of former nestmates during colony founding in the social wasp, Polistesfuscatus. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 11: 283-285. PRATTE, M. 1982. Relations anterieures et association de fondation chez Polistes gallicus. Insectes Sociaux 29: 352-357. QUELLER, D. C. AND GOODNIGHT, K. F. 1989. Estimating relatedness using genetic markers. Evolution 43: 258-275. SCHWARZ, M. P. 1986. Persistent multifemale nests in an Australian allodapine bee, Exoneura bicolor. Insectes Sociaux 33: 258-277. 1987. Intra-colony relatedness and sociality in the allodapine bee Exoneura bicolor. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 21: 387-392. 1988a. Local Resource Enhancement and sex ratios in a primitively social bee. Nature 331: 346-348.

250 Psyche [Vol. 98 1988b.Intra-specific mutualism and kin-association of cofoundresses in allodapine bees. Monit. ital. Zool. 22: 245-254. VEERESH, G. K., MALLIK, B. AND VIRAKTAMATH, C. A. 1990. Social insects and the environment. Oxford and IBH, New Delhi. WCISLO, W. T. (in review) Nest recognition in a solitary sweat bee, Lasioglossum figuersi, as an enabling mechanism for sociality.

International Journal of Peptides BioMed Research International Stem Cells International Advances in Virolog y International Journal of Genomics Journal of Nucleic Acids Zoology International Journal of http://www.hindawi.com Volume 2014 http://www.hindawi.com Volume 2014 Submit your manuscripts at http://www.hindawi.com Journal of Signal Transduction The Scientific World Journal Genetics Research International Anatomy Research International International Journal of Microbiology Biochemistry Research International Advances in Bioinformatics Archaea Enzyme Research International Journal of Evolutionary Biology Molecular Biology International Journal of Marine Biology