--- . \ \ Visibility of Targets. ~~Lr ~ I\ ~ 4 negative Contrast ... WERNER ADRIAN. positive Contrast. Eye. -2 logo.

Similar documents
DRAFT MANUSCRIPT. Title: Legibility of Text on Traffic Signs as a Function of Luminance and Size

Lab NotesIssue. Disability Glare in the Outdoor Workplace

L ight color influence on obstacle recognition in road lighting. 1. Introduction

DEA 3500: HUMAN FACTORS: THE AMBIENT ENVIRONMENT (Fall 2017)

Glare and visibility by headlight for pedestrian and elderly driver. 22 October 2018 National Traffic Safety and Environment Laboratory

DEA 350: HUMAN FACTORS: THE AMBIENT ENVIRONMENT (Spring 2008)

ASSESSMENT OF NON-COHERENT LIGHT SOURCES

Analysis of visibility level in road lighting using image processing techniques

BASIC VISUAL SCIENCE CORE

Disability glare: A study in simulated road lighting conditions

Predicting the Effects of Discomfort Glare on Visual Performance of Pilots during Nighttime Flying

Danjon noticed that the length (cusp to cusp) of the new crescent. moon was less than 180 degrees and suggested that the cause of the

Surround effects on the shape of the temporal contrast-sensitivity function

Kamelia Nikolova, Iva Petrinska, Dimitar Pavlov, Petya Djanovska Technical University of Sofia

Solar reflected glare affecting visual performance

DEA 3500: HUMAN FACTORS: THE AMBIENT ENVIRONMENT (Spring 2013)

Brightness induction: Unequal spatial integration with increments and decrements

A conventional approach to nighttime visibility in adverse weather conditions

ISSN LIGHT & ENGINEERING. Volume 20, Number 1, 2012 ENGINEERING LIGHT & Znack Publishing House, Moscow

Applied Research on Tunnel Lighting: Current Development in Japan

A Model of Local Adaptation supplementary information

Vision & Perception. Simple model: simple reflectance/illumination model. image: x(n 1,n 2 )=i(n 1,n 2 )r(n 1,n 2 ) 0 < r(n 1,n 2 ) < 1

Report No.: EASZF Page 2 of 14

Mesopic Photometry for SSL. Teresa Goodman Metrology for SSL Meeting 24 th April 2013

Lesson 2. Visual Performance 2-1

CLINICAL VISUAL OPTICS (OPTO 223) Weeks XII & XIII Dr Salwa Alsaleh

White Paper Luminance & Illuminance. Brief explanation of photometry for the application of tunnel lighting control

An Experimental Approach to a Definition of the Mesopic Adaptation Field

Velocity Thresholds in Car-Following at Night

(65th GRE, March 2011, agenda item 4(d))

POST-PROCESSING OF RADIANCE IMAGES: VIRTUAL LIGHTING LABORATORY

Accommodation to stimuli in peripheral vision

Photobiological Safety of Luminaires: Refining the New Approach

Contrast Sensitivity

When is sign lighting needed? How much is needed?

University of California, Berkeley, California, U.S.A. (Received 14 June 1965)

Experiment 3 The Simple Magnifier, Microscope, and Telescope

Fundamentals of light

Fundamentals of light

A Study of Mesopic Adaptation conditions for Night Drivers using an approximation for Oncoming headlight glare

Lighting fundamentals

A Microcomputer Program for Use with the American National Standard Practice for Roadway Lighting

Islamic University of Gaza - Palestine. Department of Industrial Engineering

Compliance with the National requirements of EUROPEAN GROUP DIFFERENCES AND NATIONAL DIFFERENCES for EN 62471:2008.

A Nighttime Visibility Meter for Secondary Roads

Effects of Asymmetric Distributions on Roadway Luminance

ACUITY AND THE CONE CELL DISTRIBUTION

TEST REPORT IEC Photobiological safety of lamps and lamp systems

Theory of colour measurement Contemporary wool dyeing and finishing

TEST REPORT IEC Photobiological safety of lamps and lamp systems

TECHNICAL NOTE. Specifying Product Performance for Mesopic Applications

Adaptation to a 'spatial-frequency doubled' stimulus

Experiment 3 The Simple Magnifier, Microscope, and Telescope

COLOR SCIENCE. Concepts and Methods, Quantitative Data and Formulae, 2nd Edition. John Wiley & Sons New York Chichester Brisbane Toronto Singapore

A COMPARISON BETWEEN DGI OBTAINED THROUGH DIFFERENT METHODS AND PERCEIVED EVALUATION INDEX DDG

UNIVERSITY OF HONG KONG Faculty of Engineering M.Sc.(Eng) in Building Services Engineering MEBS6002 LIGHTING ENGINEERING

Photometric measurements for visibility level computations

Motion Perception 1. PSY305 Lecture 12 JV Stone

EXPERIMENTAL STUDY ON WALKING SPEED IN ESCAPE ROUTE CONSIDERING LUMINOUS CONDITION, SMOKE DENSITY AND EVACUEE S VISUAL ACUITY

Photobiological safety of lamps and lamp systems

A Model of Local Adaptation

Radiometry. Energy & Power

TEST REPORT EN 62471:2008 Photobiological safety of lamps and lamp systems

LUMINOUS MEASUREMENTS

IALA Recommendation E On Marine Signal Lights Part 2 - Calculation, Definition and Notation of Luminous Range. Edition 1 December 2008

Review of optimal road surface luminance considering dazzling by headlight of the car running in the opposite direction

Eschenbach Low Vision Training Program

Photobiological safety of lamps and lamp systems

TEST REPORT IEC Photobiological safety of lamps and lamp systems

Detectability measures the difference between the means of the noisy ``signal'' distribution and the ``noise-only'' distribution.

VISIBILITY UNDER ADVERSE WEATHER CONDITION IN AUTOMOTIVE LIGHTING

the contrast threshold before and after adaptation. There is a

The Control of Glare by the AS1680 Systems

ROAD TUNNEL LIGHTING Lighting power controllers

What is a receptive field? Why a sensory neuron has such particular RF How a RF was developed?

CENTRE OF TESTING SERVICE INTERNATIONAL OPERATE ACCORDING TO ISO/IEC LVD TEST REPORT

TEST REPORT IEC and/or EN Photobiological safety of lamps and lamp systems

Shengfu LU Professor Brain Informatics Laboratory International WIC Institute Beijing University of Technology Beijing, China

APPARENT CONTRAST OF SPATIALLY AND TEMPORALLY SAMPLED GRATINGS

Spectral and photopic studies for high intensity discharge (HID) lamps

Glint and Glare Analysis

Published in: Proceeding of 9th SENVAR / 2nd ISESEE, Shah Alam, Malaysia, December 2008

Data Taken by. Exploring Telescopes

INTERNATIONAL ISO STANDARD

RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN STATIC AND DYNAMIC STEREO ACUITY 1

Spatial Vision: Primary Visual Cortex (Chapter 3, part 1)

TEST REPORT IEC / EN Photobiological safety of lamps and lamp systems

Using a Quantitative Measure to Achieve a Qualitative Lighting Design Effect! Introduction What is Luminance?

0*521. (see Fig. 5, p. 43 in Denton & Pirenne, 1954) was mounted in front of the source. The

Test Report. Project No: Report No: aR01 Report Issued Date: Customer Company & Address:

TEST REPORT IEC Photobiological safety of lamps and lamp systems

DESIGN CRITERIA FOR TUNNEL LIGHTING

Photobiological safety of lamps and lamp systems

Transformation of AAVSO Archive Visual Data to the Johnson V System

Safe Transportation Research & Education Center UC Berkeley

Photobiological safety of lamps and lamp systems

This is a repository copy of Measure for Measure. Semi-cylindrical illuminance: a semi-conceived measure?.

Design and Operation of a Glare Evaluation Meter

The Effect of a Small Nearby Village (Großmugl) on Night Sky Brightness

Project Proposal. Student safety at Humboldt State is something that is highly considered by faculty,

Transcription:

TRANSPORTATON RESEARCH RECORD 247 39 Visibility of Targets WERNER ADRAN Traffic safety is highly correlated to the amount of visual information that can be obtained from the road and its immediate environment. t is therefore a logical consequence to base any quality judgment for lighting systems on visibility criteria. This development can be observed in indoor lighting as well, where the visibility-related CRF (contrast rendering factor) was introduced as a quality criterion. Visibility as a characteristic of roadway lighting has recently been discussed in Canada. When applying visibility as a criterion, there needs to be a metric to measure it and a method for calculation to predict the visibility level to be achieved in a certain lighting installation. Outlined below are the basics arising from the physiology of the visual system. Assuming that achromatic light is in general white or near white, there needs to be a certain luminance difference between the target and background to perceive it. Figure shows a target subtending the angular size o. seen against a background luminance Lb. The target can have a higher luminance than the background (positive contrast) or appear darker than Lb (negative contrast). For both cases, a minimal luminance difference is needed to perceive the target with a certain probability level: /::,.L = LT - Lb where LT equals target luminance. n this paper, p = 99.93 percent. Figure 2 contains the results of the necessary t::..l for positive contrast as a function of the target size on a background of Lb = 3 cd/m 2 For small targets, this curve shows the following function: log t::..l = -2 logo. This reflects Ricca's law, for which summation is observed over a receptive field. The size of this field is indicated by the critical Ricca's angle, often taken from the intersection of that line with the abscissa. A more precise value can be obtained from the point of a defined deviation from the law expressed by that line. For larger o., the threshold t::..l attains a contrast value independent from the target size: log t::..l = const la This expresses Weber's law, indicating that for larger objects the threshold is dependent only on Lb and approaches School of Optometry, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario N2L 3G, Canada. Lr 4 negative Contrast Eye positive Contrast FGURE Target with angular size o. against background luminance Lb. LT is the luminance of the target. The calculation of t::..l is based on a composite of these two laws. This study introduces two auxiliary functions: the luminous flux function, (cf>), characteristic for the Ricco process, and the luminance function (L), reflecting Weber's law: Ricco: t::..l = Weber: K. o.-2 t::..l = const _, r V UJ : r... ' l O. \ \ Joq, " \ 9 "'. -z a L - 2 9 a.. t k dl oc.. 2 const. RCCO'S LAW \ FCfl Lu 3 cdlm 2,"'\, ' -' Occ WEBEA"S LAW log ll L contl --- \... \ C)Z SZE OF OBJECT -./min ---+ Ql FGURE 2 ll threshold as a function of o. at a constant background luminance Lb = 3 cd/m 2 The intersection of the Ricco and Weber functions is often taken as an indicator of the critical angle <Xe over which summation occurs.

4 Ricco: TRANSPORTATON RESEARCH RECORD 247 target size and luminance level Lb. The following equations to calculate a (a, Lb) are based on experimental data from Schmidt-Claussen ( 4) and Blackwell (5): a(a) =.36 -.972 (log a + 523 ) 2 (log a +.523) 2-2.53 (log a +.523) + 2.7895 a(l ) =.355 -.27 (log Lb + 6 ) 2 b (lo L,. + 6) 2 -.4 (log Lb + 6) + 52.28 Weber: For small targets (a< 6 min of arc), the value of a(a, Lb) can be besi approximated by f).l is derived from the combination of the two functions in the following form: ( w f).l = k. ---;- + VL )2 From Adrian's (), Aulhorn's (2), and Blackwell's (3) data, the <!> and L functions have been derived and can be calculated as follows: Adrian: Lb :?:.6 cd/m 2 The increase in threshold value f).l for a target of a = min with a shorter observation time is illustrated below (Lb cd/m 2 ): f).l = f).l Observation Time (t sec) 2... a( a, Lb) + t t=2sec ( a(a, Lb)+ t ti L,_z.., 2. 2.66 \4) = log ( 4.925 Lg- 556 ) +.684 Lg- 5867 VL =.5946 L.4 66 Aulhorn: Lb :S.48 cd/m 2 log \4) =.28 +.73 log Lb log VL = -.89 +.5275 log Lb +.227 (log Lb) 2 Blackwell:.48 cd/m 2 < Lb <.6 cd/m 2 log \4) = -.72 +.3372 log Lb +.866(og Lb) 2 log \(L = -.256 +.39 log Lb NFLUENCE OF EXPOSURE TME The data are obtained with 2 sec or unlimited observation time. For a shorter exposure time of the target, higher f).l values are needed. This influence is measured by the following equation: where a is the Blondel-Rey constant and is a function of the DFFERENCE BETWEEN ll THRESHOLDS FOR POSTVE AND NEGATVE TARGET CONTRAST So far, only targets of positive contrast have been considered. Aulhorn (2) reported that, at the same f).l, a target in negative contrast could always be seen better than a target in positive contrast. She wrote: "We face this phenomenon whatever visual function we consider" (she had investigated the luminance difference sensitivity and its relationship to the visual acuity). She produced considerable data for both positive and negative contrast. From the data, it can be concluded that the threshold differences between negative and positive targets are dependent on Lb and also on target size a. For an explanation of these phenomena, it is helpful to study the results of Remole (6). He investigated the border contrast and found inhibitory effects on either side of borders. Figure 3 is taken from Remole's publication. The lengths of the inhibitory zones are different, and the ratio between them varies with the luminance level, which accounts for the dependency on a and Lb. The arrows indicate the widths measured. Remole (6) has shown that the ratio alb and the absolute value of b depend on the luminance level. To obtain the difference between 6.L for positive and negative contrast, a factor (F cp) was derived from Aulhorn's data. /).Lneg follows from the term where 6.LP, is the value for exposure time t = 2 sec. FCP is computed according to the following equation: m. o. - ll 2.4 8,,,,,. 2

Adrian 4 where ffl = - -( 25(ogLb+l)+ 2'5) for Lb. cd/m 2 m - -( 75(og Lb + ) + 2'5) for Lb >.4 cd/m 2 j3.6lt; 488 V Lb cd/m 2 Threshold til for a target with negative contrast (darker than the background) is obtained by b... a L B FGURE 3 Stimulus field with illuminated portion of hairline visible in the dark field. Figure 4 shows the function for the contrast polarity factor (FCP) versus target size for luminance levels as used by Aulhorn in her investigations (she chose the unit asb as a basis, which leads to the odd numbers when expressed in cd/m 2 ). The curves indicate that F CP is always <, which yields smaller til thresholds for negative contrast. Figures 5, 6, and 7 allow a comparison between Aulhorn's data and the calculated function according to the method described in this paper, which is based on Adrian's and Blackwell's data. /ilpos and /ilneg thresholds are plotted versus the target size for different levels of Lb. _t <l LL. :.:==+----- -w_,_.9----,.8 - - -, - r--....7 --- --.. -- ------t---"< 'll LL. c:.6 --- --+-----+-- 'll N ;::::.!!!.5 Parameter: L = Background luminance cd/m L t r---.4-----l--.3 ---- ---.. - ---+------!-----+----!------+----;----+-----.2+----_,_----+----+-----+----+-----+----+--...--+--...---+--...-..6 2.5 4 6.3 6 25 4 63 Target size a [min] FGURE 4 The contrast polarity factor, F cp, dependent on target size a and the background luminance. The curves show the relationship between positive and negative target contrast. n negative contrast the threshold of a target of a defined size is always lower than in positive contrast at the same background luminance, so darker targets appear to be better perceived than brighter targets at the same luminance difference.

42 TRA NSPORTA TON RESEARCH R ECORD 247.-----t_,.,,-. Negative Contrast Target..---- - -- ------+------+-----------.f---;--- <l :!:!.r:. ll!!.r:. - ---calculated by Adrian x 2.4 " x Data from Aulhorn - --- - - -- - --------. i---- Parameter Target Size a... Background luminance L [cd/m 2 ] FGURE 5 Comparison between Aulhorn's data for negative contrast targets and Adrian's M.,. The data have been multiplied by F cp to convert them to negative contrast. A factor of 2.4 had to be applied to account for Aulhorn's monocular observation conditions and the age difference of her subjects. There are no data for = ' measured by Aulhorn in negative contrast. To obtain the best fit, the calculated values had to be multiplied by 2.4. This was due to the different observation conditions Aulhorn chose in contrast to those used in Adrian's and Blackwell's experiments. Aulhorn used only three subjects. One of them was 55 years old and requested a higher threshold due to reduced ocular transmittance. Furthermore, Aulhorn used monocular observation rather than binocular viewing, which was used in the other investigations. Monocular and binocular observation are known to be different by a factor of around 2, although Campbell and Green (7) reported a higher 6.L threshold for.monocular observations of.64 (see below). The 55-year-old person would demand on average.59 times higher 6.L levels than a 23-year-old subject. The weight with which the readings of the older subject were incorporated in the reported data are not available. However, if the factors of.64 for monocular observation and.59 for the higher age are considered, the total is 2.6, which explains the shift of 2.4 (keeping in mind that two younger subjects also contributed to the mean data, thus lowering the increase caused by the older one). NFLUENCE OF AGE Mortenson-Blackwell and Blackwell (8) and Weale (9) have measured ocular transmittance and found that it decreases with age. This results in higher 6.L thresholds for older people, as shown in Figure 8 (8). From those findings, a multiplier can be derived to account for the age-dependent threshold increase. The findings are obtained for positive contrast, and it is not unreasonable to assume that it holds also for negative contrast. The relatively good fit of the data for negative contrast with the calculated curves in Figure 5 justifies this assumption. The 6.L for subjects older than 23 years, on which the function assumes unity, can be found in the following way: For 23 <Age < 64 64 < Age < 75 Where Age Factor AF is (Age - 9)- AF = 2.lGU +.99 (Age - -6.6) A F = (i +.43 ll. 3 Due to the parameters influencing the light perception, as described in the previous paragraphs, the visibility of a target expressed by the luminance difference threshold can be calculated according to 6.L

Adrian 43 Positive Contrast of Target i calculated by Adrian x 2.4 x + Data from Aulhorn " +....-..._..., --7+ -- "---tt L =.38 + f. --+-- - -. - -r- -------.. -----! Parameter: L = Background luminance cd/m 2 _j -------.. -t - --- --- --- r- -r - - --- --- ----.6 2.5 4 6.3 6 25 4 63 Target size ex [min] FGURE 6 Comparison between calculated curves, as in Figure 5, and direct measurements of thresholds for targets in negative contrast. The calculated curves are obtained by using the positive thresholds of Adrian and Blackwell and applying the contrast polarity function F cp where FcP equals for positive contrast and AF equals for a young observer group with an average age of 23 years. f).l is practically constant for exposure time less than or equal to 2 sec. DSABLTY GLARE The influence of disability glare can be incorporated in a relatively simple way. Glare sources present in the visual field impair vision and require an increase in f).l to keep targets visible. The reason for that phenomenon is well known and lies in stray light produced by the sources of high illuminance in the various eye media, especially in the cornea crystalline lens and in the retinal layers. This stray light superimposes on the retinal image, which results in a reduction of the image layers. This can be expressed as With glare, L,eq represents a uniform luminance that adds to the background luminance (Lb) and is equivalent to the glare effect on the target visibility. This effect increases with smaller angular distance between the glare source and target and with growing illumination at the eye due to the glare source, according to the following expression: where k ± Ea i cd/m2 ; - r E 6 i = illumination in lux at the eye from glare source i ; 8 = glare angle in degrees between the center of the glare source and fixation line valid for.5 < 8 < 3 ; and k = age-dependent constant (for the 2- to 3-year age group, K = 9.2 is obtained). n the case of glare, the adaptation luminance around the location of the target on the retina is consequently composed of Lb and L,eq n the calculation of f).l, Lb is substituted by Lb + L seq

44 TRANSPORTATJON RESEARCH RECORD 247 Negative Contrast Target f'--- -,.. x. <l O.D J------r J-.... - _J calculated by Adian illneo x i - x + 4 - experimental data from Aulhorn 2.4 + + --r--r--------r--a-r--==::.t---'---------...;..----+:+-----------.-4.6 2.5 4 6.3 6 25 4 63 Target size a [min] FGURE 7 Luminance difference thresholds for percent probability of perception for targets of various size and brighter than their background. The curves are calculated and renect Adrian's and Blackwell's measurements. The symbols are data from Aulhorn. Multiplication by 2.4 was necessary to account for different experimental conditions (see text). VSBLTY LEVEL L V So far, this paper has dealt with the numerical description of the luminance difference threshold ill, based on experimental data.!ll indicates a value at which a target of defined size becomes perceptible with near percent probability under the observation conditions used in the laboratory experiments, which included free viewing with binocular observation (monocular in Aulhorn's study). Under practical observation conditions, however, a multiple of ill is needed depending on the visual task demand. n most cases, the luminance difference has to reach a level that allows for form perception or that renders conspicuity to the target. One researcher in the early 95s termed this the "suprathreshold factor." n an old DN (Deutsche lndustrie Norm) standard on signal lights, the multiple of the threshold was named the "safety factor" since it makes the target more visible. n CE Report 9.2 (JO), Blackwell introduced the descriptive term "visibility level (VL)," which indicates V L = fllactua l /llthreshold The visibility level needed to secure safe traffic conditions is a function of the luminance to which the eye is adapted and the degree of form perception or visual acuity that is required. An attempt to determine necessary VL levels resulted in values between and 2 for VL in the luminance range of street lighting (). t has also been shown that a direct relationship exists between VL and the subjective rating of the visibility in street lighting installation (2). The method described in this paper provides this value and allows an estimation regarding whether or not a target can be seen and how much the ill of the target is above the level of threshold perception. CONCLUSON The model presented allows the computation of the threshold luminance difference ill for various sizes of targets as a function of the background luminance Lb, seen in positive and negative contrast. ill, from which the threshold contrast C = ill Lb or the contrast sensitivity CS = Lb/ ill can be derived, applies for binocular, free viewing observations under laboratory conditions.

Adrian 45 5 4 :r: (/) UJ a: :r: t- 3 u.. UJ 2 Q. :::> 234 OBSERVER :::;:..,,-- - - L = cd/m2!,_.a 2 3 4 AGE 5 6 7 8 (YEARS) FGURE 8 Multiple of the threshold contrast required for observer of higher age in relation to the base group with an average of 23 years [adopted from the work of Mortenson-Blackwell and Blackwell (8)]. The visibility level L V indicates how much the!:l of a target is above its threshold value and can be used as a measure to evaluate visibility in lighting installations. For example, according to the latest draft of the American JES Committee, the quality of roadway lighting will be based on VL and recommendations on required visibility levels for different road categories will be made on this basis. REFERENCES. W. Adrian. Lichtteknik (in German), Vol. 2, 969, p. 24. 2. E. Aulhorn. Grae/es Archiv fur Klinische und Experimente/le Ophthalmologie (in German), Vol. 67, No., 964, p. 4. 3. H. R. Blackwell. Optical Society of America Journal, Vol. 36, 946, p. 624. 4. H. J. Schmidt-Claussen. Lichtteknik (in German), Vol. 2, 969, p. 26A. 5. H. R. Blackwell. Light and Vision. n JES Lighting Handbook 4. lluminating Engineering Society, 966. 6. A. Remole. Vision Research, Vol. 7, 977, p.,95. 7. F. W. Campbell and D. G. Green. Nature, Vol. 28, 965, p. 9. 8.. Mortenson-Blackwell and H. R. Blackwell. Project 3, Part B. lluminating Engineering Research nstitute, New York, 98. 9. R. A. Weale. llumination Engineering Society, London, Transactions, Vol. 26, No. 2, 96, p. 95.. CE Report 9.2. nternational Commission on llumination, Vienna, n.d.. W. Adrian. Journal of the lluminating Engineering Society, Vol. 6, No. 2, 987, p. 3. 2. M. S. Janoff. Subjective Rating of Visibility. Presented at the Annual Conference of the lluminating Engineering Society, Minneapolis, Minn., Aug. 988.