ESTIMATING SCALING EXPONENTS IN AUDITORY-NERVE SPIKE TRAINS USING FRACTAL MODELS INCORPORATING REFRACTORINESS

Similar documents
Lowen and Teich, JASA 2 RECEIVED:

Malvin Carl Teich. Boston University and Columbia University Workshop on New Themes & Techniques in Complex Systems 2005

ANALY SIS, SYNTHESIS, AND ESTIMATION OE'FRACTAL-RATE STOCHASTIC POINT PROCESSES

Activity Driven Adaptive Stochastic. Resonance. Gregor Wenning and Klaus Obermayer. Technical University of Berlin.

Controllingthe spikingactivity in excitable membranes via poisoning

Discrete Simulation of Power Law Noise

Learning with Ensembles: How. over-tting can be useful. Anders Krogh Copenhagen, Denmark. Abstract

Point Process Approaches to the Modeling. and Analysis of Self-Similar Trac { Center for Telecommunications Research

High-conductance states in a mean-eld cortical network model

The homogeneous Poisson process

Model neurons!!poisson neurons!

Synchronization, oscillations, and 1/ f noise in networks of spiking neurons

Is the superposition of many random spike trains a Poisson process?

The Spike Response Model: A Framework to Predict Neuronal Spike Trains

How to read a burst duration code

Neuronal Tuning: To Sharpen or Broaden?

!) + log(t) # n i. The last two terms on the right hand side (RHS) are clearly independent of θ and can be

Bayesian Estimation of Stimulus Responses in Poisson Spike Trains

+ + ( + ) = Linear recurrent networks. Simpler, much more amenable to analytic treatment E.g. by choosing

Gaussian process for nonstationary time series prediction

Stochastic modeling of a serial killer

The Bayesian Brain. Robert Jacobs Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences University of Rochester. May 11, 2017

Forecasting Wind Ramps

Fitting a Stochastic Neural Network Model to Real Data

Response-Field Dynamics in the Auditory Pathway

v n,t n

Information Theory. Mark van Rossum. January 24, School of Informatics, University of Edinburgh 1 / 35

Refractoriness in the maintained discharge of the cat's retinal ganglion cell*

SYSTEM RECONSTRUCTION FROM SELECTED HOS REGIONS. Haralambos Pozidis and Athina P. Petropulu. Drexel University, Philadelphia, PA 19104

Poisson Processes for Neuroscientists

When is an Integrate-and-fire Neuron like a Poisson Neuron?

If we want to analyze experimental or simulated data we might encounter the following tasks:

Performance Comparison of Two Implementations of the Leaky. LMS Adaptive Filter. Scott C. Douglas. University of Utah. Salt Lake City, Utah 84112

A Subspace Approach to Estimation of. Measurements 1. Carlos E. Davila. Electrical Engineering Department, Southern Methodist University

encoding and estimation bottleneck and limits to visual fidelity

Optimal Detection of Visual. Evoked Potentials. Medical Center, Dallas, Texas, USA. Fax: (214)

Temporal Integration of Sound Pressure Determines Thresholds of Auditory-Nerve Fibers

OTHER SYNCHRONIZATION EXAMPLES IN NATURE 1667 Christiaan Huygens: synchronization of pendulum clocks hanging on a wall networks of coupled Josephson j

Consider the following spike trains from two different neurons N1 and N2:

Describing Spike-Trains

Mathematical Models of Cochlear Nucleus Onset Neurons: II. Model with Dynamic Spike-Blocking State

Self-organized Criticality and Synchronization in a Pulse-coupled Integrate-and-Fire Neuron Model Based on Small World Networks

A Three-dimensional Physiologically Realistic Model of the Retina

ROYAL INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY KUNGL TEKNISKA HÖGSKOLAN. Department of Signals, Sensors & Systems

PHASE CHARACTERISTICS OF SOURCE TIME FUNCTION MODELED BY STOCHASTIC IMPULSE TRAIN

Dual Nature Hidden Layers Neural Networks A Novel Paradigm of Neural Network Architecture

Neuronal Dynamics: Computational Neuroscience of Single Neurons

Start-up Noise in 3-D Self-AmpMed

(Received 15 June 1973)

Neural spike statistics modify the impact of background noise

Schiestel s Derivation of the Epsilon Equation and Two Equation Modeling of Rotating Turbulence

Quantifying the Information in Auditory-Nerve Responses for Level Discrimination

From time series to superstatistics

a monte carlo simulation of photomultiplier resolution

(Received 10 November 1964)

The Bias-Variance dilemma of the Monte Carlo. method. Technion - Israel Institute of Technology, Technion City, Haifa 32000, Israel

A pattern recognition method for the RICH-based HMPID detector in ALICE

Disambiguating Different Covariation Types

In recent years there has been a considerable interest and a widespread research effort in the development ofavalanche photodiodes (APDs) with thin mu

Spike Count Correlation Increases with Length of Time Interval in the Presence of Trial-to-Trial Variation

Introduction to Neural Networks U. Minn. Psy 5038 Spring, 1999 Daniel Kersten. Lecture 2a. The Neuron - overview of structure. From Anderson (1995)

Elec4621 Advanced Digital Signal Processing Chapter 11: Time-Frequency Analysis

Neural Encoding: Firing Rates and Spike Statistics

What is it? Where is it? How strong is it? Perceived quantity. Intensity Coding in Sensory Systems. What must sensory systems encode?

Probabilistic Models in Theoretical Neuroscience

Research Article Hidden Periodicity and Chaos in the Sequence of Prime Numbers

arxiv: v1 [nlin.ps] 9 May 2015

Statistical Analysis of Fluctuation Characteristics at High- and Low-Field Sides in L-mode SOL Plasmas of JT-60U

High-dimensional geometry of cortical population activity. Marius Pachitariu University College London

AT2 Neuromodeling: Problem set #3 SPIKE TRAINS

(a) B-V 6 V. (b) B-V

Nonrenewal Statistics of Electrosensory Afferent Spike Trains: Implications for the Detection of Weak Sensory Signals

Supporting Online Material for

Lognormal distribution of firing time and rate from a single neuron?

SchroK dinger equation with imaginary potential

Exercises. Chapter 1. of τ approx that produces the most accurate estimate for this firing pattern.

of the dynamics. There is a competition between the capacity of the network and the stability of the

adap-org/ Jan 1994

and B. Taglienti (b) (a): Dipartimento di Fisica and Infn, Universita di Cagliari (c): Dipartimento di Fisica and Infn, Universita di Roma La Sapienza

1 Periodic stimulations of the Incoherent Feedforward Loop network

Performance of Low Density Parity Check Codes. as a Function of Actual and Assumed Noise Levels. David J.C. MacKay & Christopher P.

in a Chaotic Neural Network distributed randomness of the input in each neuron or the weight in the

Finding a Basis for the Neural State

Objective Functions for Tomographic Reconstruction from. Randoms-Precorrected PET Scans. gram separately, this process doubles the storage space for

cond-mat/ Mar 1995

Fermilab FERMILAB-Conf-00/342-E CDF January 2001

Mid Year Project Report: Statistical models of visual neurons

Average Reward Parameters

Leo Kadanoff and 2d XY Models with Symmetry-Breaking Fields. renormalization group study of higher order gradients, cosines and vortices

Terminal attractor optical associative memory with adaptive control parameter

Deviations from uniform power law scaling in nonstationary time series

Correlations in Populations: Information-Theoretic Limits

Quantal Neurotransmitter Secretion Rate Exhibits Fractal Behavior

Submitted to the Proceedings of the Third International Conference on Dynamical Aspects of Nuclear Fission

When does interval coding occur?

Nonlinear reverse-correlation with synthesized naturalistic noise

Neural variability and Poisson statistics

Kolmogorov-Smirnov method for the determination of signal

Time-Delay Estimation *

EEG- Signal Processing

Transcription:

ESTIMATING SCALING EXPONENTS IN AUDITORY-NERVE SPIKE TRAINS USING FRACTAL MODELS INCORPORATING REFRACTORINESS S.B. LOWEN Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering 44 Cummington Street Boston University, Boston, MA 02215 lowen@bu.edu M.C. TEICH Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering Department of Biomedical Engineering 44 Cummington Street Boston University, Boston, MA 02215 teich@bu.edu Fractal stochastic point processes (FSPPs) provide good mathematical models for long-term correlations present in auditory-nerve bersina number of species. Simulations and analytical results for FSPPs concur with experimental data over long times. Refractoriness-modied Poisson point processes, in contrast, model only the short-term characteristics of these data. FSPPs incorporating refractoriness yield superior results over all time scales, and analytical results for the statistics of this model agree closely with both computer simulations and auditory data. Furthermore, these processes provide a superior method for estimating the fractal exponent, which parametrizes the long-term correlation found in these recordings. 1 Introduction Certain random phenomena are well described by essentially identical events occurring at discrete times. One example is the registration of action potentials recorded by an electrode near an auditory-nerve ber 3 16 17. A (onedimensional) stochastic point process is a mathematical construction which represents these events as random points on a line. Such aprocessmaybe called fractal when a number of the relevant statistics exhibit scaling with related scaling exponents, indicating that the represented phenomenon contains clusters of points over a relatively large set of time scales 11 5 6 7. The power spectral density (PSD) and the Allan factor (AF) are two statistics which reveal this clustering 7 9 1. The PSD S(!) forapoint process, as for continuous-time processes, provides a measure of how power is concentrated as a function of the angular frequency!. The Allan factor A(T ), is dened as the ratio of the Allan variance of the event count in a specied time window 1

T to twice the mean count. The Allan variance, in turn, is the variance of the dierence between the numbers of counts in adjacent windows of duration T. For an FSPP, over a large range of times and frequencies, it can be shown that 7 9 1 PSD: S(!)= 1+(!=! 0 ) ; AF: A(T ) 1+(T=T 0 ) (1) with the average rate of the process, >0 the fractal exponent,! 0 a cuto frequency, T 0 a cuto time, and! 0 T 0 =(2; 2 ) cos(=2);( +2) 9 10.Thus in this mathematical formalism, the fractal exponent can assume values in excess of unity, as sometimes occurs in auditory recordings 9. The homogeneous Poisson point process (HPP) is perhaps the simplest stochastic point process, being fully characterized by a single constant quantity, its rate. It is not fractal, but serves as a building block for other point processes which can be. With the inclusion of refractoriness (dead- and sicktime eects), the refractoriness-modied Poisson point process (RM-P) 14 12 15 proves successful in modeling auditory-nerve spiketrainsover short time scales. With xed and exponentially distributed 20 random refractoriness together 15, the RM-P predicts an interevent-interval histogram (IIH) of the form p(t) = (1 ; r) ;1 fexp[;(t ; f)] ; exp[;(t ; f)=r]g for t>f 0 for t f. where is the original rate of events (before refractory eects), f is the portion of the refractory period that is xed, and r is the mean of the random component. However, the accurate modeling of the self-similar or fractal behavior observed in sensory-system neural spike trains over long times requires a fractal stochastic point process 16 13 17 4 18 2 9. A number of FSPP models have been developed 7 we focus on a particular process, the fractal-gaussian-noise-driven Poisson process (FGNDP) 7.TheFGNDP is important because Gaussian processes are ubiquitous, well understood, and are completely described by their means and autocovariance functions. Thus only three parameters (including the rate) are required to specify an FGNDP, since, like all FSPPs, it follows Eq. 1. The FGNDP describes the ring patterns of primary auditory aerents quite well over long time scales (several hundred milliseconds and larger) 17, although it does not accurately model neural activity over short time scales. Therefore procedures for estimating the fractal exponent based on a simple FSPP formalism that ignores refractoriness, such as a least-squares t to the logarithm of Eq. 1, necessarily yield biased results. The parameter plays a crucial role in characterizing the auditory-nerve ber recordings, since this 2 (2)

single quantity describes the long-term correlations present in such recordings, and the singularity near zero frequency in the PSD 9. 2 Models Incorporating Refractoriness and Fractal Behavior Incorporating refractory eects into the fractal behavior of the FGNDP yields the refractoriness-modied version (RM-FGNDP), a new model which does indeed mimic auditory-nerve behavior over all time and frequency scales 8. Figure 1 shows the AF obtained from a recording of an auditory-nerve ber under spontaneous conditions (solid curve), and from a single simulation of the RM-FGNDP process (dashed curve). All recordings used in this paper were obtained from one cat auditory aerent berhe cat, with a spontaneous rate of 75 spikes/sec, and a characteristic frequency (CF) of 3926 Hz. (Data collection and minimal subsequent processing methods are described in Ref. 9 and references therein.) The two curves agree within the limits imposed by uctuations inherent in a nite data length agreement also obtains for other statistics such as the IIH, not shown. Evidently, simulations of the RM-FGNDP process mimic auditory-nerve-ber data well. Simulations of non-fractal models, in contrast, exhibit AF curves which never exceed unity, and thus are not suitable as models of auditory-nerve ring behavior. Conversely, simulations of DATA SIMULATION A(T ) 10 ;3 10 ;2 10 ;1 COUNTING TIME T Figure 1: Doubly logarithmic plot of the Allan factor (AF) for auditory-nerve-ber data recorded under spontaneous conditions (solid curve), and for a simulation of the RM-FGNDP process with parameters chosen to t the data (dashed curve). 3

non-refractory models exhibit AF curves which never dip below unity, while those of the data always do. Fortuitously, for the parameter ranges employed in modeling these spike trains, the eects of the fractal uctuations are minimal over the time scales where refractoriness dominates, and vice versa thus these two eects may be decoupled in the modeling procedure. In particular, the IIH plots for the full RM-FGNDP theory, the non-fractal RM-P models, and the data all resemble each other closely. The AF and PSD for the RM-FGNDP, in contrast, do dier appreciably from those of both the (non-fractal) RM-P and (non-refractory) FGNDP models. We have developed new analytical formulas for these quantities in the presence of generalized refractoriness (including both xed and exponentially distributed random components) 10, and provide two key results below. For steady-state (equilibrium) counting 12,andforT f r, the AF assumes the form, over all counting times: A(T ) 6a2 +4a 3 + a 4 + 2 r 2 (6 ; 2 r 2 ) ; 4 3 r 3 (1 + f) 4(1 + a) 3 T with a (f + r) 10 the PSD assumes the form 1+ 2 r 2 S(!) (1 + a) 3 +(!=! 0) ; +(1+a) 3 (T=T 0 ) for equilibrium counting in the range! 1=f 1=r 10. Figure 2 provides estimated averaged Allan Factors from a typical simulated FGNDP with = 0:5, both with and without refractoriness (solid curves). Also shown are ts from Eq. 1 for the upper curve, and for the full RM- FGNDP (Eq. 3) model for the lower curve (dashed curves). Both theoretical results match their respective simulations extremely well, particularly at the longer counting times which prove most important in estimating. Table I shows the results from a number of simulations at dierent design values of the fractal exponent. These results are estimated from the simple FGNDP model of Eq. 1 (fourth column), and for the full RM-FGNDP model with refractoriness of Eq. 3 (third column). The RM-FGNDP model yields a much more reliable estimate of. Similar improvement obtains for the PSD when employing the RM-FGNDP model 10. Returning to auditory-nerve data, in Fig. 3 we present AF curves for two stimulus levels: spontaneous ring (no stimulus) as shown in Fig. 1, and +40 db:re threshold at CF. Also shown are the predictions of Eq. 3, obtained from a least-squares t of the logarithm of the AFs. For the upper and lower experimental data curves, the estimated fractal exponents of these ring patterns 4 (3) (4)

SIMULATION LEAST-SQUARES FIT A(T ) 10 ;1 10 2 10 3 COUNTING TIME T 10 4 10 5 Figure 2: Doubly logarithmic plot of the averaged Allan factor (AF) for simulations of the RM-FGNDP process, with = 1, = 0:5, and N = 100 simulations. Upper curves: f = r = 0 for simulation (solid curve)and tfromeq.1(dashedcurve). Lower curves: f = r = 1 for simulation (solid curve) and t from Eq. 3 (dashed curve). are = 0:71 and = 1:5, respectively. These are indeed substantially larger than the values = 0:48 = 0:96 obtained without taking refractoriness into account 9. Within limits imposed by a nite data size, predictions of the RM- Table 1: SIMULATION ESTIMATION Fractal Exp. Refract. Eq. 3 Bias Eq. 1 Bias =0:5 none -0.002-0.046 =0:5 f = r =1 +0.016-0.156 =1:0 none -0.012-0.027 =1:0 f = r =1-0.015-0.058 =1:5 none -0.019-0.035 =1:5 f = r =1-0.027-0.090 Bias in estimates of the fractal exponent obtained from the averaged Allan factor (AF) for simulations of the RM-FGNDP process. For all table entries, = 1 and N = 100 simulations. Results are reported for three values of (0:5, 1:0, and 1:5), both with (f = r = 1) and without (f = r = 0) refractoriness. Bias is calculated by a leastsquares t of the logarithm of the averaged simulated AF plots to the logarithm of Eq. 3, for ten values of the counting time T per decade in the range 10 3 T 10 5 (left bias column) andtoeq.1intherange10 4 T 10 5 (right column). 5

FGNDP model closely follow the AF for the experimental data over all time scales. The PSD predicted by the RM-FGNDP model also accords with that of the data over all frequency scales, as do all other statistics examined to date. Finally, a similar process, based on a gamma renewal process instead of a Poisson substrate, appears to provide an excellent t to data collected from neurons in the visual system of the cat, using these same statistics 19.Thus theoretical results obtained for this process will likely provide a superior method for estimating the fractal exponent of these neurons. 3 Conclusion Over all time scales, analytical predictions of the refractoriness-modied fractal-gaussian-noise-driven Poisson point process model, with xed and stochastic refractory periods together, match statistics of experimental data collected from cat auditory-nerve bers, and of computer simulations of this process. This model therefore provides the best method known to date for estimating the fractal exponent of neural recordings. SPONT. FIT +40 db FIT A(T ) 10 ;3 10 ;2 10 ;1 COUNTING TIME T Figure 3: Doubly logarithmic plot of the Allan factor (AF) plot for auditory data (unit L-19, with CF = 3926 Hz, threshold = 19:3 db SPL, and spontaneous ring rate = 75 spikes/sec.). Upper curves: spontaneous ring conditions (solid curve) and analytical t from Eq. 3 (longdashed curve). Lower curves: stimulus applied at CF at +40 db:re threshold (short-dashed curve) and analytical t from Eq. 3 (dotted curve). 6

Acknowledgments Supported by the Whitaker Foundation under Grant No. CU01455801 and by the Oce of Naval Research under Grant No. N00014-92-J-1251. References 1. Feurstein, M. et al., in preparation. 2. Kelly, O.E. et al. (1996), J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 99 2210-2220. 3. Kiang, N.Y-S. et al. (1965), Discharge Patterns of Single Fibers in the Cat's Auditory Nerve, Res. Monogr. No. 35 (MIT, Cambridge, MA). 4. Kumar, A.R. and Johnson, D.H. (1993), J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 93 3365. 5. Lowen, S.B. and Teich, M.C. (1991), Phys. Rev. A 43 4192{4215. 6. Lowen, S.B. and Teich, M.C. (1993), Phys. Rev. E 47 992{1001. 7. Lowen, S.B. and Teich, M.C. (1995), Fractals 3 183{210. 8. Lowen, S.B. and Teich, M.C. (1996). in Computational Neuroscience, ed J.M. Bower (Academic, New York), pp. 447-452. 9. Lowen, S.B. and Teich, M.C. (1996), J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 99 3585{3591. 10. Lowen, S.B. (1996), CTR Technical Report 449-96-15 (Columbia Univ., New York). 11. B.B. Mandelbrot (1983), The Fractal Geometry of Nature (Freeman, New York). 12. Muller, J.W. (1974), Nucl. Inst. Meth. 117 401{404. 13. Powers, N.L. and Salvi, R.J. (1992), in Abstracts of the Fifteenth Midwinter Research Meeting of the Association for Research in Otolaryngology, ed D.J. Lim (Association for Research in Otolaryngology, Des Moines, IA), Abstract 292, p. 101. 14. Ricciardi, L.M. and Esposito, F. (1966), Kybernetik 3 148{152. 15. Teich, M.C. et al. (1978), J. Opt. Soc. Am. 68 386{402. 16. Teich, M.C. (1989), IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 36 150{160. 17. Teich, M.C. (1992), in Single Neuron Computation, ed. T. McKenna et al. (Academic, Boston), pp. 589{625. 18. Teich, M.C. and Lowen, S.B. (1994), IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Mag. 13 197{202. 19. Teich, M.C. et al. (1996), J. Opt. Soc. Am. A in press. 20. Young, E.D. and Barta, P.E. (1986), J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 79 426{442. 7