Nonrandom Visitation of Brood Cells by Worker Honey Bees (Hymenoptera: Apidae)

Similar documents
Honey Bee Biology Workshop. The Queen. The Drone

Chemical signals: What is one thing in common between a colony & a human society?

Bee Colony Activities Throughout The Year

BASIC BEE BIOLOGY. Tammy Horn, KY State Apiarist

Abstract. Introduction

Peter Dutton. 28 August Halesworth& District

Honey Bees QUB Green Champions 9 th April

Honey Bees. QUB CCRCB 11 th January

Practical 5 SOCIAL BEHAVIOUR OF HONEY BEES

Bees. By: Jourdan Wu, Olakunle Olawonyi, Adina Gibson, Elizabeth Peterson. Image drawn by Adina Gibson using Sketchpad 5.1

Pheromones by Ellen Miller November 2015

Bee Behavior. Summary of an article by. Stephen Taber III from Beekeeping in the United States

Intracolonial nepotism during colony fissioning in honey bees?

the European Honeybee

Honey Bees: A Pollination Simulation

Rearing Honeybee Queens in, Apis Mellifera L. Colonies During the Activity Season of Oriental Wasps Vespa Orientalis L

Activity: Honey Bee Adaptation Grade Level: Major Emphasis: Major Curriculum Area: Related Curriculum Areas: Program Indicator: Student Outcomes:

Learning about bees - Maths Questions

ACCURACY OF MODELS FOR PREDICTING PHENOLOGY OF BLACKHEADED FIREWORM AND IMPLICATIONS FOR IMPROVED PEST MANAGEMENT


SC741 W12: Division of Labor Part I: Fixed- and Variable- Threshold Algorithms

28 3 Insects Slide 1 of 44

Swarming Biology of Honey Bees

Apis mellifera scuttelata. Common names: African honeybee also nicknamed the killer bee

split into two groups: a control group and a treatment group. Each beekeeper must have a

Glossary of Terms used in Beekeeping

Social Insects. Social Insects. Subsocial. Social Insects 4/9/15. Insect Ecology

Social Insects. Insect Ecology

The Honey Bee Pollinators Charlie Vanden Heuvel BG BEES

INSTRUCTIONS TO TEACHERS. Bee Life Grade 2 Science and Technology Unit

Bee Communica,on A Hive Mind

Lesson Plan: Vectors and Venn Diagrams

Eusocial species. Eusociality. Phylogeny showing only eusociality Eusocial insects. Eusociality: Cooperation to the extreme

Juvenile hormone profiles of worker honey bees, Apis mellifera, during normal and accelerated behavioural development

Responses of queenright and queenless workers of Apis cerana to 9-keto-2(E)-decenoic acid, a pheromonal constituent of the mandibular gland

Biology of the Colony. Dr. Deborah Delaney

Latency time and absence of group effect: two examples

Pollination of Pumpkin and Winter Squash - Thanks to Bumble Bees! Dr. Kimberly Stoner Connecticut Agricultural Experiment Station New Haven

Ode-to-nata (Aeshna canadensis) productions presents

SOBA Bee School April, 2015

Available online at ScienceDirect. Procedia Computer Science 20 (2013 ) 90 95

Funding for the duplication of this publication is provided by the St. Lucie County Board of County Commissioners.

Apiguard Frequently Asked Questions

What is insect forecasting, and why do it

In-hive pollen transfer between bees enhances cross-pollination of plants

The Effect of Temperature on Hind wing Vein of Apis cerana cerana during Sealed Brood s Development 1)

Introduction to Swarm Robotics

Outline. 1 Initial Definitions. 2 Examples of SI in Biology. 3 Self-Organization. 4 Stigmergy. 5 Swarm Robotics

The Biology of the Honeybee, Apis Mellifera

Effects of colored light on Vanessa Cardui Caterpillars

CORRELATIVE ANALYSES OF BROOD RATIO AND ROYAL JELLY PRODUCTION

Lesson: The Buzz on Bees

USE OF APIGUARD Frequently Asked Questions

There are approximately 25,000 species of Bee in the World There are almost 4000 species of Bee in North America There are approximately 1000

Biology Principles of Ecology Oct. 20 and 27, 2011 Natural Selection on Gall Flies of Goldenrod. Introduction

VARIABILITY OF CHEMOSENSORY STIMULI WITHIN HONEYBEE (Apis mellifera) COLONIES: Differential Conditioning Assay for Discrimination Cues

o Can you find any nectar? Brood? Honey? o Can you find any drones and drone cells? o Can you find the queen bee?

Objectives. Bee Basics. Apis mellifera. Honey bees 3/13/2019. All about bees Pheromones in the hive Obtaining bees Foraging and nutrition Bee diseases

Module 1: Darwin s Bees

Pollination A Sticky Situation! A lesson from the New Jersey Agricultural Society s Learning Through Gardening program

Effects of Dietary Protein Levels on Royal Jelly Production and Hypopharyngeal Gland Development of ZND No. 1 Italian Honeybee

Life History of the Honey Bee Tracheal Mite (Acari: Tarsonemidae)

Division of Labour and Task Allocation

Polyphenic Insects. genotype X environment = phenotype POLYPHENISM. genetic polymorphism vs polyphenism. the peppered moth.

Lesson: Honeybees and Trees

Approximate Pacing for First Grade Insects and Plants Unit

Bees Are Cool. Bee First Aid

SYSTEMATICS, MORPHOLOGY AND PHYSIOLOGY

Name: Date Block A Trail of Termites

Heat shielding: a task for youngsters

Seasonal Variation in a Hymenopterous Parasitoid, Holcotetrastichus rhosaces

Pollination Lab Bio 220 Ecology and Evolution Fall, 2016

Searching for the egg-marking signal in honeybees

Bee Basics. Objectives. Honey bees 3/15/2018. All about bees Pheromones in the hive Obtaining bees Foraging and nutrition Protecting pollinators

The Bee, Part I: Dance of the Bees Gioietta Kuo

Observation system for the control of the hive environment by the honeybee (Apis mellifera)

Cambridge International Examinations Cambridge Pre-U Certificate

The Biology of the Honeybee, Apis Mellifera

When do honey bee guards reject their former nestmates after swarming?

the primary pollinator for more than 50 fruit and vegetable crops, which form the nutritional

Musk thistle and Canada thistle

Exploited superorganisms how life history shapes the reproductive strategies of honeybees

Expression Differences in the Caste Development of Honeybee Using Solexa Sequencing Method

Entomological Accuracy in the Popular Press:

Supplemental Figure 1. Mating pair of Poecilographa decora. Photo by Steve Marshall, Professor and Collection Director, University of Guelph, Guelph,

VARIATION IN AND RESPONSES TO BROOD PHEROMONE OF. THE HONEY BEE (Apis mellifera)

ROLES OF INDIVIDUAL HONEYBEE WORKERS AND DRONES IN COLONIAL THERMOGENESIS

Lesson: Honeybees and Trees

13 November 2005 Volume XIII No. 11

The Influence of Photoperiod on the Flight Activity of Honeybees

Honeybees: Foraging Behavior, Reproductive Biology And Diseases (Insects And Other Terrestrial Arthropods: Biology, Chemistry And Behavior)

STEREOCHEMISTRY OF HOST PLANT MONOTERPENES AS MATE LOCATION CUES FOR THE GALL WASP Antistrophus rufus

Effects of Feeding by Liquorice Root Extract on Some Biological Aspects of Honey Bees(Apis mellifera L.)

Introduction. Description. Bumble bee:

Bees. Garden Classroom. 90 minutes. Grade 6 4/10. ESY Berkeley Teaching Staff Edible Schoolyard Project Berkeley, CA. Bees Safety Pollinators Honey

Word Match Match the vocabulary words with the defi nitions below.

Comparative study of the hygienic behavior of Carniolan and Africanized honey bees directed towards grouped versus isolated dead brood cells

Selection for late pupariation affects diapause incidence and duration in the flesh fly, Sarcophaga bullata

NATURE S. Insect Pollinators, Plants, and. The pedigree of honey Does not concern the bee; A clover, anytime, to her Is aristocracy.

a British scientist who left his fortune to the United States Congress had trouble getting the Smithsonian project off the ground.

Transcription:

Journal of lnsect Behavior, Vol. 4, No. 2, 1991 Nonrandom Visitation of Brood Cells by Worker Honey Bees (Hymenoptera: Apidae) Zhi-Yong Huang 1'2 and Gard W. Otis t Accepted June 28, 1990; revised August 6, 1990 The visitation pattern by worker honey bees to cells in the brood nest was monitored on an artificially created brood pattern consisting of about one-fourth brood cells evenly distributed among empty cells. The majority (63 %) of the observed workers selectively entered larval cells. In contrast, some workers avoided egg cells when presented a choice of egg vs empty cells. The results suggest that larvae produce a general signal indicating their presence to worker bees. Eggs also seem to produce a signal, which is perceived to be different from the one from larvae. KEY WORDS: honey bee; Apis mellifera; brood signal; communication. INTRODUCTION Communication among members is important in insect societies, as it facilitates colony coordination and integration. Recognition of castes is an important part of communication, as it is a precondition for many other interactions. For example, in a honey bee colony, a queen first has to be recognized as a queen before other activities such as retinue formation and feeding with royal jelly (Snodgrass, 1956) can proceed. Similarly, the ability of worker bees to recognize brood, its stage of development, sex, and caste, is a prerequisite for appropriate brood care and larval feeding (Free, 1987). Department of Environmental Biology, University of Guelph, Guelph, Ontario, Canada, NIG 2W1. 2Current address: Department of Entomology, University of Illinois, 320 Morrill Hall, 505 South Goodwin, Urbana, Illinois 61801. 177 0892-7553/91/0300-0177506.50/0 9 1991 Plenum Publishing Corporation

178 Huang and Otis Workers distinguish pupae of different ages (Free and Winder, 1983). They apparently can distinguish the larvae of all three castes, since drone, worker, and queen larvae receive food with different compositions (Gontarski, 1954; Shuel and Dixon, 1959; Jung-Hoffmann, 1966). In addition, nurse bees can also recognize the age of larvae, as the feeding behavior and composition of larval food vary with larval age (Shuel and Dixon, 1959; Jung-Hoffmann, 1966; Thrasyvoulou et al., 1983; Brouwers, 1984; Beetsma, 1985; Brouwers et al., 1987). Workers also seem to determine the food requirement of larvae during the inspections (Huang, 1988). The recognition of certain brood characteristics and other conditions could occur at different levels. A worker could determine all those characteristics in one step without actually entering each cell. Alternatively, it could perceive a series of signals in steps: first differentiating brood cells from other cells, perhaps without actually going into each cell; then determining whether the brood is in the egg or larval stage; and finally, after receiving more elaborate stimuli, recognizing the actual age of either egg or larva and responding accordingly. To understand further brood-worker behavioral interactions, we tested whether worker bees would visit brood cells preferentially among a patch of mixed cells. A positive finding would suggest that brood has a relatively longrange (over the cell depth) signal or cue to communicate its presence to worker bees, resulting in an increased nursing efficiency. MATERIALS AND METHODS The experiment was conducted at the field laboratory of the University of Guelph, Ontario. The honey bees were a mixture of European bee races (Apis mellifera L.). A four-frame observation hive was established in late May 1987. The colony population was estimated to have 12,000-15,000 worker bees (estimated by counting bees in samples of quadrate on each frame) throughout the summer. The room temperature was controlled at 31 + I~ and kept dark except during the actual observation periods, when the room was illuminated with multiple fluorescent lights. Previous study (Huang and Otis, 1990) under the same lighting conditions reported highly consistent data on feeding behavior compared to those conducted under red light (Brouwers et al., 1987). A cohort of 100 worker bees less than 18 h old was color-marked with Testor's paint and introduced to the observation hive. About a week later, a queen was confined on an empty comb for 24 h in a source hive to obtain large numbers of eggs less than 24 h old. A brood pattern as shown in Fig. 1 was created by destroying every other egg both vertically and horizontally. This comb and another one (with larvae 1 to 3 days old and some capped brood) from the same source colony replaced two of the original combs in the observation hive at the start of the experiment. The location of each cell with an egg

Nonrandom Visitation of Honey Bee Brood Cells 179 Fig. 1. Special brood pattern created to study the visitation of worker bees to brood vs empty cells. The pattern was created by destroying ever,./other egg both horizontally and vertically. Approximately one of four cells contained an egg (or a larva after it hatches). This photo was taken after the experiment was over, when all cells contained capped brood. and the perimeter of the area with eggs were marked on the glass surface of the observation hive. The queen in the observation hive remained confined within a 12 12-cm iron-mesh screen cage during the experiment, through which she could be fed by workers and laid some eggs within. No signs of queen rearing were observed. Observations started the next day and terminated 6 or 7 days later, when some cells were beginning to be capped by worker bees. During observations, a marked bee was randomly chosen in the brood area, and its visitation sequence to cells was recorded. When a bee inserted its head or head and thorax into a cell, the content of the cell (e.g., none, egg, larva) was recorded. Observation of a particular bee was terminated when it left the delineated brood area. Observations (50 _+ 10 min) were made once a day between 0800 and 1800, and five to eight marked bees were followed during each time period. Two trials were performed for this experiment in the same observation hive 20 days apart. Observations were made of 7- to 13-day-old bees in trial 1 and 10- to 15-dayold bees in trial 2. Two things can happen if a bee can discriminate brood cells from empty

180 Huang and Otis ones: it either enters brood cells preferentially or does so to empty cells. A bee could also not discriminate brood cells from empty cells and thus visit both types of cells "randomly"; i.e., the visitation rates to either cells depend on their relative abundance. The serial visitations to both cell types by a worker bee during an observation bout can be described by a binomial distribution. We used the following formula to calculate the probability of a bee either (1) preferentially entering brood cells--this hypothesis is tested when observed number of visits to brood cells are larger than expected--or (2) preferentially entering empty cells--this is tested when observed number of visits to brood cells are smaller than expected: N! v~ ZP(>Y) =T~.xv.p tl-p)x, T= Y,Y+ 1...N (1) N! vl EP(<Y) = T.~-~.T p (1 _p)x, T= Y, Y- 1... 0 (2) where p is the probability that a randomly-selected cell contains brood. The value p is the ratio of brood cells to total cells (brood + empty), which was close to 0.25 in both trials. X is the number of observed visits to empty cells, Y to brood cells, and N is the total of X and Y. A value of ~ P (( > Y) or Z P ( < Y) smaller than 0.05 indicates that it is unlikely (i.e., P < 0.05) that such an event would occur if the bee is visiting indiscriminately, and the null hypothesis can thus be rejected for that particular bee. Experimental-wise significance was tested by X 2 test (Steel and Torrie, 1980). RESULTS AND DISCUSSION The results reported in Tables I and II are itemized by each "bee bout"-- the numbers of visits to both empty and brood cells by a bee during one observation period. Since bees were not individually identified, the same bee could potentially be recorded in several bee bouts here. Nevertheless, due to the relatively large number of bees compared to the bee bouts (200 vs 86), this should not be a serious bias. With a few exceptions, workers bees biased their visits to larval cells, and the majority (59 and 69% for trials 1 and 2, respectively) of them tested significant at 5 % level or higher (Tables I and II). X 2 test indicated that both these proportions were too high to be attributable to the associated type I error rate (P < 0.001). At the other extreme, two bees (Nos. 24 and 25, Table II) bias their visits strongly toward empty cells, but only one (No. 24) was significant; the other one was marginal. For cells with eggs, the picture appears to be quite different. Among a total of 24 bees observed, only 2 bees (No. 1 in Table I and No. 1 in Table II) significantly preferred egg cells, while more bees (5 of 12 in

Table I. Results of Trial 1 Brood Bee age Bee No. visits No. visits stage (days) bout to brood a to empty ~ E P ( > y)h ~ p ( < y), Eggs 7 1 6 (2.3) 3 (6.7) 0.01"* 2 5 (3.3) 8 (9.7) 0.21 3 1 (2.3) 8 (6.7) 0.30 4 6 (5.5) 16 (16.5) 0.49 5 4 (3.7) 11 (11.3) 0.54 6 4 (3.7) 11 (11.3) 0.54 7 4 (3.3) 9 (9.7) 0.42 8 4 (l.8) 3 (5.2) 0.07 Eggs 8 9 7 (6.3) 18 (18.7) 0.44 10 3 (2.3) 6 (6.7) 0.40 11 6 (5.5) 16 (16.5) 0.48 12 3 (2.8) 8 (8.2) 0.58 Larvae 9 13 6 (3.3) 7 (9.7) 0.08 14 7 (3.8) 8 (11.2) 0.06 15 8 (2.5) 2 (7.5) 0.00"** 16 1t (3.8) 4 (11.2) 0.00"** 17 5 (1.8) 2 (5.2) 0.01"* 18 11 (3.5) 3 (10.5) 0.00"** 19 7 (2.8) 4 (8.2) 0.01"* Larvae 10 20 4 (1.3) 1 (3.7) 0.02* 21 8 (2.8) 3 (8.2) 0.00"* 22 7 (2.0) 1 (6.0) 0.00"** 23 3 (2.3) 6 (6.7) 0.40 24 4 (4.0) 12 (12.0) 0.60 25 4 (2.5) 6 (7.5) 0.23 Larvae 11 26 8 (3.0) 4 (9.0) 0.00"* 27 6 (5.8) 17 (17.2) 0.53 28 8 (3.3) 5 (9.7) 0.01"* 29 5 (3.3) 8 (9.7) 0.21 30 1 (2.3) 8 (6.7) 0.30 31 3 (23) 6 (6.7) 0.40 32 8 (20) 0 (6.0) 0.00"** 33 9 (3.0) 3 (9.0) 0.00"** Larvae t2 34 11 (2.8) 0 (8.2) 0.00"** 35 9 (2.8) 2 (8.2) 0.00"** 36 9 (2.5) 1 (7.5) 0.00"** 37 4 (4.3) 13 (12.7) 0.57 38 4 (3.3) 9 (9.7) 0.42 39 t0 (4.3) 7 (12.7) 0.00"* Larvae 13 40 6 (5.5) 16 (16.5) 0.49 41 5 (3.0) 7 (9.0) 0. t6 42 8 (2.3) 1 (6.7) 0.00"** 43 7 (3.3) 6 (9.7) 0.02* 44 6 (2.0) 2 (6.0) 0.00"* 45 4 (3.0) 8 (9.0) 0.35 46 8 (3.8) 7 (11.2) 0.02* ""No. visits to brood" and "No. visits to empty" represents the number of observed visits to brood (egg/larval) cells and empty cells, respectively. The ratio of brood cells to total cells in this trial is 240/957. The numbers in the parentheses are the expected number of visits to brood cells and empty cells, if random, respectively. They were calculated as, respectively, 0.251 x total visits (No. visits to brood + No. visits to empty) and (1-0.251) total visits. ~'"E P( > Y)" is the calculated accumulative binomial probability for a bee to visit the observed number of brood cells and more, when assuming the bees are visiting the brood cells randomly. This test was done on bee bouts which biased toward brood cells. c"ep(< Y)" is the calculated accumulative binomial probability for a bee to visit the observed number of brood cells and less, when assuming the bees are visiting the brood cells randomly. This test was done on bee bouts which biased against brood cells.

182 Huang and Otis Table II. Results of Trial 2. Brood Bee age Bee No. visits No. visits stage (days) bout to brood" to empty ~ ~P( > Y) EP(<Y) Eggs 10 1 6 (1.6) 1 (5.4) 0.00"** 2 4 (2.3) 6 (7.7) 0.18 3 1 (5.7) 24 (19.3) 4 3 (4.6) 17 (15.4) 5 1 (3.7) 15 (12.3) Eggs 11 6 0 (2.7) 12 (9.3) 7 2 (4.1) 16 (13.9) 8 0 (3.0) 13 (10.0) 9 0 (3.4) 15 (11.6) 10 i (3.9) 16 (13.1) 11 1 (4.4) 18 (14.6) 12 1 (4.1) 17 (13.9) Larvae 12 13 14 (4.4) 5 (14.6) 0.00"** 14 7 (2.7) 5 (9.3) 0.01"* 15 9 (3.7) 7 (12.3) 0.00"* 16 6 (3.2) 8 (10.8) 0.08 17 6 (2.7) 6 (9.3) 0.04* 18 4 (2.3) 6 (7.7) 0.18 0.01"* 0.30 0.09 0.04* 0.18 0.03* 0.02* 0.07 0.05* 0.06 Larvae 13 19 7 (2.3) 3 (7.7) 0.00"* 20 6 (2.7) 6 (9.3) 0.04* 21 8 (3.4) 7 (11.6) 0.01"* 22 11 (3.2) 3 (10.8) 0.00"** 23 12 (4.8) 9 (16.2) 0.00"** Larvae 14 24 0 (2.7) 12 (9.3) 25 0 (2.5) 11 (8.5) 26 10 (3.0) 3 (10.0) 0.00"** 27 4 (1.6) 3 (5.4) 0.05* 28 13 (3.4) 2 (11.6) 0.00"** 29 9 (3.7) 7 (12.3) 0.00"* 30 4 (1.6) 3 (5.4) 0.05* 0.04* 0.06 Larvae 15 31 5 (3.0) 8 (10.0) 0.15 32 4 (3.2) 10 (10.8) 0.40 33 12 (4.1) 6 (13.9) 0.00"** 34 9 (3.9) 8 (13.1) 0.01"* 35 11 (4.1) 7 (13.9) 0.00"** 36 7 (3.9) 10 (13.1) 0.07 37 13 (4.6) 7 (15.4) 0.00"** 38 4 (2.1) 5 (6.9) 0.13 ~The ratio of brood cells to total cells is 280/1223 in this trial. The numbers in the parentheses are the expected number of visits to brood cells and empty ceils, if random, respectively. They are calculated as, respectively, 0.229 x total visits (No. visits to brood + No. visits to empty), and (1-0.229) X total visits. Other column headings the same as in Table I.

Nonrandom Visitation of Honey Bee Brood Cells 183 trial 2) preferentially visited empty cells over egg cells (X 2 test, P < 0.01). Preferential visitation to larval cells was observed so frequently (62% of bees observed with both trials pooled, P < 0.001) that it probably represents a real biological phenomenon. In addition, the data suggest that cells with eggs are at times avoided. The different behavior of bees to egg and larval ceils seems to indicate the brood signal is stage specific; i.e., the egg cells are recognized as different from larval cells without entering the cells first. Thirty-seven percent of the worker bees failed to make the distinction between empty and larval cells. Those bees may have been less experienced or less specialized on brood rearing, or they may have been doing brood nestrelated tasks other than brood tending. It has been well established that the temporal schedule of bees is not a rigid one. Individuals of the same age can behave quite differently in the same colony, with a rather high degree of plasticity (Lindauer, 1953; Kolmes, 1985). Since a majority of bees preferentially selected cells with larvae over empty ones, it appears that the larvae in the cells emitted a signal to indicate their presence, as proposed by Huang (1988). The apparent ability of larvae to indicate their presence by a general signal is not too surprising, because nurse bees would thus be able to locate larvae without entering every cell and, as a result, increase their efficiency of nursing. Eggs may also have a signal, although the signal seems repellent since some bees avoided egg cells. Lineburg (1924) observed apparent "pausing" of nurse bees over cells without entering, frequently when the cell contains an egg or a young larva. In addition, he suggested that the queen may sense eggs, larvae, or even debris in cells by hesitating over cells without entering, and workers may also avoid egg cells without entering them. We observed similar behaviors, with the bees pausing and lowering the antennae toward the cell before entering or rejecting a cell. It is possible the bees were sensing the cell content during these pauses, and then entering or ignoring the particular cell as dictated by their physiological and/or behavioral conditions at that moment. Our observations, together with those of Lineburg (1924), suggest that the signal(s)/cue(s) indicating the existence of larvae and eggs is (are) chemical in nature. A few studies have shown that volatile chemicals from larvae could be used in various ways of communication. Free and Winder (1983) suggested that a relatively involatile pheromone on the body surface of brood is responsible for the recognition of worker and drone larvae and pupae. Koeniger and Veith (1984) chemically identified a brood pheromone which induces warming behavior by workers. More recently, Le Conte et al. (1989) reported that simple aliphatic esters emitted from drone and worker larvae are used by a parasitic mite Varroa as cues to find its suitable hosts. Further studies are needed to see if the above-studied chemicals could be used by worker bees for larval recognition prior to entering cells.

184 Huang and Otis ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The authors are indebted to R. W. Shuel and S. E. Dixon for valuable advice and discussions. R. W. Shuel, D. J. C. Fletcher, C. Scott-Dupree, D. L. G. Noakes, D. B. McKeown, G. E. Robinson, and P. K. Visscher made suggestions to improve the manuscript. This project was supported by a Canadian (CIDA) scholarship to Z.-Y. Huang and a grant to G. W. Otis from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada. REFERENCES Beetsma, J. (1985). Feeding behavior of nurse bees, larval food composition and caste differentiation in the honey bee (Apis mellifera L. ). In Hrlldobter, B., and Lindauer, M. (eds.), Experimental Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, Mass., pp. 407-410. Brouwers, E. V. M. (1984). Glucose/fructose ratio in the food of honeybee larvae during caste differentiation. J. Apic. Res. 23: 94-101. Brouwers, E. V. M., Ebert, R., and Beetsma, J. (1987). Behaviourial and physiological aspects of nurse bees in relation to the composition of larval food during caste differentiation in the honeybee. J. Apic. Res. 26:11-17. Free, J. B. (1987). Pheromones of Social Bees, Comstock, New York. Free, J. B., and Winder, M. E. (1983). Brood recognition by honeybee (Apis mellifera) workers. Anim. Behav. 31: 539-545. Gontarski, H. (1954). Untersuchungen fiber die Verwertung von Pollen und Hefe zur Brutpflege der Honigbiene. Z. Bienenforsch. 2: 161-180. Huang, Z.-Y. (1988). Mechanism of Hypopharyngeal Gland Activation by Brood of Honey Bees (Apis mellifera L. ), Ph.D. dissertation, University of Guelph, Ontario, Canada. Huang, Z.-Y., and Otis, G. W. (1990). Inspection and feeding of larvae by worker honey bees: Effect of starvation and food quantity. J. Insect Behav. (in press). Jung-Hoffmann, I. (1966). Die Determination yon K6nigin und Arbeiterin der Honigbiene. Z. Bienenforsch. 8: 296-322. Koeniger, N., and Veith, H. J. (1983). Glyceryl-l,2-dioleate-3-palmitate as a brood pheromone of the honey bee (Apis mellifera L. ). Experientia 39: 1051-1052. Kolmes, S. A. (1985). A quantitative study of the division of labour among worker honey bees. Z. Tierpsychol. 68: 287-302. Le Conte, Y., Arnold, G., Trouiller, J., Masson, C., Chapper, B., and Ourisson, G. (1989). Attraction of the parasitic mite Varroa to the drone larvae of honey bees by simple aliphatic esters. Science 245: 638-639. Lindauer, M. (1953). Division of labour in the honeybee colony. Bee World 34: 63-73, 85-91. Lineburg, B. (1924). The feeding of honeybee larvae. Bull. U.S. Dept. Agr. 1222: 25-37. Shuel, R. W., and Dixon, S. E. (1959). Studies in the mode of action of royal jelly in honeybee development. II. Respiration of newly emerged larvae on various substrates. Can. J. Zool. 37: 803-813. Snodgrass, R. E., (1956). Anatomy of the Honey Bee, Comstock, Cornell University Press, Ithaca, N.Y. Steel, R. G. D., and Torrie, J. H. (1980). Principles and Procedures of Statistics, a Biometrical Approach, McGraw-Hill, New York. Thrasyvoulon, A. T., Collison, C. H., and Benton, A. W. (1983). Electrophoretic patterns of water-soluble proteins of honeybee larval food. J. Apic. Res. 22: 142-145.