A Driver Interface for a Road Hazard Warning System: Development and Preliminary Evaluation

Similar documents
UMTRl. ;q,j. Development and Driver Understanding of Hazard Warning and Location Symbols for IVSAWS

Snow Plow Safety Quick Reference Guide

SUMMARY. ) t, UNIT. Constant velocity represents uniform motion. Acceleration causes a change in velocity.

Evaluation of fog-detection and advisory-speed system

Snow Removal Policy WARREN COUNTY ENGINEER S OFFICE (WCEO) WARREN COUNTY HIGHWAY DEPARTMENT. October 16, 2014

MUNICIPALITY OF ANCHORAGE PUBLIC TRANSPORTATION DEPARTMENT AnchorRIDES TRANSPORTATION SYSTEM

A Thesis Proposal. Agrawal, Ravi. Submitted to the Office of Graduate Studies of Texas A&M University

Organized Chain-Up and VSL

Dust Storms in Arizona: The Challenge to Ensure Motorist Safety Jennifer Toth, P.E. Deputy Director for Transportation

FIREHOUSE MAGAZINE EMERGENCY VEHICLE OPERATIONS PERSONALLY OPERATED VEHICLES P.O.V. S KILL

ALASKA ITS. Vehicles and Observations in the cloud

Snow Removal Equipment Visibility

EFFECTS OF WEATHER-CONTROLLED VARIABLE MESSAGE SIGNING IN FINLAND CASE HIGHWAY 1 (E18)

Town of Windsor. Department of Public Works

Crisis Coordinator Communique. Follow us on: facebook.com/ kennesawstateoem K Social Media

PILOT STUDY: PAVEMENT VISUAL CONDITION AND FRICTION AS A PERFORMANCE MEASURE FOR WINTER OPERATIONS

1,836 people die and 136,309 people are injured every year from icy roads in the US.

GREEN SHEET. California Department of Forestry and Fire Protection (CAL FIRE)

H IG HWAY WINTER OPERATIONS SNOW REMOVAL AND ICE CONTROL POLICY. Revised January 4, 2016

Session 6: and Evaluation. M c Farland Management, LLC

Standard Highway Sign Border Specifications

City of Hailey Public Works - Street Division

Choosing a Safe Vehicle Challenge: Analysis: Measuring Speed Challenge: Analysis: Reflection:

City of Laramie, Wyoming SNOW AND ICE POLICY

SNOW AND ICE REMOVAL. Policy i

City of Brainerd, Minnesota Snowplowing Policy

The Use of Geographic Information Systems (GIS) by Local Governments. Giving municipal decision-makers the power to make better decisions

PW 001 SNOW REMOVAL AND SANDING FOR ROADWAYS AND SIDEWALKS October 6, 2014 (#223-14) Original October 19, 2015; October 15, 2018 Public Works

1. Major streets, Central Business District (CBD) and school bus routes. 2. Local streets 3. Downtown parking lots

Town of Bowden Province of Alberta Policy Document ( ) SNOW REMOVAL POLICY

Evaluating the Potential of Remote Sensing Rural Road and Travel Conditions

CITY OF NEW LONDON WINTER ROAD & SIDEWALK MAINTENANCE POLICY

City of Saginaw Right of Way Division Snow and Ice Removal Policy January 18, 2016

Work Sheet. Adverse Condition Factors Module 9

Karl Reichle. Mark Owens. and. present :

Coconino County Common Snow Plow Questions/Concerns

ShakeAlert Phase 1: West Coast Earthquake Early Warning. Doug Given, USGS EEW Coordinator Education Symposium, Dec. 4, 2018

PRESENTER'S GUIDE "HAZMAT LABELING" Training for the OSHA HAZARDOUS WASTE OPERATIONS and EMERGENCY RESPONSE (HAZWOPER) REGULATION

WINTER MAINTENANCE TRAINING VINCE LOMBARDI

STATISTICAL ANALYSIS OF LAW ENFORCEMENT SURVEILLANCE IMPACT ON SAMPLE CONSTRUCTION ZONES IN MISSISSIPPI (Part 1: DESCRIPTIVE)

Checklist: Deposing the Driver in an Auto Accident

UNIVERSITY OF WISCONSIN-PARKSIDE TORNADO WARNINGS AND ALERT POLICY ADMINISTRATIVE POLICY NUMBER 39 TABLE OF CONTENTS

When is sign lighting needed? How much is needed?

Whether you are driving or walking, if you come to a flooded road, Turn Around Don't Drown

TOWN OF THORNTON, NEW HAMPSHIRE WINTER AND INCLEMENT WEATHER POLICY AND PRIORITIES

Motor vehicle and pedal cycle conspicuity - part 3: retroflective and fluorescent materials, disability glare of red markings.

John Laznik 273 Delaplane Ave Newark, DE (302)

MUNICIPALITY OF ANCHORAGE PUBLIC TRANSPORTATION DEPARTMENT AnchorRIDES PARATRANSIT SYSTEM

Dunn County Snow Removal Policy

CITY OF BEAVER DAM SNOW & ICE REMOVAL POLICY

WINTER OPERATIONS SNOW REMOVAL AND ICE CONTROL POLICY Rochester, NH (Includes Highway, Sidewalks, Parking Areas)

OXFORD HEIGHTS SUBDIVISION EMERGENCY EVACUATION PLAN

2018 EMERGENCY GUIDELINES Plan revision date: March 8, 2018

Tahlequah Public Schools Inclement Weather Transportation Plan

CUSTER COUNTY SNOW REMOVAL PROCEDURES

The following emergency snow routes are generally cleared first:

Typical information required from the data collection can be grouped into four categories, enumerated as below.

Identify the letter of the choice that best completes the statement or answers the question.

Land Navigation Table of Contents

Weather Information for Road Managers

Responsive Traffic Management Through Short-Term Weather and Collision Prediction

Effect of Driving Environment on Drivers Eye Movements: Re-Analyzing Previously Collected Eye-tracker Data

City of Lee s Summit Public Works Operations

TOWN OF WINDSOR SNOW CONTROL PLAN

Severe Storm Response Version # 1-0-1

MDSS Functional Prototype Display System Preview April 2002

The Road to Improving your GIS Data. An ebook by Geo-Comm, Inc.

HSQE Briefing November 2017

Inclement Weather Preparedness. Tornadoes and Severe Thunderstorms

Denton County SKYWARN Guidelines

Town of Barnstable. Department of Public Work. Snow and Ice Control Operations Plan

Xiaoguang Wang, Assistant Professor, Department of Geography, Central Michigan University Chao Liu,

Snow Cover. Snow Plowing and Removal Services Best Practices Guide. l

FANNIN COUNTY A.R.E.S. and R.A.C.E.S. Reference Manual SKYWARN MISSION TO LOCATE POSSIBLE THREATS TO LIFE AND PROPERTY. And

CITY OF EAST PEORIA SNOW AND ICE CONTROL PROGRAM INTRODUCTION

SNOW REMOVAL GUIDE. City Of Orange Township. Public Works Snow Removal Hotline: (973) My Orange Hotline: (973)

BOONE COUNTY Snow Removal Program and Policy SECTION 1, INTRODUCTION:

What s New in the World of Winter Maintenance Technology. Laser Road Surface Sensor (LRSS) Functional Description

Home Owners. Age Male/Female. Snow removal for his/her driveway or private property

StormReady Supporter Application Form updated by NWS Chanhassen on April 7, Name of Applying Entity: Point of Contact: Title:

Establishing Winter Standard Operating Procedures

Transportation and Road Weather

Data Collection. Lecture Notes in Transportation Systems Engineering. Prof. Tom V. Mathew. 1 Overview 1


Neighborhood Locations and Amenities

Trainee Manual C02 CITY OF SASKATOON. Water and Sewer Section. Severe Storm Response Trainee Manual. Version # 1-0-3

Real-time, Adaptive Prediction of Incident Delay for Advanced Traffic Management Systems

Municipal Act, 2001 Loi de 2001 sur les municipalités

106 PURDUE ENGINEERING EXTENSION DEPARTMENT

A CASE STUDY ON EDUCATING ENGINEERING STUDENTS ON THE CHALLENGES OF URBANIZED AT-GRADE INTERSECTIONS

H. T. Zwahlen, A. Russ, and T. Schnell 1

GIS Needs Assessment. for. The City of East Lansing

Decision Support Part 1: Tools to aid travel planning

CITY OF ORILLIA POLICY MANUAL. Winter Operations. Table of Contents. WINTER CONTROL Objectives 2. WINTER CONTROL Season 3

SNOW CONTROL OPERATIONS 2017 PUBLIC SERVICES DEPARTMENT MAINTENANCE DIVISION

Severe Weather Hazards Are Real

Hobart and William Smith Colleges. Hazard Communication Program

A FIELD EXPERIMENT ON THE ACCURACY OF VISUAL ASSESSMENT OF WINTER ROAD CONDITIONS

Emergency Traffic Management In the Case of Severe Winter Conditioins

Transcription:

A Driver Interface for a Road Hazard Warning System: Development and Preliminary Evaluation Paul Green University of Michigan Transportation Research Institute Human Factors Division 2901 Baxter Road Ann Arbor, Michigan 48109-2150 USA ABSTRACT This paper concerns in-vehicle warnings for accidentprovocative situations (accidents ahead, new traffic signals, police cars on emergency runs, etc.). Candidate warnings were developed for 30 hazards based on drawings from 10 drivers. Subsequently, 75 drivers ranked these warnings. Preferred warnings were not always in the standard U.S. highway sign set. In general, text warnings were slightly preferred over symbolic warnings. In a third experiment, 20 drivers were shown 10 hazard location cue designs: 2 text, 4 arrow, 3 overview, and 1 inside-out. Of the location cues, text ("on right," "behind," etc.) was best understood. 1. INTRODUCTION This paper summarizes a detailed technical report concerning the driver interface for a prototype invehicle warning system (Hoekstra, Williams, and Green, 1993) to which readers are directed for additional details. This study was part of a large multi-year project concerning the safety and ease of use of new driver information systems. The objectives were to develop human factors guidelines (Green, Levison, Paelke, and Serafin, 1993), methods for testing the safety and ease of use (Green, 1993b), and a model to predict human performance (Levison, 1993; Levison and Cramer, 1993) while using these interfaces. A complete list of the project reports and other publications is included in the final overview report (Green, 1993a). To focus this research on applications, pre-competitive interfaces were designed for five systems, including the warning system described in this paper. As currently envisioned, low power radio beacons would be placed on or near fixed hazards (e.g., narrow underpasses), mobile hazards (e.g., police cars on a run) or temporary hazards (e.g., accidents). The beacon would transmit a hazard-specific message. Nearby vehicles would receive these messages and a warning system, such as IVSAWS (In-vehicle Safety and Advisory Warning System), would display a warning to drivers. The rationale for IVSAWS, and its predecessor, SHAWS (Safety Hazard Advance Warning System) are described in the literature (Peterson and Boyer, 1975; Meyer, Reaser, Keller, Wilson, and Vadeboncoeur, 1982). Prior to this project, only one experiment has been conducted pertaining to the driver interface (Erlichman, 1992). In that experiment, 13 Hughes employees where shown eight symbols on a color display in a laboratory. In general, subjects preferred colored icons over monochrome, warning tones over none at all, and wanted spoken text to accompany the presentation. Given the state of knowledge when this effort was initiated, three questions were examined: 1. Which hazards need warnings? 2. What graphics best represent those hazards? 3. How well are various location cues for hazards understood? It was decided to make the warning system visual, rather than auditory (primarily speech). This was done to minimize competition with existing auditory warnings, promote international use by avoiding language-specific (spoken) messages, and minimize development cost. Also, past experience suggested driver reaction to new speech-based warning systems would be poor. ("Your door is ajar." "No, a door is a door, not a jar.") 2. WHICH HAZARDS NEED WARNINGS? Hazards needing warnings were identified from the benefit scores in Green, Serafin, Williams, and Paelke, 1991 and Serafin, Williams, Paelke, and Green, 1991, the Streff, Ervin, and Blower, 1991 review of selected accident case files, and the discussion in Meyer, Reaser, Keller, Wilson, and Vadeboncoeur, 1982. The empirical data was supplemented with discussions with people the

warnings should protect (postal vehicle drivers, hearse drivers, fire fighters, etc.). These efforts led to the identification of 30 hazards for which warnings should be considered. (See Tables 1, 2, and 3.) Table 1 In-vehicle signing hazards Accident ahead. Hazard 1 mile ahead. Hazard approaching in opposite direction. Lanes shift/jog to the right. New stop sign. New traffic light. Right lane closed, merge into left 2 lanes. Road construction area, speed limit 45 mi/hr. Road construction. Sharp curve, speed limit 30 mi/hr. Traffic light out of order. Train approaching railroad track. Table 2 Atypical vehicle hazards 3. WHAT GRAPHICS BEST REPRESENT THOSE HAZARDS? 3.1 Experiment 1 - Symbol Production To obtain ideas for candidate warning graphics, 10 drivers drew images for the 30 situations of interest and a hazard location indicator. ("You are approaching an area where there is road construction. The symbol should be...") Subjects ranged in age from 21 to 47, with a mean age of 33. Figure 1 shows some typical responses. Drawings represented a wide variety of concepts, even for warnings for which there is a standard highway sign. For the road construction hazard, only three people drew a highway worker (similar to one in the standard sign), including one who drew the worker with a jackhammer instead of a shovel. Likewise, for the railroad crossing hazard, only three people drew the indicative R s inside of the large X. Those who drew the RXR were not the same people who drew the highway worker for construction. Farm vehicle. Mail delivery truck. Parked utility company vehicle. Plow/gravel vehicle plowing or sanding. School bus loading or unloading children. Slow moving vehicle. Tow truck with flashers & disabled vehicle. Trash truck. Wide load. Table 3 Emergency vehicle hazards High speed ambulance with siren & flashers. High speed fire truck with siren & flashers. High speed police car with siren & flashers approaching from behind. High speed police car with siren & flashers approaching from right. High speed police car with siren & flashers in pursuit. High speed police car with siren & flashers. Parked ambulance with flashers. Parked fire truck with flashers. Parked police car with flashers. Figure 1 Responses to question concerning high speed fire truck with siren and flashers. 3.2 Experiment 2 - Testing at a Driver Licensing Office The purpose of this experiment was to determine driver preferences for pictorial warnings developed

from the production experiment, along with warnings based on highway signs and text. Subjects (3 groups of 25 [42 men and 33 women]) responded while waiting at a local driver licensing office. They were from 17 to 81 years old, with a mean age of 30. Subjects completed one of three surveys, each of which concerned a particular class of warnings (invehicle signing, atypical vehicles, and emergency vehicles), along with three general questions. This approach was chosen due to the limited availability of subjects. Each survey contained a series of alternatives for each warning, which subjects ranked from best (=1) to worst. Figure 2 shows an example. Results from this experiment are summarized as a series of figures, each of which shows the mean ranks for each graphic along with explanatory text. See Figure 3 for partial example. 1. An ambulance is approaching you at high speed with its flashers on. AMBULANCE AMBULANCE APPROACHING WATCH FOR AMBULANCE Figure 2 Example ranking question. G E C A ROAD CONSTRUCTI 6 5 4 3 2 1 Worst Best F D B CONSTRUCTI Figure 3 Mean ranks for road construction warnings. Based on the results of the ranking experiment and consistency constraints, recommended warnings were developed. For some warnings, both text and graphic versions were developed. (See Table 4 for examples.)

Table 4 Recommended warnings for traffic control devices. Format Mixed Text Disabled OUT OF traffic ORDER BROKEN signal TRAFFIC SIGNAL New traffic signal TRAFFIC SIGNAL New stop sign STOP STOP SIGN 4. HOW WELL ARE VARIOUS LOCATION CUES FOR HAZARDS UNDERSTOOD? 4.1 Field Experiment on Hazard and Hazard Location Understandability Twenty licensed drivers participated in this experiment, 10 young (21 to 29 years old), and 10 older (59 to 73). Equal numbers of men and women were in each age group. This experiment examined understanding of hazard warnings, understanding of location cues, and their combined understanding. Parts 1 and 2 were conducted while driving a 1991 Ford Taurus station wagon on a fixed route through a residential neighborhood. In part 3, subject imagined they were driving through an intersection. (The car was actually parked.) Simulated IVSAWS displays were printed on 4x6 inch cards and mounted near the middle of the instrument panel. In Part 1, drivers were shown the 10 hazard warnings in Figure 4 and drivers said what each meant. At least two warnings were examined in each of the 3 categories (in-vehicle signs, atypical vehicle, emergency vehicle) Hazard warnings were either text, graphics, or mixed. In Part 2, they were shown the moving police car warning and a location cue, and said what they jointly meant. Figure 5 shows the location cues for ahead. Six locations were actually examined (ahead, ahead to the left, ahead to the right, to the left, to the right, and behind). In Part 3, drivers were shown 40 hazard type-location cue pairs constructed by combining the 10 hazards with 4 of the 6 directional cues. (See Figure 6.) Drivers identified the hazard and pointed to its location. It was believed that informing the driver of the hazard and its location (rather than having drivers search for it) would improve warning system effectiveness. The experiment ended (in the laboratory) with subjects ranking the location cues from best to worst. Graphics Graphics Graphics Text Text moving ambulance stopped police car moving police (with flashers) ACCIDENT SLOW DOWN ROAD CONSTRUC- TION Text Mixed Mixed Mixed Mixed TRAIN AT CROSSING R R train at crossing STOP OUT OF ORDER Figure 4 IVSAWS warnings examined in the field experiment. STOP! school bus un/loading-stop

AT 12 O'CLOCK O'clock Text Windshield Grid Overview Plot Eye Perspective Arrow Hand Figure 5 Warning location cues. STOP! TO LEFT Figure 6 Example IVSAWS message. The graphics for stationary hazards (new stop sign ahead, out of order traffic signal ahead, road construction, and accident ahead) were most clearly understood, perhaps because of drivers experience with signs. When vehicles were depicted (stopped police car, moving police car, moving ambulance, and school bus un/loading), drivers were able to identify the type of vehicle shown but sometimes missed the motion cues (stopped versus moving vehicle). Of the two train at crossing hazard symbols, the text message was most widely understood. Overall, the text-based location cues (o'clock and text) were most clearly understood, followed by the overview, inside-out, and arrow formats. (See Table 5.) The text design had the most number one rankings (seven), and the highest overall mean rank (3.3). However, Kruskal-Wallis analysis indicated that differences in design preferences were not statistically significant. For the overview designs (plot, grid, overview), the lines representing quadrants around the vehicle were sometimes interpreted too literally, leading drivers to believe the lines represented an intersection. For the arrow designs (perspective and regular arrow), some subjects interpreted the arrow as a command to move in that direction, not as an indication of hazard location. Some subjects thought the arrow was a vector representing vehicle speed and/or direction. Table 5 Location cue understandability & preferences. Design Format Percent Correct Mean Preference Rank (1=best, 10=worst) Text Text 100 3.3 O'clock Text 94 6.5 Plot Overview 93 6.3 Overview Overview 84 4.8 Grid Overview 83 5.6 Windshield Inside-out 78 7.0 Hand Arrow 75 4.8 Eye Arrow 68 7.9 Perspective Arrow 66 5.6 Arrow Arrow 59 3.5 5. CONCLUSIONS This paper describes three experiments concerning the development and preliminary evaluation of warnings for road hazards. In the first experiment, ideas for warning were obtained from a small sample of drivers by having them draw suggestions. These ideas proved helpful in developing candidate warnings and the experiment was easy to conduct. In the second experiment, candidate warnings developed from subject drawings were ranked from best to worst, allowing the set of alternatives to be reduced. This experiment proved to be quick and easy to conduct. The preferred warning format (text or graphics) varied from warning to warning. Also, warnings based on existing standard highway signs were not always preferred.

The third experiment, conducted in a vehicle (sometimes moving, sometimes stationary) using cards to represent warnings, provided a low-cost method for evaluating warning alternatives, while still providing a real-world context. In general, text (ahead, to the left, etc.) was the cue preferred by drivers to indicate the location of a hazard and led to the highest understandability. The purpose of this study was to demonstrate methods for developing and evaluating warnings in vehicles, and to provide a reasonable set of alternatives for further evaluation. (See Green, Williams, Hoekstra, George, and Wen, 1993 for a subsequent on-road experiment in which eye fixation data was collected.) This effort was not intended to develop a set of warnings ready for incorporation in a national or international standard, though it does provide an excellent beginning. Should standards be desired, additional development and testing should be carried out. 6. REFERENCES Erlichman, J. (1992). A Pilot Study of the In- Vehicle Safety Advisory and Warning System (IVSAWS) Driver-Alert Warning System Design, Proceedings of the Human Factors Society 36th Annual Meeting, Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors Society, pp. 480-484. Green, P. (1993a). Human Factors of In-Vehicle Driver Information Systems: An Executive Summary (Technical Report UMTRI-93-18), Ann Arbor, MI: The University of Michigan Transportation Research Institute. Green, P. (1993b). Procedures and Acceptance Limits for Assessing the Safety and Ease of Use of Driver Information Systems (Technical Report UMTRI-93-13), Ann Arbor, MI: The University of Michigan Transportation Research Institute. Green, P., Serafin, C., Williams, M., and Paelke, G. (1991). What Functions and Features Should Be in Driver Information Systems of the Year 2000? (SAE paper 912792), Vehicle Navigation and Information Systems Conference (VNIS'91), Warrendale, PA: Society of Automotive Engineers, pp. 483-498. Green, P., Levison, W., Paelke, G., and Serafin, C. (1993). Suggested Human Factors Guidelines for Driver Information Systems (Technical Report UMTRI-93-21), Ann Arbor, MI: The University of Michigan Transportation Research Institute. Green, P., Williams, M., Hoekstra, E., George, K., and Wen, C. (1993). Initial On-the-Road Tests of Driver Information System Interfaces: Examination of Navigation, Traffic Information, IVSAWS, and Vehicle Monitoring (Technical report UMTRI-93-32), Ann Arbor, MI: The University of Michigan Transportation Research Institute. Hoekstra, E., Williams, M., and Green, P. (1993). Development and Driver Understanding of Hazard Warning and Location Symbols for IVSAWS (Technical Report UMTRI-93-16), Ann Arbor, MI: The University of Michigan Transportation Research Institute. Levison, W.H. (1993). A Simulation Model for the Driver's Use of In-Vehicle Information Systems (TRB Paper 930935), paper presented at the Transportation Research Board Annual Meeting, Washington, DC., January 10-14, 1993. Levison, W.H., and Cramer, N.L. (1993). Description of the Integrated Driver Model (BBN Technical Report 7840), Cambridge, MA: Bolt Beranek and Newman. Meyer, J., Reaser, J., Keller, R., Wilson, R., and Vadeboncoeur, J. (1982). Feasibility and Concept Selection of a Safety Hazard Advance Warning System (SHAWS)--Volume II--Technical Report, (Technical Report FHWA/RD-81/124), Washington, DC: US Department of Transportation, Federal Highway Administration, National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. Peterson, D., and Boyer, D. (1975). Feasibility Study of In-Vehicle Warning Systems, (Technical Report DOT-HS-801-569), Washington, DC: US Department of Transportation. Serafin, C., Williams, M., Paelke, G., and Green, P. (1991). Functions and Features of Future Driver Information Systems (Technical Report UMTRI-91-16), Ann Arbor, MI: The University of Michigan Transportation Research Institute. Streff, F., Ervin, R., and Blower D. (1991). In- Vehicle Safety Advisory and Warning System (IVSAWS)--Task B--Final Report (Technical Report UMTRI-91-33), Ann Arbor, MI: The University of Michigan Transportation Research Institute. 7. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The author would like to thank Marie Williams and Eileen Hoekstra for assistance in conducting this research.