Latency time and absence of group effect: two examples

Similar documents
Self-Organization in Social Insects

Modelling the Role of Trail Pheromone in the Collective Construction of Termite Royal Chambers

The Phase-Ordering Kinetics of Cemetery Organization in Ants

Decrease in Number of Piles. Lecture 10. Why does the number of piles decrease? More Termites. An Experiment to Make the Number Decrease More Quickly

Introduction to Swarm Robotics

Outline. 1 Initial Definitions. 2 Examples of SI in Biology. 3 Self-Organization. 4 Stigmergy. 5 Swarm Robotics

Pheromones by Ellen Miller November 2015

Apis mellifera scuttelata. Common names: African honeybee also nicknamed the killer bee

Optimal Traffic Organisation in Ants under Crowded Conditions.

Nest excavation in ants: group size effects on the size and structure of tunneling networks

Practical 5 SOCIAL BEHAVIOUR OF HONEY BEES

VI" Autonomous Agents" &" Self-Organization! Part A" Nest Building! Autonomous Agent! Nest Building by Termites" (Natural and Artificial)!

Model of Droplet Dynamics in the Argentine Ant Linepithema Humile (Mayr)

Honey Bee Biology Workshop. The Queen. The Drone

Emergent Teamwork. Craig Reynolds. Cognitive Animation Workshop June 4-5, 2008 Yosemite

SC741 W12: Division of Labor Part I: Fixed- and Variable- Threshold Algorithms

Available online at ScienceDirect. Procedia Computer Science 20 (2013 ) 90 95

Biology of the Colony. Dr. Deborah Delaney

28 3 Insects Slide 1 of 44

Rearing Honeybee Queens in, Apis Mellifera L. Colonies During the Activity Season of Oriental Wasps Vespa Orientalis L

Excavation and aggregation as organizing factors in de novo construction by mound-building termites

Bee Behavior. Summary of an article by. Stephen Taber III from Beekeeping in the United States

Chemical signals: What is one thing in common between a colony & a human society?

Eusocial species. Eusociality. Phylogeny showing only eusociality Eusocial insects. Eusociality: Cooperation to the extreme

Supplementary Information

Rapid morphological changes in populations of hybrids between Africanized and European honey bees

Swarm-bots and Swarmanoid: Two experiments in embodied swarm intelligence

The Topological Fortress of Termites

Heat-balling wasps by honeybees

Models of Termite Nest Construction Daniel Ladley Computer Science 2003/2004

Name: Date Block A Trail of Termites

Templates. Template is a pattern used to construct another pattern Used in conjunction with sorting behaviour:

Intracolonial nepotism during colony fissioning in honey bees?

Self-organizing nest construction in ants: individual worker behaviour and the nest s dynamics


When do honey bee guards reject their former nestmates after swarming?

NATURE S. Insect Pollinators, Plants, and. The pedigree of honey Does not concern the bee; A clover, anytime, to her Is aristocracy.

Many biological systems display large-scale features involving

There are approximately 25,000 species of Bee in the World There are almost 4000 species of Bee in North America There are approximately 1000

Comparative Rates of Recruitment to Pollen Sources by Africanized and European Honey Bees (Apis Mellifera L.)

Honey Bees QUB Green Champions 9 th April

Behavioral Syndromes in Individual Honeybees

Mathematical modeling of complex systems Part 1. Overview

Honey Bees. QUB CCRCB 11 th January

Honeybees: Foraging Behavior, Reproductive Biology And Diseases (Insects And Other Terrestrial Arthropods: Biology, Chemistry And Behavior)

Abstract. Introduction

Swarming Biology of Honey Bees

Complexity of Environment and Parsimony of Decision Rules in Insect Societies

Bee Colony Activities Throughout The Year

In-hive pollen transfer between bees enhances cross-pollination of plants

Learning about bees - Maths Questions

Chapter 14 The Evolution of Social Behavior (1 st lecture)

Responses of queenright and queenless workers of Apis cerana to 9-keto-2(E)-decenoic acid, a pheromonal constituent of the mandibular gland

DISTRIBUTION OF VARIOUS CASTES IN DIFFERENT PARTS OF THE MOUND OF THE TERMITES ODONTOTERMES OBESUS RAMBUR (ISOPTERA-TERMITIDAE)

Glossary of Terms used in Beekeeping

Exploited superorganisms how life history shapes the reproductive strategies of honeybees

Inverse Brascamp-Lieb inequalities along the Heat equation

Peter Dutton. 28 August Halesworth& District

Swarm Intelligence W13: From Aggregation and Segregation to Structure Building

The University of Birmingham School of Computer Science MSc in Advanced Computer Science. Behvaiour of Complex Systems. Termite Mound Simulator

Social Insects. Social Insects. Subsocial. Social Insects 4/9/15. Insect Ecology

Social Insects. Insect Ecology

depending only on local relations. All cells use the same updating rule, Time advances in discrete steps. All cells update synchronously.

Moving without a purpose: an experimental study of swarm guidance in the Western honey bee, Apis mellifera

Research Article Nest Digging by Leaf-Cutting Ants: Effect of Group Size and Functional Structures

Polyphenic Insects. genotype X environment = phenotype POLYPHENISM. genetic polymorphism vs polyphenism. the peppered moth.

BASIC BEE BIOLOGY. Tammy Horn, KY State Apiarist

How does starvation affect spatial organization within nests in Lasius niger?

Observation system for the control of the hive environment by the honeybee (Apis mellifera)

Constants: Acceleration due to gravity = 9.81 m/s 2

Constants: Acceleration due to gravity = 9.81 m/s 2

Comb construction in mixed-species colonies of honeybees, Apis cerana and Apis mellifera

The use of conspecific and interspecific scent marks by foraging bumblebees and honeybees

Three-dimensional architectures grown by simple stigmergic agents

Drywood Termite Identification Guide

Activity: Honey Bee Adaptation Grade Level: Major Emphasis: Major Curriculum Area: Related Curriculum Areas: Program Indicator: Student Outcomes:

Lesson Plan: Vectors and Venn Diagrams

Collective Decision-Making in Honey Bee Foraging Dynamics

An ant colony algorithm for multiple sequence alignment in bioinformatics

Searching for the egg-marking signal in honeybees

Swarm Intelligence Systems

SOCIAL ANIMALS. -Spectacular -Photographed -Studied -Appreciated. The PINNACLE of evolution???

SOBA Bee School April, 2015

CORRELATIVE ANALYSES OF BROOD RATIO AND ROYAL JELLY PRODUCTION

Bee Communica,on A Hive Mind

Comparative study of the hygienic behavior of Carniolan and Africanized honey bees directed towards grouped versus isolated dead brood cells

Effects of Dietary Protein Levels on Royal Jelly Production and Hypopharyngeal Gland Development of ZND No. 1 Italian Honeybee

Nonrandom Visitation of Brood Cells by Worker Honey Bees (Hymenoptera: Apidae)

Honey Bee Species. Apis mellifera (the European or Western honey bee) Michael Birt

Division of Labour and Task Allocation

Objectives. Bee Basics. Apis mellifera. Honey bees 3/13/2019. All about bees Pheromones in the hive Obtaining bees Foraging and nutrition Bee diseases

Discrete dragline attachment induces aggregation in spiderlings of a solitary species

Bee Basics. Objectives. Honey bees 3/15/2018. All about bees Pheromones in the hive Obtaining bees Foraging and nutrition Protecting pollinators

Dept. of Chemistry, Biotechnology and Food Science, Aas Norway

Bees Are Cool. Bee First Aid

THE EVALUATION OF HONEYBEE COLONIES, USING A DATABASE WITH APPLICATION IN HONEYBEE BREEDING PROGRAMS.

SYSTEMATICS, MORPHOLOGY AND PHYSIOLOGY

4 Questions relating to Behavior

Accretion of Phobos and Deimos in an extended debris disc stirred by transient moons

Bees: The most important pollinators

Transcription:

Latency time and absence of group effect: two examples Jean-Louis Deneubourg 1, Eric Bonabeau 2, Guy Theraulaz 3 (1) Unit of Theoretical Behavioral Ecology, Service de Chimie-Physique, CP 231, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Boulevard du triomphe, 1050 Brussels, Belgium (2) Santa Fe Institute, 1399 Hyde Park Road, Santa Fe, NM 87501, USA (3) Laboratoire d'ethologie et de Psychologie Animale, CNRS - UMR 5550, Université Paul Sabatier, 118 route de Narbonne, 31062 Toulouse Cédex, France Abstract We suggest that group effect need not be invoked to explain the differences in latency times exhibited by groups of different sizes in the initiation of building in the termite Macrotermes subhyalinus (Rambur), and in the initiation of aggressive behavior in various subspecies of the honey bee Apis mellifera. A simple model is presented, that is fully consistent with experimental data. 1

In a remarkable series of laboratory experiments Bruisnsma (1979) studied the building behavior of the fungus-growing termite Macrotermes subhyalinus, in particular the construction of the royal chamber in the presence of a live physogastric queen. Among his observations is the fact that the time it takes for a group of termites to initiate the building of the royal chamber (which, following Bruinsma, we shall call building "latency time") is a nonlinearly decreasing function of group size. Such an observation could be interpreted as resulting from a group effect (Grassé, 1946), or socially facilitated behavior (Clayton, 1978). However, a simple model, fully consistent with the data of Bruinsma (1979), can very satisfactorily account for this observation. The behavior of individual workers during building seems to be made up of relatively few and relatively simple activities: a worker picks up a piece of soil close to or underneath the queen, transports the pellet to a site of deposition (a narrow band around the queen at 2-5 cm from her and determined by a pheromonal template that she emits), where it deposits and cement it. This behavior, combined with an attraction towards sites where pellets have already been deposited, leads to the formation of pillars or columns a few centimeters away from the queen. Pillars are lengthened until they reach a height of between 0.5-0.8 cm; workers then start to build horizontal lamellae that grow from the pillars to form a roof over the queen; finally, the interpillar space is filled with pellets to form walls. The building latency time, that is, more precisely, the time elapsed between the intoduction of workers in the arena with the queen and the first observed grasping of a soil pellet within 0.5 cm of the queen, is the focus of the present note. Fig. 1 shows the decrease of latency time with group size, for varnished and unvarnished workers. Varnished workers were unable to lay trail. It can be seen in Fig. 1 that there is no significant difference between varnished and unvarnished workers with respect to the latency time (whereas subsequent stages of the building process are affected by the inability of varnished workers to lay trail: for example, such workers were unable to start pillars): this indicates that trail is not involved in the determination of the latency time. It could be that increased direct communications among termites when group size (and hence density) increases accelerate the process by stimulating workers to start building. More generally, a group effect, or socially facilitated behavior, could be invoked: individuals may modulate their behavior in response to the presence of other group members through omnipresent signals or cues (by omnipresent signals, we mean signals that do not result from specific events in space or time (Grassé, 1946; Wilson, 1971)). Let 2

us consider the following alternative answer, that relies on the idea that the decrease in latency time is simply a numerical effect: let us assume that each individual has a fixed probability p, independent of group size, and uncorrelated to the behavior of other workers, of picking up a pellet per unit time. The probability that no worker among N workers picks up a pellet within a time unit is then given by 1 p ( ) N. The probability P(t) that the latency time be equal to t time units is the probability that no worker picked up a pellet during the first t-1 time units and that at least one worker picked up a pellet within the t th time unit. Therefore, assuming that the process is not correlated in time (that is, the fact that an individual does or does not pick up a pellet within a given unit of time does not depend on its previous actions), we obtain P t ( ) 1 1 p. The average latency time under this hypothesis is given by t = t=1 ( ) [ N t 1 ( ) ] N ( ) = 1 p tp t [ ( ) ] N ( ) = 1 1 p 1. (1) In order to evaluate the performance of the fromula given in equation (1), we can rewrite equation (1) as log( 1 t 1 ) = Nlog( 1 p). (2) We can now test for a linear relationship between log 1 t 1 log 1 t 1 ( ) and N. Fig. 2 shows ( ) as a function of N for 37 experiments with various values of N, where t is latency time observed in one given experiment: it can be fitted by a straight line (p=0.001 min -1, r=0.786, df=35, P<0.001). A group effect would lead to a dependence of p on N: our result suggests that a group effect, if it exists, is not significant, although it would be useful to conduct a new series of experiments, using the same procedures, but with a greater number of replicates. Another example, where the decrease in latency time can be explained as a numerical effect resulting from the increase in group size, can be found in studies of colony defence or aggressiveness in bees. There are many such studies, and we give just a few examples of tests made with various subspecies of Apis mellifera. Collins and Rothenbuler (1978) performed laboratory experiments to measure, for different group sizes, the time before the first reaction to a particular chemical, isopentyl 3

acetate, or IPA, the major component of the sting alarm pheromone (Boch et al., 1962). Let N be the number of bees in a cage, and t the average time to first reaction. Fig. 3 shows log 1 t 1 ( ) as a function of N: that it can be well fitted by a straight line (r=0.951, df=2, P<0.025) suggests that the decrease in t when group size increases is likely to result from a numerical effect, where there is a fixed probability p (given by equation (2)) per individual to respond to the pheromone within a time unit. Here, it appears that p=0.0044 s -1. Collins et al. (1988) measured the number of seconds, denoted by t, until the first bee was seen on two blue suede sting targets mechanically waved in front of the entrance, and simultaneously measured the number N of guards at the entrance. It is found once again that a linear relationship between ( ) and N, where t is the average t over experiments, gives a reasonable log 1 t 1 fit, although three experimental points are certainly not sufficient to reach a significant conclusion. However, each of these experimental points has been obtained for a different subspecies of Apis mellifera (one Africanized A. m. scutellata, one European and one hybrid subspecies), suggesting that N (the number of guards), but not p (the probability of responding per individual per unit time), is characteristic of the subspecies. Using this remark (which is interesting as it indicates that the intrinsic aggressiveness, measured by p, of Africanized honey bees is not different from that of European honey bees), combined with Collins et al.'s (1988) observation that the number of stings in a target after 60 s is roughly proportional to the number of individuals guarding the nest entrance, we can also interpret data from Stort (1974). Stort (1974) studied the time before the first sting when a small leather ball is kept in motion just outside the hive entrance of colonies of Apis mellifera ligustica (European subspecies) and Apis mellifera adansonii (Africanized subspecies). Although he did not simultaneously measure the number of guards, so that a direct relationship between t and N is not directly available, he did measure the number of stings in the leather ball after 60 s. Assuming that this quantity is a linear function of the number of guards, we can test the hypothesis that the first reaction time decreases with increasing group size because of a numerical effect: let S be the number of stings in the target after 60 s, and let us assume that S=kN. It is ( ) and S will imply a linear ( ) and N. Fig. 4 shows log( 1 t 1 ) as a function of S, obvious that a linear relationship between log 1 t 1 relationship between log 1 t 1 where the experimental points corresponding to the two subspecies have been mixed. This curve can be fitted by a straight line (r=0.780, df=12, P<0.001), suggesting that the numerical effect can explain most of the observed decrease in t. In 4

addition, the fact that the same straight line can well approximate the data from both subspecies is consistent with our hypothesis that both subspecies can be characterized by the same p. Finally, as was the case with termites, more experiments would be welcome to confirm these results on honey bees. In conclusion, we have presented evidence that the decrease of building latency time in Macrotermes subhyalinus and of time of first reaction to a perturbation in various subspecies of Apis mellifera can be explained by a simple numerical effect. This result is interesting in that it indicates that a strongly nonlinear dependence of some feature (here the building latency time or the time of first sting) on group size may not result from a group effect, involving communication among group members or more generally a modulation of behavior in response to the presence of other group members, but can simply reflect a numerical effect. Although the possibility of group effects or socially facilitated behavior cannot be rejected in many cases, we believe that some other cases where group effects have been invoked, especially to explain the decrease of the time before a first event occurs as a function of group size (latency time), could be reevaluated along the same lines. 5

References Boch, R., D. A. Shearer and B. C. Stone, 1962. Identification of iso-amyl acetate as an active component in the sting pheromone of the honey bee. Nature 195: 1018-1020. Bruinsma, O. H., 1979. An analysis of building behaviour of the termite Macrotermes subhyalinus (Rambur). PhD Thesis, Landbouwhogeschool, Wageningen. Clayton, D. A., 1978. Socially facilitated behavior. Quart. Rev. Biol. 53: 373-392. Collins, A. M. and W. C. Rothenbuler, 1978. Laboratory test of the response to an alarm chemical, isopentyl acetate, by Apis mellifera. Ann. Ent. Soc. Am. 71: 906-909. Collins, A. M., T. E. Rinderer and K. W. Tucker, 1988. Colony defence of two honeybee types and their hybrid. 1. Naturally mated queens. J. Apic. Res. 27: 37-40. Grassé, P.-P., 1946. Sociétés animales et effet de groupe. Experientia 2: 77-82. Stort, A. C., 1974. Genetic study of aggressiveness of two subspecies of Apis mellifera in Brazil. 1. Some tests to measure aggressiveness. J. Apic. Res. 13: 33-38. Wilson, E. O., 1971. The Insect Societies. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. 6

Figure captions Fig. 1: Latency time as a function of group size found by Bruinsma (1979; p 39) for varnished and unvarnished workers of Macrotermes subhyalinus. Fig. 2: log( 1 t 1 ) as a function of the number of workers, where t is the time it takes before a worker of Macrotermes subhyalinus first picks up a pellet. Best linear fit is shown. Fig. 3: log( 1 t 1 ) as a function of the number of individuals of Apis mellifera guarding the nest entrance, where t is the average time (average taken over 75 experiments for all points; error bars not shown) to first reaction to isopentyl acetate (IPA) diluted in paraffin oil (1:10 in volume). Best linear fit is shown. Fig. 4: log( 1 t 1 ) as a function of the number of stings after 60 s in a leather ball kept in motion just outside the nest entrance, where t is the average time (average taken over 5 experiments for each point; error bars not shown) until the first sting in the ball. Best linear fit is shown. Data corresponding to two subspecies of Apis mellifera have been mixed. 7