Spike Count Correlation Increases with Length of Time Interval in the Presence of Trial-to-Trial Variation

Similar documents
Trial-to-Trial Variability and its. on Time-Varying Dependence Between Two Neurons

Disambiguating Different Covariation Types

Statistical Smoothing of Neuronal Data p.1

A Rate and History-Preserving Resampling Algorithm for Neural Spike Trains

!) + log(t) # n i. The last two terms on the right hand side (RHS) are clearly independent of θ and can be

The homogeneous Poisson process

ASSESSMENT OF SYNCHRONY IN MULTIPLE NEURAL SPIKE TRAINS USING LOGLINEAR POINT PROCESS MODELS 1

Statistical Modeling of Temporal Evolution in Neuronal Activity. Rob Kass

A Framework for Evaluating Pairwise and Multiway Synchrony Among Stimulus-Driven Neurons

Dynamical Constraints on Computing with Spike Timing in the Cortex

Assessment of synchrony in multiple neural spike trains using loglinear point process models

Model neurons!!poisson neurons!

Extracting Dynamical Structure Embedded in Neural Activity

SPATIOTEMPORAL ANALYSIS OF SYNCHRONIZATION OF NEURAL ENSEMBLES FOR SPATIAL DISCRIMINATIONS IN CAT STRIATE CORTEX

Finding a Basis for the Neural State

Consider the following spike trains from two different neurons N1 and N2:

Lateral organization & computation

Correlations Without Synchrony

Collective Dynamics in Human and Monkey Sensorimotor Cortex: Predicting Single Neuron Spikes

What do V1 neurons like about natural signals

The Bayesian Brain. Robert Jacobs Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences University of Rochester. May 11, 2017

Improvements to the Sensitivity of Gravitational Clustering for Multiple Neuron Recordings

Correlations and neural information coding Shlens et al. 09

The Effect of Correlated Variability on the Accuracy of a Population Code

Introduction to neural spike train data for phase-amplitude analysis

Neural Encoding: Firing Rates and Spike Statistics

Comparison of single spike train descriptive models. by information geometric measure

Chapter 3 Analysis and Interpretation of Interval and Count Variability in Neural Spike Trains

Probabilistic Inference of Hand Motion from Neural Activity in Motor Cortex

Decision-making and Weber s law: a neurophysiological model

3 Neural Decoding. 3.1 Encoding and Decoding. (r 1, r 2,..., r N ) for N neurons is a list of spike-count firing rates, although,

A Monte Carlo Sequential Estimation for Point Process Optimum Filtering

On similarity measures for spike trains

Adaptive contrast gain control and information maximization $

Mid Year Project Report: Statistical models of visual neurons

Mathematical Tools for Neuroscience (NEU 314) Princeton University, Spring 2016 Jonathan Pillow. Homework 8: Logistic Regression & Information Theory

3.3 Population Decoding

Probabilistic Modeling of Dependencies Among Visual Short-Term Memory Representations

Statistical modeling of spiking activity in large scale neuronal networks

False discovery rate regression: an application to neural synchrony detection in primary visual cortex

Phase synchronization between LFP and spiking activity in motor cortex during movement preparation

+ + ( + ) = Linear recurrent networks. Simpler, much more amenable to analytic treatment E.g. by choosing

Diego A. Gutnisky and Kresimir Josic J Neurophysiol 103: , First published Dec 23, 2009; doi: /jn

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

Automatic Learning of Synchrony in Neuronal Electrode Recordings

How Behavioral Constraints May Determine Optimal Sensory Representations

Nonlinear reverse-correlation with synthesized naturalistic noise

When is an Integrate-and-fire Neuron like a Poisson Neuron?

Comparing integrate-and-fire models estimated using intracellular and extracellular data 1

Analyzing Neuroscience Signals using Information Theory and Complexity

Synchrony in Neural Systems: a very brief, biased, basic view

Measuring spike train reliability

Synfire Waves in Small Balanced Networks

Neural Decoding. Chapter Encoding and Decoding

Bayesian Estimation of Stimulus Responses in Poisson Spike Trains

When does interval coding occur?

Neural Coding: Integrate-and-Fire Models of Single and Multi-Neuron Responses

AT2 Neuromodeling: Problem set #3 SPIKE TRAINS

Dependence of Neuronal Correlations on Filter Characteristics and Marginal Spike Train Statistics

Describing Spike-Trains

Power-Law Neuronal Fluctuations in a Recurrent Network Model of Parametric Working Memory

CHARACTERIZATION OF NONLINEAR NEURON RESPONSES

Motor Cortical Decoding Using an Autoregressive Moving Average Model

Extending a biologically inspired model of choice: multialternatives, nonlinearity and value-based multidimensional choice

SPIKE TRIGGERED APPROACHES. Odelia Schwartz Computational Neuroscience Course 2017

Neural information often passes through many different

Hierarchy. Will Penny. 24th March Hierarchy. Will Penny. Linear Models. Convergence. Nonlinear Models. References

Neuroscience Introduction

Maximum Likelihood Estimation of a Stochastic Integrate-and-Fire Neural Model

Exercises. Chapter 1. of τ approx that produces the most accurate estimate for this firing pattern.

Spatiotemporal Structure in Large Neuronal Networks Detected from Cross-Correlation

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn.2283

Training and spontaneous reinforcement of neuronal assemblies by spike timing

State-Space Analysis of Time-Varying Higher-Order Spike Correlation for Multiple Neural Spike Train Data

Emergence of resonances in neural systems: the interplay between adaptive threshold and short-term synaptic plasticity

Activity Driven Adaptive Stochastic. Resonance. Gregor Wenning and Klaus Obermayer. Technical University of Berlin.

The Power Ratio and the Interval Map: Spiking Models and Extracellular Recordings

EEG- Signal Processing

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

CORRELATION TRANSFER FROM BASAL GANGLIA TO THALAMUS IN PARKINSON S DISEASE. by Pamela Reitsma. B.S., University of Maine, 2007

An algorithm for detecting oscillatory behavior in discretized data: the damped-oscillator oscillator detector

Supporting Online Material for

The Spike Response Model: A Framework to Predict Neuronal Spike Trains

Semi-Supervised Classification for Intracortical Brain-Computer Interfaces

Relation between Single Neuron and Population Spiking Statistics and Effects on Network Activity

Establishing a Statistical Link Between Network Oscillations and Neural Synchrony

Time-rescaling methods for the estimation and assessment of non-poisson neural encoding models

A Learning Theory for Reward-Modulated Spike-Timing-Dependent Plasticity with Application to Biofeedback

Bayesian Computation in Recurrent Neural Circuits

Partitioning neuronal variability

The receptive fields of neurons are dynamic; that is, their

Encoding or decoding

Natural Image Statistics

Neural Spike Train Analysis 1: Introduction to Point Processes

Statistical models for neural encoding, decoding, information estimation, and optimal on-line stimulus design

Synaptic input statistics tune the variability and reproducibility of neuronal responses

Power Spectrum Analysis of Bursting Cells in Area MT in the Behaving Monkey

A Multivariate Time-Frequency Based Phase Synchrony Measure for Quantifying Functional Connectivity in the Brain

Statistical models for neural encoding

The idiosyncratic nature of confidence

Transcription:

NOTE Communicated by Jonathan Victor Spike Count Correlation Increases with Length of Time Interval in the Presence of Trial-to-Trial Variation Robert E. Kass kass@stat.cmu.edu Valérie Ventura vventura@stat.cmu.edu Department of Statistics and Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, U.S.A. It has been observed that spike count correlation between two simultaneously recorded neurons often increases with the length of time interval examined. Under simple assumptions that are roughly consistent with much experimental data, we show that this phenomenon may be explained as being due to excess trial-to-trial variation. The resulting formula for the correlation is able to predict the observed correlation of two neurons recorded from primary visual cortex as a function of interval length. 1 Introduction Simultaneously recorded cortical neurons often exhibit correlations in spike counts over substantial periods of time, and this has been interpreted as producing important limitations on capacities for neural coding (Shadlen & Newsome, 1998; Zohary, Shadlen, & Newsome, 1994). However, it has also been observed that spike count correlations decrease as the length of the time interval decreases (Averbeck & Lee, 2003; Reich, Mechler, & Victor, 2001). We provide here a simple theoretical explanation of this phenomenon as a necessary consequence of excess trial-to-trial variation. The purposes of this work are two: first, to emphasize the importance of specifying interval length when interpreting spike count correlation, and, second, to focus further attention on excess trial-to-trial variation as an indicator of common neuronal input. 2 Results Our object is to analyze the correlation of a pair of spike counts across repeated trials in the presence of excess trial-to-trial variability, under reasonable assumptions, when the measurement interval and therefore each expected count increases. Let us consider random variables Yr 1 and Yr 2 representing theoretical spike counts over an interval of length T for two Neural Computation 18, 2583 2591 (2006) C 2006 Massachusetts Institute of Technology

2584 R. Kass and V. Ventura neurons recorded simultaneously on trial r. To simplify some formulas, we assume the two spike count probability distributions are the same, but this does not affect the essence of the result. We will also assume the following: 1. Within trials, the expected spike counts increase proportionally to T. 2. The within-trial variance is proportional to the within-trial expectation. 3. After conditioning on the trial, Y 1 r and Y2 r are independent. Assumptions 1 and 2 are roughly consistent with many observed data (see Shadlen & Newsome, 1998, for references). Assumption 1 concerns the within-trial expected spike counts. In the absence of excess trial-to-trial variation, the within-trial expected spike count would equal the trial-averaged spike count. When there is excess trial-to-trial variation, the neuronal response depends on some external or internal state S r that varies with the trial. The within-trial expected spike count is the number that would be produced by, hypothetically, averaging spike counts over trials with identical values of the state S r. Assumption 2 is much more general than the Poisson assumption, which would require the within-trial variance to equal the within-trial expectation. Assumption 3 eliminates short timescale effects and will be discussed below. We also introduce a random variable X r to represent excess trial-to-trial variation and will take the expectation of each spike count on trial r to be f (X r ) when T = 1, for some function f (x) (see equation 2.1 below). In the absence of excess trial-to-trial variation, f (X r ) would be constant across trials. 2.1 Monotonically Increasing Correlation. Under these three assumptions, the correlation of Yr 1 and Yr 2 and the conditional expectation and variance of Yr i given the trial may be computed in terms of T. Before proceeding, we make two observations. First, under assumption 1, we may write the expectation conditionally on the trial effect X r in the form E(Y i r X r) = Tf(X r ), (2.1) where f (X r ) becomes the expected spike count when T = 1. Second, under assumption 2, we may write V(Y i r X r) = k E(Y i r X r), and combining this with equation 2.1, we have V(Y i r X r) = ktf (X r ). (2.2)

Trial-to-Trial Variation Can Explain Spike Count Variation 2585 In the case of Poisson counts, we would have k = 1. Underdispersion occurs when k < 1 and overdispersion when k > 1. In computing the correlation of Yr 1 and Y2 r we will use equations 2.1 and 2.2 together with elementary formulas for the variances and covariance in terms of the conditional variances, covariance, and expectations. For i = 1, 2, we have V(Yr i ) = E(V(Yi r X r)) + V(E(Yr i X r)) = E(kTf (X r )) + V(Tf(X r )) = kte( f (X r )) + T 2 V( f (X r )), and, applying assumption 3 and equation 2.1, COV ( Yr 1, ) ( ( Y2 r = E COV Y 1 r, Yr 2 X )) ( ( ) ( )) r + COV E Y 1 r X r, E Y 2 r X r = 0 + T 2 V( f (X r )). Writing µ = E( f (X r )) and σ 2 = V( f (X r )), we therefore obtain COR ( Yr 1, ) T 2 σ 2 Y2 r = ktµ + T 2 σ = 2 T T + ω, (2.3) where ω = kµ/σ 2. This shows that the correlation will increase monotonically as T increases and will vanish as T 0. To see the implication of equation 2.3, suppose that the trial-to-trial variation takes the form E ( Y i r X r) = Tf(Xr ) = Tce Xr, where c is the firing rate when T = 1andX r = 0, and, for simplicity, suppose further that X r has a normal distribution. It is easily verified that if X r has mean a and variance b 2, then and E ( e Xr ) = e a+b2 /2 V ( e Xr ) = e 2a+b2 (e b2 1). These give the ratio V ( ) e Xr E (e Xr ) = ea+b2 /2 ( e b2 1 ).

2586 R. Kass and V. Ventura Using this formula, we may compute the correlation in equation 2.3 for various scenarios. For example, with a firing rate of c = e a = 20 spikes per second and b = 12.5% trial-to-trial variation, correlations for counts in intervals of length 2 ms, 100 ms, and 1000 ms become.0006,.03, and.24, which are roughly consistent with those reported by Reich et al. (2001). We do not mean to suggest that trial-to-trial variation may be described well by normally distributed effects that are constant in time (see below). These values are provided, rather, to help interpret the predictions of equation 2.3. 2.2 Nonmonotonic Correlation. According to equation 2.3, the spike count correlation will increase monotonically and, furthermore, will approach 1 for sufficiently long time intervals. However, Averbeck and Lee (2003) report an increase of correlation as a function of T up to a maximum, and then a subsequent decline. We now show that such effects could also be due to excess trial-to-trial variability. Suppose that the excess trial-to-trial variation is as described previously up to T 1, but that it disappears afterward. Such effects have been reported previously (e.g., Baker, Spinks, Jackson, & Lemon, 2001). Under assumptions1,2,and3,whent T 1, equation 2.3 still applies. For T > T 1,we write Yr i = Yi r1 + Yi r2,whereyi r1 is the spike count in [0, T 1], and Yr2 i the spike count in [T 1, T], for neuron i on trial r. Because the spike counts Yr2 1 and Yr2 2 in [T 1, T] contain no excess trial-to-trial variation, they are mutually independent and are also independent from the spike counts Yr1 1 and Y2 r1 in [0, T 1 ]. Then, for T > T 1,wehave V ( Yr i ) ( ) ( ) ( = V Y i r1 + V Y i r2 = kt1 µ + T1 2 σ 2) + k(t T 1 ) and COV ( Yr 1, ) ( Y2 r = COV Y 1 r1, Yr1 2 ) = T 2 1 σ 2, so that, for T > T 1, COR ( Yr 1, Y2 r ) T 1 = T 1 + w + k T 1 (T T σ 2 1 ). (2.4) Under these conditions, the correlation will increase with T for T < T 1 and will decrease with T for T > T 1. The modified assumption that trial-to-trial variability vanishes for T > T 1 is not supposed to reflect accurately a real situation. Rather, we have provided equation 2.4 to indicate possible nonmonotonic behavior. Other, more realistic forms of time-varying trial-to-trial effects could also produce correlations that are nonmonotonic in T. One relatively simple alternative

Trial-to-Trial Variation Can Explain Spike Count Variation 2587 form of time-varying trial-to-trial variation is given, and its predictions are compared with data below. 2.3 Illustration with V1 Data. We illustrate using data from two neurons recorded simultaneously in the primary visual cortex of an anesthetized macaque monkey (Aronov, Reich, Mechler, & Victor, 2003, units 380506.s and 380506.u, 90 degree spatial phase), which were part of the Reich et al. (2001) study. Figures 1B and 1C show their peristimulus time histograms (PSTHs). Ventura, Cai, and Kass (2005) established that these two neurons had excess trial-to-trial variation, whose effects were shared across the neurons (see also Figure 1D), but that the neurons were independent once these effects were removed. Figure 1A displays the correlation of spike counts for increasing time intervals. The data for these two neurons were recorded from the same electrode, with an accuracy of 2.8 milliseconds, so that it was impossible to detect joint spikes occurring at time lags less than 2.8 milliseconds. This induces an artifactual negative correlation, clearly apparent in Figure 1A for the smallest time interval. A fit of equation 2.3 to the data is overlaid in Figure 1A. It captures reasonably well the general trend of the correlation. However, it fails to follow a leveling off evident at intervals greater than about 200 milliseconds, which may be due to nonconstant trial-to-trial variation. Indeed, Ventura et al. (2005) showed that this pair of neurons had highly significant nonconstant trial-to-trial effects and that the firing rate of neuron i on trial r could be described better by the function P i r (t) = Pi (t) e w0r φ(t), (2.5) with P i (t) being the average firing rate of neuron i over many trials, and w 0r φ(t) being a nonconstant contribution shared across the two neurons. According to equation 2.5, the excess trial-to-trial variation is due to trialspecific effects w 0r that modulate the function φ(t). We could not produce analytical results, like those of equation 2.3, for the model of equation 2.5 because it is too complicated. Instead, we have calculated the predicted correlation curve by numerical simulation. We simulated 1000 pairs of spike trains from model 2.5 fitted to the data, which we used to compute the correlation as a function of interval length T. 1 We also adjusted the correlation function for the recording accuracy of 2.8 msec. 2 Figures 1B and 1C 1 Specifically, we sampled with replacement 1000 values w0r from the histogram in Figure 1F and then simulated pairs of Poisson spike trains with rates P i (t)e w 0r φ(t), i = 1, 2, with P i (t)andφ(t) shown in Figures 1B, 1C, and 1E. 2 We identified all the occurrences of simultaneous spikes within 2.8 msec and, for each occurrence, retained the spike of only one neuron, chosen with probability proportional to its firing rate.

2588 R. Kass and V. Ventura A Correlation 0.2 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0 Correlation predicted by: Eq. 2.3 Eq. 2.5 Eq. 2.5 with 2.8 ms recording accuracy 0 50 100 150 200 250 Interval length T (ms) Data and fit of model in Equation 2.5 Firing rate (Hz) 0 204060 B neuron 1 0 200 400 Time (ms) 0 204060 C neuron 2 0 200 400 Time (ms) neuron 2 0 5 10 15 D Total spike count 0 5 10 15 20 neuron 1 φ (t) 0.8 0.2 0.4 E 0 200 400 Time (ms) F 4 2 0 2 4 w0

Trial-to-Trial Variation Can Explain Spike Count Variation 2589 show estimates of P i (t), i = 1, 2, taken to be the smoothed PSTHs, Figure 1E displays a fit of the function φ(t), and Figure 1F the histogram of the fitted trial-specific effect coefficients w 0r. In Figure 1A, the correlation predicted by equation 2.5 appears as the dashed curve, and the correlation after adjustment for the recording accuracy of 2.8 msec appears as the largedashed curve. While equation 2.5 is itself a simplified representation of excess trial-to-trial variation and should not be expected to fit the data perfectly, these curves track the observed correlation quite well. 3 Discussion Trial-to-trial variation is of interest not only for its physiological significance (Azouz & Gray, 1999; Hanes & Schall, 1996), but also because it confounds assessments of correlation (Brody, 1999a, 1999b; Ben-Shaul, Bergman, Ritov, & Abeles, 2001; Grün, Riehle, & Diesmann, 2003). We have shown here that excess trial-to-trial variation produces spike count correlations that vary with the length of time interval during which the counts are recorded. This follows, essentially, from assumptions 1 and 2 by formulas 2.1 and 2.2. When assumption 3 holds, monotonicity holds throughout the interval during which there is excess trial-to-trial variation and, furthermore, the correlation vanishes as T 0. We have also noted that when the excess trial-to-trial variation disappears after time T 1, the spike count correlation will decline after it reaches a maximum at an interval of length T 1. This behavior conforms to observations reported in Averbeck and Lee (2003). When there is correlation in the spike timing so that assumption 3 fails, it would be reasonable to assume, analogous to assumption 2, that the withintrial covariance between the two neurons spike counts is proportional to Figure 1: (A) Correlation between the spike counts for two neurons in primary visual cortex, as a function of interval length T. For each T, we plotted the box plot (quantiles and 10th and 90th quartiles) of the correlations obtained by sliding the interval along experimental time. The solid curve is equation 2.3 with ω = 34. The dashed curve is the correlation function predicted by model 2.5 fitted to the data; the large-dashed curve is also for data as in equation 2.5 but with recording accuracy of 2.8 milliseconds to match the observed data. The activity of the two neurons was recorded during 64 trials from an anesthetized monkey; the stimulus in each trial was a standing sinusoidal grating that appeared at time 0 and disappeared at 237 ms. (B, C) Raw and smoothed PSTHs, P i (t), i = 1, 2, of the two neurons. (D) Within-trial spike counts for the complete interval of observation, which suggests that the neurons have shared effects of trial-to-trial variation. (E) The fitted firing rate modulating function φ(t) and (F) a histogram of the coefficients w 0r.

2590 R. Kass and V. Ventura the within-trial expectation. We write: COV ( Y 1 r, Y2 r X r ) = cte ( Y 1 r X r ). (3.1) In this case, monotonicity holds as long as c < k, and it is easy to show that COR(Y 1, Y 2 ) c/k as T 0. Note that in the absence of trial-to-trial variation, f (X r ) becomes a constant, and under assumption 3, the spike counts are uncorrelated. If assumption 3 fails but equation 3.1 holds, in the absence of trial-to-trial variation, the correlation becomes constant and does not increase with the length of time interval. Under assumptions 1 and 2, an increase in spike count correlation with length of time interval, as in Figure 1A, is an indication of excess trial-to-trial variation that is shared across the two neurons. (For related methods and additional analyses of these data, see Ventura et al., 2005.) We have offered our analysis in the usual spirit of those made with simplifying assumptions. We would not expect excess trial-to-trial variation to be summarized accurately by a single number, here represented as X r. Ventura et al. (2005) have shown how somewhat more complicated phenomena involving trial-to-trial variability may be described. However, we would expect equations 2.3 and 2.4 to capture dominant effects, as illustrated in Figure 1A, and to provide insight into the possible origin of widely observed correlations. References Aronov, D., Reich, D. S., Mechler, F., & Victor, J. D. (2003). Neural coding of spatial phase in V1 of the macaque monkey. J. Neurophysiol., 89, 3304 3327. Averbeck, B. B., & Lee, D. (2003). Neural noise and movement-related codes in the macaque supplementary motor area. J. Neurosci., 23, 7630 7641. Azouz, R., & Gray, C. M. (1999). Cellular mechanisms contributing to response variability of cortical neurons in vivo. J. Neurosci, 19, 2209 2223. Baker, S. N., Spinks, R., Jackson, A., & Lemon, R. N. (2001). Synchronization in monkey motor cortex during a precision grip task. I. Task-dependent modulation in single-unit synchrony. J. Neurophysiol., 85, 869 885. Ben-Shaul, Y., Bergman, H., Ritov, Y., & Abeles, M. (2001). Trial to trial variability in either stimulus or action causes apparent correlation and synchrony in neuronal activity. J. Neurosci. Methods, 111, 99 110. Brody, C. D. (1999a). Correlations without synchrony. Neural Computation, 11, 1537 1551. Brody, C. D. (1999b). Disambiguating different covariation. Neural Computation, 11, 1527 1535. Grün, S., Riehle, A., & Diesmann, M. (2003). Effect of across trial nonstationarity on joint-spike events. Biol. Cybernetics, 88, 335 351. Hanes, D. P., & Schall, J. D. (1996). Neural control of voluntary movement initiation. Science, 274, 427 430.

Trial-to-Trial Variation Can Explain Spike Count Variation 2591 Reich, D. S., Mechler, F., & Victor, J. D. (2001). Independent and redundant information in nearby cortical neurons. Science, 294, 2566 2568. Shadlen, M. N., & Newsome, W. T. (1998). The variable discharge of cortical neurons: Implications for connectivity, computation, and information coding. J. Neurosci., 18(10), 3870 3896. Ventura, V., Cai, C., & Kass, R. E. (2005). Trial-to-trial variability and its effect on timevarying dependence between two neurons. J. Neurophysiology, 94, 2928 2939. Zohary, E., Shadlen, M. N., & Newsome, W. T. (1994). Correlated neuronal discharge rate and its implications for psychophysical performance. Nature, 370, 140 143. Received March 22, 2005; accepted April 14, 2006.