Motor vehicle and pedal cycle conspicuity - part 3: retroflective and fluorescent materials, disability glare of red markings.

Similar documents
(65th GRE, March 2011, agenda item 4(d))

Driving at night. Harris Driving school. Harris Driving School

Safe Transportation Research & Education Center UC Berkeley

Snow Removal Equipment Visibility

Hail Hail can occur unexpectedly; slow down and use dipped headlights. After a hailstorm the road surface will be slippery so drive carefully.

Re-Establishing a Brand

Islamic University of Gaza - Palestine. Department of Industrial Engineering

Predicting the Effects of Discomfort Glare on Visual Performance of Pilots during Nighttime Flying

White Paper Luminance & Illuminance. Brief explanation of photometry for the application of tunnel lighting control

Identify the letter of the choice that best completes the statement or answers the question.

Safety and Performance Benefits Associated with the Use of a Spotter Mirror: Impact on Driver Lane- Change Planning and Execution

Mesopic Photometry for SSL. Teresa Goodman Metrology for SSL Meeting 24 th April 2013

MODELING PERIPHERAL VISIBILITY UNDER HEADLAMP ILLUMINATION

Safe Driving in Bad Weather Conditions

FEDERAL RESOLUTION PROPOSAL INTRODUCTION FORM

The Control of Glare by the AS1680 Systems

A Driving Simulation Based Study on the Effects of. Road Marking Luminance Contrast on Driving Safety. Yong Cao, Jyh-Hone Wang

TECHNICAL NOTE. Specifying Product Performance for Mesopic Applications

Towards a fully LED-based solar simulator - spectral mismatch considerations

Delay Attribution Board. Guidance No. DAB-28

Measuring Momentum: Using distance moved after impact to estimate velocity

L ight color influence on obstacle recognition in road lighting. 1. Introduction

GATEWAY SCIENCE B651/02 PHYSICS B Unit 1 Modules P1 P2 P3 (Higher Tier)

SEVERE WEATHER PLAN FOR SPECIAL EVENTS or VENUES. Plan/Event Information for:

Lab NotesIssue. Disability Glare in the Outdoor Workplace

A Case Study: Fallacies in ANSI/ISEA 107, 2010 High Visibility Safety Clothing Standard

FINAL CONTRACT REPORT WET NIGHT VISIBILITY OF PAVEMENT MARKINGS: EXECUTIVE SUMMARY. Ronald B. Gibbons, Ph.D. Jonathan Hankey, Ph.D.

In the Matter of the Study of State Police Stop Activity at the Southern end of the New Jersey Turnpike

Glare and visibility by headlight for pedestrian and elderly driver. 22 October 2018 National Traffic Safety and Environment Laboratory

Introducing speckle noise maps for laser vibrometry

THE INFLUENCE OF LENS HAZE ON HEADLAMP BEAM PATTERNS

Assessing system reliability through binary decision diagrams using bayesian techniques.

Traffic Progression Models

CHAPTER 3. CAPACITY OF SIGNALIZED INTERSECTIONS

When is sign lighting needed? How much is needed?

Simultaneous lightness contrast with double increments

SUMMARY. ) t, UNIT. Constant velocity represents uniform motion. Acceleration causes a change in velocity.

Velocity Thresholds in Car-Following at Night

A comparison of modelling approaches for the time-limited dispatch (TLD) of aircraft

CIVL /8904 T R A F F I C F L O W T H E O R Y L E C T U R E - 8

Mathematical Methods 2018

Static risk mapping using a Geographic Information System

A Study of Mesopic Adaptation conditions for Night Drivers using an approximation for Oncoming headlight glare

Snow Removal Policy WARREN COUNTY ENGINEER S OFFICE (WCEO) WARREN COUNTY HIGHWAY DEPARTMENT. October 16, 2014

YOUR VEHICLE WINDOWS

Ground vibrations from accelerating and braking road vehicles

Customer Management Report (San Mateo) All Detection Types 01-Dec-2016 to 31-Dec-2016 Operator Id: %

Who needs a spotter? AGENDA Role of a Spotter Circle of Safety Parking Practices

Disability glare: A study in simulated road lighting conditions

University of Moratuwa, Sri Lanka. EE3231: Design of Electrical Installation

Customer Management Report (Laguna Woods) All Detection Types 01-Feb-2013 to 28-Feb-2013 Operator Id: %

PLOW MOUNT KIT FOR YAMAHA VIKING P/N ASSEMBLY / OWNERS MANUAL. Application PLOW PUSH FRAME NO or

SAINT PAUL HOLMES OPINION BY v. Record No JUSTICE LAWRENCE L. KOONTZ, JR. April 16, 1999 JOHN DOE

Radiant heat transfer network in the simulated protective clothing system under high heat flux

Checklist: Deposing the Driver in an Auto Accident

H156/01 Mark Scheme June 20XX SPECIMEN MARKING INSTRUCTIONS

Customer Management Report (San Mateo) All Detection Types 01-Dec-2015 to 31-Dec-2015 Operator Id: %

H IG HWAY WINTER OPERATIONS SNOW REMOVAL AND ICE CONTROL POLICY. Revised January 4, 2016

Transportation and Road Weather

Maintenance/ Discontinued

DRAFT MANUSCRIPT. Title: Legibility of Text on Traffic Signs as a Function of Luminance and Size

E/ECE/324/Rev.2/Add.127/Amend.7 E/ECE/TRANS/505/Rev.2/Add.127/Amend.7

Downhole Seismic Velocity Logging Test

Work Sheet. Adverse Condition Factors Module 9

This is a repository copy of Measure for Measure. Semi-cylindrical illuminance: a semi-conceived measure?.

CH 2, Sample Problem 2

PLOW MOUNT KIT FOR POLARIS RANGER P/N ASSEMBLY / OWNERS MANUAL

Active Traffic & Safety Management System for Interstate 77 in Virginia. Chris McDonald, PE VDOT Southwest Regional Operations Director

of an algorithm for automated cause-consequence diagram construction.

c) What are cumulative curves, and how are they constructed? (1 pt) A count of the number of vehicles over time at one location (1).

Experimental assessment of mixed-mode partition theories for fracture toughness in laminated composite beams

Chapter 5 Forward-backward asymmetry: the effect of body position and pointer shapes

Answers to examination-style questions. Answers Marks Examiner s tips

ISSN LIGHT & ENGINEERING. Volume 20, Number 1, 2012 ENGINEERING LIGHT & Znack Publishing House, Moscow

Review of optimal road surface luminance considering dazzling by headlight of the car running in the opposite direction

DRIVE RESPONSIBLY, ARRIVE SAFELY! Advice for driving in wintry conditions. We wish you a safe and comfortable journey!

PROBABILITY.

GIS-BASED VISUALISATION OF TRAFFIC NOISE

Fractal dimensions of computer simulated agglomerates

DAYLIGHT, TWILIGHT, AND NIGHT VARIATION IN ROAD ENVIRONMENT-RELATED FREEWAY TRAFFIC CRASHES IN KOREA

FIREHOUSE MAGAZINE EMERGENCY VEHICLE OPERATIONS PERSONALLY OPERATED VEHICLES P.O.V. S KILL

DEA 3500: HUMAN FACTORS: THE AMBIENT ENVIRONMENT (Fall 2017)

Good Spirit School Division Parents/Guardians

APPENDIX D Summary of Comments Made by Respondents in the Margins of the Survey

The Haberdashers' Aske's Boys School Elstree, Herts. 13+ Entrance Examination 2015 PHYSICS

National Rural ITS Conference 2006

Tahlequah Public Schools Inclement Weather Transportation Plan

106 PURDUE ENGINEERING EXTENSION DEPARTMENT

TAKE ROLL CALL TO DETERMINE IF THERE IS A QUORUM OF MEMBERS PRESENT

A Driver Interface for a Road Hazard Warning System: Development and Preliminary Evaluation

Smoke Fog Superfog - Smoke and Fog mixture Heavy rain and wind

AERODROMES PANEL (AP) VISUAL AIDS WORKING GROUP (VAWG) FIFTH MEETING. Montréal, Canada, 25 to 27 June 2008

Elementary Physics October 8, 2007

Show all workings for questions that involve multiple choice.

Drilling & Fracking a well near Grass River: Notes from an informal conversation with O.I.L. Energy

VIRGINIA S I-77 VARIABLE SPEED LIMIT SYSTEM FOR LOW VISIBILITY CONDITIONS

Roundabout Level of Service

TABLE OF CONTENTS. Page BACKGROUND...1 METHOD...3 PROCEDURE...6 RESULTS...8 READING TIME EVALUATION...13 DISCUSSION...17 REFERENCES...

Solar radiation data validation

REDUCING PEAK POWER IN AUTOMATED WEAPON LAYING

Transcription:

Loughborough University Institutional Repository Motor vehicle and pedal cycle conspicuity - part 3: retroflective and fluorescent materials, disability glare of red markings. This item was submitted to Loughborough University's Institutional Repository by the/an author. Citation: COOK, S., 1998. Motor vehicle and pedal cycle conspicuity - part 3: retroflective and fluorescent materials, disability glare of red markings. Loughborough: Loughborough University Metadata Record: https://dspace.lboro.ac.uk/2134/546 Please cite the published version.

Motor Vehicle and Pedal Cycle Conspicuity Part 3: Retro-reflective and Fluorescent Materials - Disability Glare of Red Markings Project Number 9/33/13 Undertaken on behalf of The Department of Environment, Transport and the Regions (DETR) Prepared by Sharon Cook March 1998 Checked by...

1.0 Aim The aim of the assessment was to determine if the application of retro-reflective red material to the rear face of heavy and long vehicles and their trailers would mask the detection of the stop lamp signal. Stop lamp conspicuity is dependent upon the key characteristics of colour (red) and luminous intensity (up to 100cd). The placement of high performance retro-reflective red material in close proximity to the stop lights may reduce their conspicuity since the material would reduce both the colour and luminance contrast of the stop lamp. 2.0 Methodology 2.1 Introduction Two tests were devised to measure the extent of the effect of the retro-reflective material on the detection of the stop lamp. Test 1 was designed to determine if drivers could distinguish between both the stop and tail lamps, or the tail lamp only, being on in the presence of the material. This would replicate the scenario where a driver, having looked in the rear view mirror, returns their attention to the truck which they are following and has to determine if the stop lamps have been applied in the intervening period. This was considered to be a difficult task since the driver would not have observed the change in state of the stop lamp from off to on, but would have to make an absolute judgement as whether the intensity of red light emitting from the truck was greater than just that of the tail light. Test 2 similarly had the aim of detecting the stop lamp in its on state but this time it replicated the scenario where the driver s visual attention had been diverted from the truck to the opposite kerbside (as though a pedestrian was about to step out). Thus although the driver was not looking directly at the truck, there would be an awareness of it in their peripheral vision. 2.2 Variables Both studies were conducted in the hours of darkness at a local test site. A rig was built to represent the rear of a truck and a light board containing off-the-shelf truck lights was mounted at 1.1m from the ground. A 2m long, 50mm wide strip of red T779 1 ICE Ergonomics Ltd

retro-reflective material, of the reflective performance defined by the Draft Regulation, was positioned horizontally at 0mm separation above the stop lamps. It was moved vertically in successive 50mm increments to a separation distance of 300mm. This permitted testing above and below the 6 (150mm) material to lamp separation recommended by Olson et al 1992 and Ziedman et al 1981. A worst case scenario was replicated in which the lights would appear at their dimmest and the material at its brightest. Using data from Cobb 1990 which measured the on-road performance of vehicle lamps, the tail lamp intensity was set at 2cd and the stop lamp intensity at 20cd. The viewing distance from the driver to the truck was 135m which was within the range where the materials would be viewed at their brightest and accommodated the 70mph stopping distance of 96m. The truck rig was viewed under main beam since it is feasible that a driver may find himself behind a truck in such a situation, and may have to make decisions in the scenarios described. Vehicle lights from a road passing behind the test rig added visual noise to the test scene similar to that encountered on the road. 2.3 Subjects / Participants 14 male and 6 female drivers aged between 22 and 75 participated in the study. 10 participants were young (less than 45 years old) and 10 were old (more than 60 years old). 2.4 Procedure For Test 1, the participant was positioned in the drivers seat of a Ford Mondeo which was directly behind the truck rig at a distance of 135m from it. The participant was instructed to look into their laps until the experimenter instructed them to look up. On giving this instruction, the experimenter started a timer and the participant looked along the test site to the truck rig. The participant had to make a decision, as quickly as possible, as to whether the stop lamps were on or not and report their response to the experimenter. On hearing the response the experimenter stopped the timer and recorded the nature of the response and the time taken to give it. The participant then looked down and awaited the next instruction to look up. This was undertaken ten times but in only half those instances was the stop lamp on. When the ten repetitions had been completed the T779 2 ICE Ergonomics Ltd

participant was instructed to rest briefly whilst the next condition was set up. The initial condition was that of no material, followed by the material being placed directly adjacent to the lamps at 0mm and then in successive 50mm increments. As soon as the same number of correct identifications had been obtained for two successive conditions as for the initial condition in which no material was present, the test was stopped. For Test 2, the participant was again seated in the drivers seat of a Ford Mondeo but this time they were instructed not to look directly at the truck but instead to offset their line of gaze in the order of 5. The participant had to maintain their offset gaze throughout each condition but were prompted at 8-10 second intervals that the stop lamp may come on. At each prompt the participant had to be aware that the stop lamp may be activated at some time over the next 8 seconds. Ten repetitions were undertaken with the stop lamp being activated for half those instances. On the occasions when it was activated, a time delay was incorporated varying from 0-4 seconds. A second experimenter activated the stop lamp, after the given time delay, which in turn started a timer. If the participant noticed the stop lamp coming on, they had to call out to the main experimenter, who stopped the clock and recorded the time. 3.0 Results 3.1 Test 1 The aim of the test was to identify the level of material-lamp separation at which the stop lamps were correctly detected as on or off with the same degree of accuracy as for the no material condition. A safety check was included by requiring that this was achieved for two successive separation conditions so reducing the possibility that the first incidence of the correct matching of responses was due to chance. The graph below shows the level of material-lamp separation at which participants identified the stop lamp status with the same degree of accuracy as for the no material conditions. For three-quarters of the subjects the material appeared to T779 3 ICE Ergonomics Ltd

have no effect at all since the number of times they correctly identified the stop lamp as on or off was the same for the material at a separation of 0mm, i.e. directly adjacent to the stop lamp as for the no material condition. For three subjects a vertical separation of 50mm was required and for the remaining two, a separation of 100mm was needed. This data would suggest that for a vertical separation distance of 100mm between the lamp and the material all subjects could identify the status of the stop lamp with the same degree of accuracy as if no material was present. 15 Number of Participants 10 5 0 0 50 100 Vertical Separation (mm) Fig.1: Graph to show the minimum level of material-lamp separation at which participants matched their no-material detection rates However, whilst the subjects may have been as accurate at their given separation distances as for the no material condition, it is important to note any effect on their decision time. Correctly identifying if the stop lamp is on or off with the same degree of accuracy as for the no material condition can still be detrimental to road safety if it takes drivers longer to arrive at those correct decisions. Further analysis of the participants response times was therefore undertaken to investigate this factor. The reaction time data was analysed by comparing the time taken to respond when the material was at the different separation distances with the reaction time in the no material condition. The reaction time for correctly detecting the stop lamp as on (termed a hit ), was analysed separately from the time to correctly detect the stop was off (termed a correct rejection ). T779 4 ICE Ergonomics Ltd

Analysis of the participants hit reaction time is illustrated in graph 2 below. This confirms that for over half of the subjects there was no significant difference in the time taken to identify a stop lamp as on when the material was at 0mm compared to when there was no material in place at all. However there were four subjects who correctly identified the stop lamps as on for two successive conditions, but were affected by the presence of the material to the extent that the time taken to confirm the lamp as on was always significantly longer in the presence of the material. Number of Subjects 12 10 8 6 4 2 0 0 50 100 >=150 Seperation Distance (mm) Fig.2: The minimum level of separation at which the material did not affect the correct detection time of an ON stop lamp Analysis of the participants correct rejection reaction time i.e. the time it took them to correctly confirm that the stop lamp was off, is illustrated in graph 3 below. This shows that, compared to the hit reaction times, only a quarter of the participants were unaffected by the material when it was placed at 0mm from the lamp. Similarly six, compared to four, participants who were always able to distinguish off from on were so affected by the presence of the material that the time taken to confirm the lamp as off was always significantly longer than when the material was present. T779 5 ICE Ergonomics Ltd

Number of Subjects 8 6 4 2 0 0 50 100 >=150 Separation Distance (mm) Fig.3: The minimum level of separation at which the material did not affect the correct detection time of an OFF stop lamp 3.2 Test 2 The aim of the test was to identify the level of material-stop lamp separation at which the stop lamps were correctly detected as on or off with the same degree of accuracy as for the no material condition. This was conducted for the situation when the participants were not directly observing the stop lamps. When there was no material present, all the participants were able to distinguish with complete accuracy between the stop lamp being on and off i.e. all participants obtained all 5 hits (i.e. correctly identified the stop lamp as being on when it was on) and all obtained all 5 correct rejections (i.e. correctly identified the stop lamp as being off when it was off). Since the material must not interfere with the detection of the stop lamp it is important to know at what level of material-lamp separation, the hit and correct rejection rates obtained in the no material condition are matched. In terms of the number of hits there are significantly fewer correct identifications when the material-lamp separation is 0mm, 50mm and 150mm. At 200mm and above there is no difference in the number of correct identifications of the stop lamp as on compared to the no material condition. See Table 1. T779 6 ICE Ergonomics Ltd

Table 1: Mean number of correct detection for an ON stop lamp Subject Material condition None 0mm 50mm 100mm 150mm 200mm 250mm 300mm 1 5 No data 4 5 4 4 4 5 2 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 3 5 5 4 5 4 5 5 5 4 5 3 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 4 6 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 7 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 8 5 4 3 5 2 4 4 3 9 5 5 4 5 4 4 5 5 10 5 3 4 5 5 5 5 5 11 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 12 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 13 5 5 5 4 5 4 5 5 14 5 2 3 3 3 0 1 3 15 5 5 3 5 4 4 3 1 16 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 17 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 18 5 5 5 5 4 5 5 5 19 5 4 4 4 4 5 5 5 20 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 Mean 5 4.53 4.45 4.80 4.45 4.50 4.60 4.55 T-test compared to no material condition 0.03 0.00 0.10 0.01 0.07 0.09 0.07 In terms of the number of correct rejections i.e. the number of times that the stop lamp is correctly identified as being off, the material appears to have no effect at all; whatever the material-lamp separation distance, the number of correct rejections does not differ significantly from the no material condition. See Table 2. T779 7 ICE Ergonomics Ltd

Table 2: Mean number of correct detection times for an OFF stop lamp Subject Material condition None 0mm 50mm 100mm 150mm 200mm 250mm 300mm 1 5 No data 5 5 5 5 5 5 2 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 3 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 4 5 5 4 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 6 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 7 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 8 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 9 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 10 5 5 5 4 4 5 5 5 11 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 12 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 13 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 14 5 5 5 5 4 5 5 5 15 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 16 5 5 5 5 4 5 5 5 17 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 18 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 19 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 20 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 5 Mean 5 5 4.95 4.95 4.85 5 5 5 T-test compared to no material condition No difference 0.33 0.33 0.08 No difference No difference No difference However, as for Test 1, it is important to take into account the effect of the material on the time taken to make the decision when determining suitable levels of separation. Due to the design of the test, it was only appropriate to record reaction times for the number of hits. Analysis of this data indicates that at material-lamp separation distances of 0mm, 50mm and 100mm it takes significantly longer to correctly identify the stop lamp coming on compared to the no material condition. However for separation distances of 150mm and greater, there is no difference in the time to correctly detect the stop lamp compared to the no material condition. See Table 3. T779 8 ICE Ergonomics Ltd

Table 3: Correct detection times for an ON stop lamp Subject Material condition None 0mm 50mm 100mm 150mm 200mm 250mm 300mm 1 1.36 No data 1.88 2.09 2.03 1.60 1.44 2.28 2 1.74 1.48 1.19 1.97 1.15 1.29 1.38 1.24 3 1.87 1.61 2.34 1.86 1.35 1.33 1.34 1.42 4 1.13 1.13 1.10 1.15 1.02 0.97 1.46 0.93 5 1.56 2.68 1.85 2.69 1.91 1.38 1.49 1.90 6 1.24 1.53 1.42 1.57 1.33 1.36 2.01 1.38 7 1.05 0.97 0.89 0.98 0.80 1.02 0.92 0.96 8 1.24 1.40 1.19 1.18 1.40 1.28 1.19 1.21 9 1.41 2.29 2.03 2.20 2.01 1.61 1.78 1.64 10 0.95 1.14 1.80 1.00 0.92 0.92 1.00 0.97 11 0.93 1.68 1.23 0.92 0.99 0.93 0.99 0.84 12 0.84 1.12 1.73 1.00 0.88 1.29 0.94 0.91 13 0.89 0.95 0.92 1.01 0.92 1.17 0.97 0.89 14 1.00 1.44 0.99 1.12 1.03 No data 1.45 1.03 15 1.07 1.39 1.05 1.98 1.28 1.35 1.16 1.87 16 1.02 1.85 1.35 1.14 1.10 1.06 1.20 1.11 17 1.00 0.85 0.87 0.85 0.84 0.85 0.79 0.83 18 0.93 1.24 1.37 1.23 1.49 1.86 1.51 1.45 19 1.11 1.13 1.21 1.24 1.23 1.53 1.28 1.24 20 1.29 1.64 1.37 1.27 1.59 1.74 1.72 1.48 Mean 1.18 1.45 1.39 1.42 1.26 1.29 1.30 1.28 T-test compared to no material condition 0.01 0.02 0.01 0.28 0.22 0.10 0.23 4.0 Conclusions To ensure that the addition of red retro-reflective material does not detract from the conspicuity of the stop lamps, the separation distance between the material and the lamp must be such that the rate of detecting the stop lamp is equivalent to that of the no material condition. The results of Test 1 suggest that, in terms of the number of times that the stop lamp is correctly reported as being on or off, the material-lamp separation should be a minimum 100mm. However, when an allowance is made for the time taken to make these responses, a separation distance of 200mm would be more appropriate. T779 9 ICE Ergonomics Ltd

The results of Test 2 suggest that in order to maintain the same number of correct identifications of the on stop lamp as in the no material condition, the minimum material-lamp separation should be 200mm. This separation distance would also accommodate the number of correct identifications of the off top lamp since these could be accommodated by a separation of 0mm. If account is taken of the time to correctly identify the on stop lamps, a minimum separation distance of 150mm is suggested. Taking these results as a whole it would seem prudent to suggest a minimum material-lamp separation distance of 200mm. This would accommodate both the numbers of responses and the time taken to make them under the conditions of looking directly at the truck (Test 1) and observing it in the visual periphery (Test 2). 5.0 References COBB. J. (1990), Roadside survey of vehicle lighting 1989. Transport and Road Research Laboratory, Research Report 290. OLSON. P. et al (1992), Performance requirements for large truck conspiciuty enhancements. UMTRI-92-8. ZIEDMAN, K. et al (1981), Improved commercial vehicle conspicuity and signalling systems; Task II: Analysis, Experiments and Design Recommendations. Vector Enterprises Inc., Santa Monica, CA. T779 10 ICE Ergonomics Ltd