Children s Understanding of Generalisation Transformations

Similar documents
A methodology on natural occurring lines segmentation and generalization

DESIGN AND IMPLEMENTATION OF A PROTOTYPE SOFTWARE FOR INTRODUCING CARTOGRAPHY TO CHILDREN

An Information Model for Maps: Towards Cartographic Production from GIS Databases

A TRANSITION FROM SIMPLIFICATION TO GENERALISATION OF NATURAL OCCURRING LINES

Improving Map Generalisation of Buildings by Introduction of Urban Context Rules

Design and Development of a Large Scale Archaeological Information System A Pilot Study for the City of Sparti

Variation of geospatial thinking in answering geography questions based on topographic maps

Geography. Programme of study for key stage 3 and attainment target (This is an extract from The National Curriculum 2007)

COMPARISON OF MANUAL VERSUS DIGITAL LINE SIMPLIFICATION. Byron Nakos

A CARTOGRAPHIC DATA MODEL FOR BETTER GEOGRAPHICAL VISUALIZATION BASED ON KNOWLEDGE

AN ATTEMPT TO AUTOMATED GENERALIZATION OF BUILDINGS AND SETTLEMENT AREAS IN TOPOGRAPHIC MAPS

Year 8 standard elaborations Australian Curriculum: Geography

GTECH 380/722 Analytical and Computer Cartography Hunter College, CUNY Department of Geography

Digitization in a Census

Children needing support to achieve key skills. Children surpassing key skills. Geography Progression in Skills Key Stage One. Locational Knowledge

Cartography and Children Commission: Proposals for the next 4 years

APC PART I WORKSHOP MAPPING AND CARTOGRAPHY

MAP SCALE, ELEMENTS & USE

Cartography and Geovisualization. Chapters 12 and 13 of your textbook

THE CREATION OF A DIGITAL CARTOGRAPHIC DATABASE FOR LOCATOR MAPSl

THE USE OF EPSILON CONVEX AREA FOR ATTRIBUTING BENDS ALONG A CARTOGRAPHIC LINE

The maps in this resource can be freely modified and reproduced in the classroom only.

Pikes Lane Primary School. Geography Statement of Practice

Ladder versus star: Comparing two approaches for generalizing hydrologic flowline data across multiple scales. Kevin Ross

Geographical knowledge and understanding scope and sequence: Foundation to Year 10

Development of a Cartographic Expert System

DATA DISAGGREGATION BY GEOGRAPHIC

Exploring representational issues in the visualisation of geographical phenomenon over large changes in scale.

Interactive Cumulative Curves for Exploratory Classification Maps

Progression of skills in Geography

Curriculum and Assessment in Geography at KS3

GEOGRAPHY POLICY STATEMENT. The study of geography helps our pupils to make sense of the world around them.

MEADOWS PRIMARY SCHOOL and NURSERY GEOGRAPHY POLICY

Towards a formal classification of generalization operators

Taxonomies of Building Objects towards Topographic and Thematic Geo-Ontologies

Dublin City Schools Social Studies Graded Course of Study Grade 5 K-12 Social Studies Vision

Range of Opportunities

Cell-based Model For GIS Generalization

Assessment Design. AGTA Conference Rotorua NZ January 2015

STAG LANE JUNIOR SCHOOL GEOGRAPHY POLICY

Convergence measure of sketched engineering drawings

OBJECT-ORIENTATION, CARTOGRAPHIC GENERALISATION AND MULTI-PRODUCT DATABASES. P.G. Hardy

HOLY CROSS CATHOLIC PRIMARY SCHOOL

GEOGRAPHY POLICY. Date: March Signed: Review: March 2019

Mapping Welsh Neighbourhood Types. Dr Scott Orford Wales Institute for Social and Economic Research, Data and Methods WISERD

THE ROLE OF COMPUTER BASED TECHNOLOGY IN DEVELOPING UNDERSTANDING OF THE CONCEPT OF SAMPLING DISTRIBUTION

St John s Catholic Primary School. Geography Policy. Mission Statement

Exploring the Influence of Color Distance on the Map Legibility

The Estimation of Numerosity on Dot Maps

APC Part I Workshop. Mapping and Cartography. 14 November 2014

Geography Policy. for Hertsmere Jewish Primary School

ST AMBROSE CATHOLIC PRIMARY SCHOOL Geography Policy

European Solar Days May Report Spain

Line generalization: least square with double tolerance

Using OS Resources - A fieldwork activity for Key Stage 2

GENERALIZATION OF DIGITAL TOPOGRAPHIC MAP USING HYBRID LINE SIMPLIFICATION

ENV208/ENV508 Applied GIS. Week 2: Making maps, data visualisation, and GIS output

Overview. GIS Data Output Methods

Measuring locations of critical points along cartographic lines with eye movements

Subject Progression Map

Seymour Centre 2017 Education Program 2071 CURRICULUM LINKS

Assisted cartographic generalization: Can we learn from classical maps?

Implementing Visual Analytics Methods for Massive Collections of Movement Data

Basic principles of cartographic design. Makram Murad-al-shaikh M.S. Cartography Esri education delivery team

A Note on Methods. ARC Project DP The Demand for Higher Density Housing in Sydney and Melbourne Working Paper 3. City Futures Research Centre

SYLLABUS FORM WESTCHESTER COMMUNITY COLLEGE Valhalla, NY lo595. l. Course #: PHYSC NAME OF ORIGINATOR /REVISOR: ALENA O CONNOR

ASSESSMENT OF STUDENT LEARNING Department of Geology University of Puerto Rico at Mayaguez. Progress Report

AdV Project: Map Production of the DTK50

Conceptual Aspects of 3D Map Integration in Interactive School Atlases

Designing Better Maps

VISUAL ANALYTICS APPROACH FOR CONSIDERING UNCERTAINTY INFORMATION IN CHANGE ANALYSIS PROCESSES

Rebecca Nayler Medium term planning. Curriculum area GEOGRAPHY - AUTUMN 2016

Place Syntax Tool (PST)

Academic Affairs Assessment of Student Learning Report for Academic Year

GIS Visualization: A Library s Pursuit Towards Creative and Innovative Research

New National Curriculum Geography Skills Planning KS1

SOCIAL STUDIES GRADE 6. I Can Checklist REGIONS AND PEOPLE OF THE EASTERN HEMISPHERE. Office of Teaching and Learning Curriculum Division

Gedney Church End and Lutton St Nicholas Federated Primary Schools. Geography Policy

a system for input, storage, manipulation, and output of geographic information. GIS combines software with hardware,

JUNCTION MODELING IN VEHICLE NAVIGATION MAPS AND MULTIPLE REPRESENTATIONS

Year 34 B2 Geography - Continents and Oceans 2018 Key Skills to be covered: Taken from Level 3 Taken from Level 4

Community Cartography (lesson #1)

BUILDING TYPIFICATION AT MEDIUM SCALES

Orienteering, the map and child development. (Organised outdoor play with maps)

Theory, Concepts and Terminology

The Community Maps Project Craig Roland! The University of Florida! January 2002!

Landmarks Paula Owens 5 7 years

Harmonizing Level of Detail in OpenStreetMap Based Maps

Diverse Places 3 days

1. Origins of Geography

GTECH 380/722 Analytical and Computer Cartography Hunter College, CUNY Department of Geography

GTECH 380/722 Analytical and Computer Cartography Hunter College, CUNY Department of Geography

Topic: Fundamentals of Topographic Map (Cartographic Technique) Honors

Lecture 10 Mapping Quantities: Dot Density Maps

Risky world. What is risk? Is our local area a risky place? To understand what is meant by risky places

Your web browser (Safari 7) is out of date. For more security, comfort and. the best experience on this site: Update your browser Ignore

Automatic Generalization of Cartographic Data for Multi-scale Maps Representations

Geography Policy 2014

POLITICAL SYSTEMS. (ps ) Evaluate positions taken regarding the necessity of government and the purposes of government.

Transcription:

Children s Understanding of Generalisation Transformations V. Filippakopoulou, B. Nakos, E. Michaelidou Cartography Laboratory, Faculty of Rural and Surveying Engineering National Technical University of Athens bfilippa@central.ntua.gr bnakos@central.ntua.gr emichael@survey.ntua.gr Abstract Children are using maps presenting their local environment (large scale maps), their country or the surrounding countries (medium or small scale maps), as well as world maps (very small scale maps). Do children understand the decreasing map scales? Can children recognise the differences of spatial representations in various scale maps? Behind these two questions the process of cartographic generalisation is hidden. The aim of the present study is to clarify the children s conception of generalisation transformations. A questionnaire was given to 12-18 year-old secondary school children. The answers record children s response to the generalisation transformations. The results are helpful for teaching purposes and for designing more effective maps for children. Introduction According to the school curriculum children are using maps and atlases as tools of learning the content of geographic courses. Aim of teaching geography is the knowledge and understanding of spatial relationships. Children are using maps presenting their local environment (large scale maps), their country or the surrounding countries (medium or small scale maps), as well as world maps (very small scale maps). Do children understand the decreasing map scales? Can children recognise the differences of spatial representations in various scale maps? Behind these two questions the process of cartographic generalisation is hidden. Generalisation is the process that affects more than any other cartographic transformation the information that is contained within a map. Generalisation affects both location and meaning of information [Keates, 1989]. Furthermore, by reducing the map scale two crucial cartographic problems are arising. The first one refers to the reduction of the available space for the representation of a specific area and the second one to the need of enlarging the physical dimensions of spatial objects in order to make them clearly visible. The aim of the present study is to clarify the children s conception of generalisation transformations. Spatial transformations such as simplification, aggregation, amalgamation, merging, collapse, displacement, etc., which control the graphic modification caused by generalisation, were tested. For the purpose of the study sample maps depicting feature changes due to scale reduction were composed, by applying the above mentioned generalisation transformations. A questionnaire was given to 12-18 year-old secondary school children. The answers record children s response to the generalisation transformations. The results are helpful for teaching purposes and for designing more effective maps for children. The same results are critical for building a robust environment on the topic of the didactic of both geography and cartography. The state of the art The understanding of how map-readers -especially the younger ones- conceive the decreasing map scales and recognise the modifications of the portrayed information on maps would contribute to the effective design of maps and atlases and the teaching of cartography. To date, there has been little investigation on the subject. Anderson [1995] stressed that little work has been done on what constitutes appropriate level of map generalisation and complexity for children. In a recent research, 11 to 14 years of age, who were invited to construct a series of thematic maps using a specially designed software tool, revealed a weak notion of both spatial and numerical factors underpinning symbol generalisation [Wiegand and Tait, 1999]. Heamon [1973] examined how 8 to 14 years old draw the outline of towns and villages presented on photographs and maps and concluded that children from the third and fourth year of the primary school onwards can be given the opportunity to make generalisations based on spatial data. Gimeno and Bertin [1983] described a graphic method of teaching cartography, which enabled 10 to 11 year of age to discover by themselves fundamental principles of graphic symbolisation and apply the visual means to represent the concept of order. The same authors also described how 9 to 10 and 10 to 11 years old discovered the procedures used to develop a map, which synthesises several phenomena. The applied the processes of selection and generalisation transformations to the selected information like simplification, smoothing, collapse, and refinement to develop the map.

Castner [1983] emphasised the need to distinguish between scale-dependent generalisation and function-dependent generalisation when children are introduced to maps and mapping. The objectives of the present study are twofold. The first is about recognition of a region presented on a map as the scale of the map reduces. The second is about understanding the spatial transformations defined by McMaster and Shea [1992], that alter the representation of the information of the maps as the scale reduces. Generalisation transformations People see and use maps of a wide range of scales, as for example, large scale maps of their local environment, medium scale maps of their country or of extended geographical areas, and small scale maps of continents or of the world. As the scale of the map decreases the representation of geographical information is imposed to modifications in order to retain graphic legibility and respond to the purpose of the map. Information at the lower level of classification hierarchy may be omitted. Features of the representation may be simplified, exaggerated or enhanced. These changes may cause overlap between features, leading to their displacement. Competition of space may enforce the reduction of features dimensionality, as for example from area to line, or dictate the join of features. All these processes constitute the application of generalisation. After a review of different definitions for generalisation Keates [1989] concluded that the principle elements of the process seem to fall into two main groups: scale and graphic requirements (legibility), and characteristics and importance. The scale of the map controls the available space, in which there are minimum requirements for graphic legibility. Legibility itself depends on symbol size, form and colour and the amount of information concentrated within an area. The retention of the essential characteristics of the phenomena represented (in terms of shape and configuration) and the graphic emphasis according to the degree of importance attached to the features facilitates correct interpretation. Different transformations in the course of generalisation are introduced to the map to maintain legibility and to accommodate relative importance. These transformations affect both components of the information contained within a map location and attribute at location. Various transformation sets have been described [Shea and McMaster, 1989; Mackaness, 1991; McMaster and Shea, 1992; Jones, 1997]. McMaster and Shea [1992] described in a comprehensive way ten transformations altering the representation of the data from a spatial perspective and two from attribute perspective. As the scale decreases the map-reader may recognise the effects of the following spatial transformations: 1. Simplification transformations have the effects of retaining the least number of data points necessary to represent the character of map features and of rejecting the redundant point considered to be unnecessary to display line s character. 2. Smoothing transformations act on lines by decreasing the constituent detail and adjusting the positions of existing points. They produce a simplified version by planing away small perturbations and capturing only the most significant trends of the line. 3. Aggregation transformations merge point symbols of the same class into a single area symbol when the points come into a close proximity and prohibit each other from being portrayed. 4. Amalgamation transformations apply to a set of adjacent area features that are of the same class. They merge these features into a larger element retaining the general characteristic of the region. 5. Merging transformations act on two or more adjacent lines, with a separate distance between them and merge them into one line positioned approximately halfway between them. 6. Collapse transformations reduce the dimensionality of an area feature to a point or a line. 7. Refinement/typification transformations. Refinement transformations act when features of the same class are either too numerous or too small to be portrayed to scale. They eliminate the smallest features or those, which add little to the general impression of the distribution. The general pattern of the features is maintained with those features that are chosen to remain shown on their correct location. Typification transformations act in a similar way to the refinement process maintaining the general pattern of the features but with the remaining features shown in approximate location. 8. Exaggeration transformations apply to features to enlarge or exaggerate their shape or size to meet the specific requirements of the reduction of the scale. 9. Enhancement transformations apply to features that need to be exaggerated or emphasised to meet the specific requirements of the map. They deal primarily with the symbolisation component and not with the spatial dimensions of the feature although they produce some spatial enhancement. 10. Displacement transformations are used to face the problem that arises when two or more features come in conflicting either by overlapping each other or becoming too close to be clearly discernible. The features are moved from their true planimetric locations to allow for the application of symbology. Attribute transformations include classification, and symbolisation:

1. Classification is the process of ordering, scaling and grouping features by their attributes. 2. Symbolisation is the process of graphically coding information and placing it into a map context [Robinson et al., 1995]. Research Method As it comes out from the review of the existing studies, focused on the keywords: children cartography and generalisation, there is a lack of information about the way children react to the generalisation procedure. The aim of the present study is to give a first insight on the conjunction of these two cartographic aspects. Children s reaction to generalisation procedure can be investigated through their ability in applying the spatial transformations of generalisation. Usually, the generalisation of a spatial object is obtained by applying more than one transformation simultaneously. For the purpose of this study, in order to investigate each spatial transformation separately, the investigation method was designed so that each one of eight spatial transformations was checked independently. The eight spatial transformations of generalisation are: simplification (see Fig. 1a), amalgamation (see Fig. 1b), merging (see Fig. 1c), collapse (see Fig. 1d&e), typification (see Fig. 1f), exaggeration (see Fig. 1g), displacement (see Fig. 1h), and aggregation (see Fig. 1i). (a) (b) (c) (d) (e) (f) (g) (h) (i) Figure 1. The eight spatial transformations of generalisation used in the study. The spatial transformations of generalisation used in the study are illustrated in Fig. 1. Each part of Fig. 1 at the left side portrays an object in the original scale (large scale) and at the right side the object after the application of the transformation in the derived scale (small scale). Subjects A total of 64 subjects took part in the research at this preliminary stage. Thirty one of them were secondary school (aged: 12-18), from various high schools, both public and private, of Athens district area. Furthermore, twenty two undergraduate (aged: 22-25) of the Faculty of Rural and Surveying Engineering of National Technical University of Athens and eleven graduate (aged: 25-35) of the same Faculty in Geoinformatics. Materials The subjects had to take a test, which had two parts. At the first part, two equally sized rectangular segments of topographic maps (one original and one derived), centred at the same location, in different scales (with a ratio scale 1:2) were given. The subjects were asked to enclose in a frame on the derived map the region depicted on the original map. At the second part, a brief description on the eight spatial transformations of generalisation was given along with graphical examples (see Fig. 1). Eight sample maps, composed for the purpose of the study, each one in two scales (one original and one derived), consisted the test material of the second part. In the derived scale map one or more objects were missing and the subjects were asked to draw the missing object(s) by applying the most appropriate according to their opinion spatial transformation of generalisation. The test (part 1 and part 2) took place in an office by personal interview lasted for half an hour. Fig. 2 illustrates all sample maps used for the second part of the study. It has to be noticed that the order of the spatial transformations between the sample maps and the description examples did not have the same sequence.

Figure 2. The eight sample maps at the original (left) and the derived (right) scale used in the study. Evaluation The answers were evaluated quantitatively. Concerning the first part of the study where the subjects were asked to enclose in a frame on the derived map the region depicted on the original map the answers were scored as 0%, 25%,

50%, 75%, and 100%. In the second part, the questions concerning the transformations, the evaluation was based on two criteria. With the first criterion the conception of each spatial transformation of generalisation by the subject was evaluated. With the second criterion the graphical representation drawn by the subject was evaluated. We tried not to be biased by the drawing ability of the subjects. Each answer was scored with 0 or 1 for the conceptual criterion, 0 or 1 for the graphic criterion and 0 or 1 for the judgement of the total result. Each questionnaire was evaluated by two persons separately, and there was no significant difference between the scores of the two evaluations. Tables 1, 2, 3 and 4 present the preliminary results of the study. Table 1. Results of area reduction test in first part of the study (% distribution). Area Frame High school Univ. graduate 0% 11 4 9 25% 0 23 18 50% 25 23 27 75% 36 36 28 100% 28 14 18 Table 2. Results of spatial transformations of generalisation against conceptual criterion (% correct answers). Spatial transformations High school Univ. graduate Simplification 100 100 100 Aggregation 29 46 46 Amalgamation 61 82 91 Merging 68 91 82 Collapse 50 58 55 Typification 50 59 46 Exaggeration 89 73 73 Displacement 82 100 100 Table 3. Results of spatial transformations of generalisation against graphic criterion (% correct answers). Spatial transformations High school Univ. graduate Simplification 87 86 100 Aggregation 29 36 27 Amalgamation 61 82 73 Merging 68 91 73 Collapse 43 45 18 Typification 50 55 46 Exaggeration 54 41 36 Displacement 79 77 100 Table 4. Total results of second part of the study (% correct answers). Spatial transformations High school Univ. graduate Simplification 96 91 100 Aggregation 29 46 46 Amalgamation 61 82 91 Merging 68 91 82 Collapse 50 59 55 Typification 50 55 46 Exaggeration 86 64 64 Displacement 82 86 100

Discussion Although the research has not been integrated a few comments can be stated based on the first preliminary results. The subjects that have already participated in the study are faced as belonging to three groups: the high school, the undergraduate, and the graduate of the University. Comparing the results of these three groups we can notice: The high score of the high school children (in comparison to the one of the University ) in the first part of the study. The small differences in the scores among the three subject s groups, in most of the questions concerning the spatial transformations of generalisation. The understanding of each spatial transformation varies quite a lot. Simplification, amalgamation, merging, exaggeration, and displacement were successfully applied. Collapse and typification were not applied by almost half of the subjects, and aggregation was the spatial transformation that subjects got the lowest score. Finally, it has to be mentioned that high school had no significant difficulty in applying the spatial transformations of generalisation, although they did not have any previous cartographic education or practice on generalisation. Displacement Exaggeration Total results Typification Collapse Merging Amalgamation Aggregation Simplification Univ. graduate High school 0 20 40 60 80 100 % correct answers Figure 3. Comparison of the results of the three groups of subjects. References Anderson, J. (1995). Maps for and by children: possible contributions by cartographers. Proceedings of 17 th International Cartographic Conference, Spain, Barcelona, Vol. 1, 384-392. Castner, H.W. (1983). Research in cartographic communication: any implications for introducing maps and mapping to young children. The Operational Geographer, 2, 13-16. Jones, C. B. (1997). Geographical Information Systems and Computer Cartography. Essex: Addison Wesley Longman Ltd. Gimeno, R., & Bertin, J. (1983). The cartography lesson in elementary school. In D.R.F. Taylor (Ed.), Graphic Communication and Design in Contemporary Cartography (pp. 231-256). Chichester: John Wiley & Sons. Heamon, A.J. (1973). The maturation of spatial ability in Geography. Educational Research, 16(1), 63-66. Keates, J.S. (1989). Cartographic Design and Production. Essex: Longman Scientific & Technical. Mackaness, W. (1991). Integration and evaluation of map generalisation. In Map Generalization: Making rules for knowledge representation (Eds. B. P. Buttenfield and R. B. McMaster, pp. 217-226). Longman Scientific & Technical, Essex. McMaster, R. (1992). Generalization in Digital Cartography. Washington: Association of American Geographers. Robinson, A.H., Sale, R.D., Morrison, J.L., Muehrcke, Ph.C. (1995). Elements of Cartography (5 th edition). New York: John Wiley & Sons. Shea, K.S., and McMaster, R.B. (1989). Cartographic generalization in a digital environment: when and how to generalize. Proceedings of AutoCarto 9, Baltimore, Maryland, 56-67. Wiegand, P., and Tait, K. (1999). Promoting children s collaborative learning in cartography with a software mapping tool. Proceedings of 19 th International Cartographic Conference, Canada, Ottawa, Vol. 1, 499-505.