Supplementary Figure 1. Characterization of the single-photon quantum light source based on spontaneous parametric down-conversion (SPDC).

Similar documents
BASIC VISUAL SCIENCE CORE

Supplementary Figures

Concerns of the Psychophysicist. Three methods for measuring perception. Yes/no method of constant stimuli. Detection / discrimination.

Observation of the nonlinear phase shift due to single post-selected photons

The Bayesian Brain. Robert Jacobs Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences University of Rochester. May 11, 2017

OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I

Supplementary Materials for

Γ43 γ. Pump Γ31 Γ32 Γ42 Γ41

Supplementary Materials for

Confocal Microscopy Imaging of Single Emitter Fluorescence and Hanbury Brown and Twiss Photon Antibunching Setup

Surround effects on the shape of the temporal contrast-sensitivity function

COLOR SCIENCE. Concepts and Methods, Quantitative Data and Formulae, 2nd Edition. John Wiley & Sons New York Chichester Brisbane Toronto Singapore

Supplemental Information. Noise and Correlations. in Parallel Perceptual Decision Making. Thomas U. Otto and Pascal Mamassian

CLINICAL VISUAL OPTICS (OPTO 223) Weeks XII & XIII Dr Salwa Alsaleh

!) + log(t) # n i. The last two terms on the right hand side (RHS) are clearly independent of θ and can be

12/2/15. G Perception. Bayesian Decision Theory. Laurence T. Maloney. Perceptual Tasks. Testing hypotheses. Estimation

Entanglement and Bell s Inequalities. Benjamin Feifke, Kara Morse. Professor Svetlana Lukishova

+ + ( + ) = Linear recurrent networks. Simpler, much more amenable to analytic treatment E.g. by choosing

Hue discrimination in peripheral vision under conditions of dark and light adaptation*

Contrast Sensitivity

LIST of SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIALS

The idiosyncratic nature of confidence

A Model of Local Adaptation supplementary information

Chemistry 524--Final Exam--Keiderling May 4, :30 -?? pm SES

Schemes to generate entangled photon pairs via spontaneous parametric down conversion

Lab 3-4 : Confocal Microscope Imaging of Single-Emitter Fluorescence and Hanbury-Brown and Twiss Set Up, Photon Antibunching

Fresnel Equations cont.

Adaptation in the Neural Code of the Retina

arxiv: v1 [physics.optics] 12 Feb 2013

Testing The Existence of Single Photons

A DECISION-MAKING THEORY OF VISUAL DETECTION l

Temporal context calibrates interval timing

Is the Human Visual System Invariant to Translation and Scale?

Vision & Perception. Simple model: simple reflectance/illumination model. image: x(n 1,n 2 )=i(n 1,n 2 )r(n 1,n 2 ) 0 < r(n 1,n 2 ) < 1

Implementation and evaluation of data analysis strategies for time-resolved optical spectroscopy

Parametric down-conversion

The structure of laser pulses

Practical aspects of QKD security

In Situ Imaging of Cold Atomic Gases

Digital Holographic Measurement of Nanometric Optical Excitation on Soft Matter by Optical Pressure and Photothermal Interactions

Lecture 9. PMTs and Laser Noise. Lecture 9. Photon Counting. Photomultiplier Tubes (PMTs) Laser Phase Noise. Relative Intensity

Lateral organization & computation

PART 2 : BALANCED HOMODYNE DETECTION

Ângelo Cardoso 27 May, Symbolic and Sub-Symbolic Learning Course Instituto Superior Técnico

On a proposal for Quantum Signalling

Optical solitons and its applications

Electromagnetic Waves

Supplementary Figure 1 Comparison of single quantum emitters on two type of substrates:

Single Emitter Detection with Fluorescence and Extinction Spectroscopy

On a proposal for Quantum Signalling

If we want to analyze experimental or simulated data we might encounter the following tasks:

Visualization of Xe and Sn Atoms Generated from Laser-Produced Plasma for EUV Light Source

Highly Efficient and Anomalous Charge Transfer in van der Waals Trilayer Semiconductors

Simple scheme for efficient linear optics quantum gates

(Received 15 June 1973)

Paper Review. Special Topics in Optical Engineering II (15/1) Minkyu Kim. IEEE Journal of Quantum Electronics, Feb 1985

Quantum and Nano Optics Laboratory. Jacob Begis Lab partners: Josh Rose, Edward Pei

Optoelectronic Applications. Injection Locked Oscillators. Injection Locked Oscillators. Q 2, ω 2. Q 1, ω 1

File name: Supplementary Information Description: Supplementary Figures, Supplementary Notes and Supplementary References

Ultrafast Dynamics and Single Particle Spectroscopy of Au-CdSe Nanorods

Radiometry. Energy & Power

Statistical Tools and Techniques for Solar Astronomers

Lecture 15. Theory of random processes Part III: Poisson random processes. Harrison H. Barrett University of Arizona

AQA Physics /7408

Increasing your confidence Proving that data is single molecule. Chem 184 Lecture David Altman 5/27/08

Supplemental material for Bound electron nonlinearity beyond the ionization threshold

1 Mathematical description of ultrashort laser pulses

Counting Photons to Calibrate a Photometer for Stellar Intensity Interferometry

QPR No. 80 VIII. NOISE IN ELECTRON DEVICES. Academic and Research Staff. Prof. H. A. Haus Prof. P. Penfield, Jr. Prof. R. P. Rafuse.

Bayesian Inference. Will Penny. 24th February Bayesian Inference. Will Penny. Bayesian Inference. References

Laboratory 3&4: Confocal Microscopy Imaging of Single-Emitter Fluorescence and Hanbury Brown and Twiss setup for Photon Antibunching

CHEM Outline (Part 15) - Luminescence 2013

RADIO SYSTEMS ETIN15. Lecture no: Equalization. Ove Edfors, Department of Electrical and Information Technology

Confocal Microscope Imaging of Single-Emitter Fluorescence and Photon Antibunching

Quantum Optics and Quantum Information Laboratory Review

Adaptation to a 'spatial-frequency doubled' stimulus

APPARENT CONTRAST OF SPATIALLY AND TEMPORALLY SAMPLED GRATINGS

Computer Modelling and Numerical Simulation of the Solid State Diode Pumped Nd 3+ :YAG Laser with Intracavity Saturable Absorber

Take that, Bell s Inequality!

Time resolved optical spectroscopy methods for organic photovoltaics. Enrico Da Como. Department of Physics, University of Bath

Bayesian probability theory and generative models

UV-Vis optical fiber assisted spectroscopy in thin films and solutions

3.3 Population Decoding

Supplementary Information for Coherent optical wavelength conversion via cavity-optomechanics

3 Neural Decoding. 3.1 Encoding and Decoding. (r 1, r 2,..., r N ) for N neurons is a list of spike-count firing rates, although,

LFFs in Vertical Cavity Lasers

Dynamic Causal Modelling for fmri

Richard Miles and Arthur Dogariu. Mechanical and Aerospace Engineering Princeton University, Princeton, NJ 08540, USA

Chapter 26: Properties of Light

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

Comparison of receptive fields to polar and Cartesian stimuli computed with two kinds of models

High quantum efficiency S-20 photocathodes for photon counting applications

Assessment of Threshold for Nonlinear Effects in Ibsen Transmission Gratings

Quantum information processing using linear optics

1 Instrument response function

The functional organization of the visual cortex in primates

Nonlinear ultrafast fiber optic devices based on Carbon Nanotubes

Decision-making and Weber s law: a neurophysiological model

Simulation and Implementation of Decoy State Quantum Key Distribution over 60km Telecom Fiber

Pulsed Lasers Revised: 2/12/14 15: , Henry Zmuda Set 5a Pulsed Lasers

Transcription:

.2 Classical light source.8 g (2) ().6.4.2 EMCCD SPAD 2 3.2.4.6.8..2.4.6.8.2 Mean number of photon pairs per pump pulse 4 5 6 7 8 9 2 3 4 Supplementary Figure. Characterization of the single-photon quantum light source based on spontaneous parametric down-conversion (SPDC). Experimentally measured g (2) () values of the SPDC source using an SPAD (squares) and an EMCCD (circles) as a detector in the idler arm as a function of SPDC pump power (mean photon pairs per pulse). Lines: numerical simulations taking into account the parameters and theory of operation of the SPAD (dashed) and the EMCCD (solid) (see Methods for details). Gray dotted line shows performance of classical light source with g (2) ()=, i.e. Poisson distributed. Error bars denote s.e.m.

Performance of an ideal detector as a function of efficiency and noise and comparison to the experiment Probability of correct response.53.52.5 η=.4 η=.3 η=.2 η=. Experiment 5 6 7 8 9 2 2 22 23 24 25 26 27.5.5.5 2 2.5 3 Average number of noise events (d) Supplementary Figure 2. Performance of an ideal detector in two alternative force choice (2AFC). Simulations of the performance of an ideal detector in 2AFC trials based on signal detection theory (SDT). Performance decreases as a function of average number of noise events per integration time. Baseline at.5 corresponds to performance expected from random guessing. Different colors denote simulations with different parameters of efficiency (η). Range for d and η based on data on physiological measurements (Supplementary Table ). Experimental data is shown as black circle with error bar at the estimated level of noise. 2

a Probability of correct response Subject specific probabilities of correct response in two alternative force choice (2AFC) paradigm is a function of average photon number at the cornea.9.8.7.6.5 t = 62 ± 6 Subject A d Discriminability index ( m) 4 3 2 Subject specific discriminability index in two alternative force choice (2AFC) paradigm as a function of average photon number at the cornea =.9 ±.7 Subject A.4 2 4 6 8 2 4 Photons at cornea 2 4 6 8 2 4 Photons at cornea b Probability of correct response.9.8.7.6.5 t = 49 ± 6 Subject B e Discriminability index ( m) 4 3 2 =.4 ±.4 Subject B.4 2 4 6 8 2 4 Photons at cornea 2 4 6 8 2 4 Photons at cornea c Probability of correct response.9.8.7.6.5 t = 6 ± 7 Subject C f Discriminability index ( m) 4 3 2 =.7 ±.3 Subject C 28 29 3 3 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 4.4 2 4 6 8 2 4 2 4 6 8 2 4 Photons at cornea Photons at cornea Supplementary Figure 3. Subject specific probabilities for giving a correct answer in the high photon range. (a) - (c) The probability of correct response for the high photon range (2-4) using classical Poisson light for combined confidence ratings subjects A, B and C respectively. Solid lines are fits used to extract the sensitivity threshold, defined as number of photons required to reach a performance level equal to the half of the maximum value, indicated as a number in top left corner. Baseline at.5 corresponds to random guessing performance. (d) - (f) Corresponding discriminability index (Δm) (Supplementary Note 2). Solid lines show power fits to the data ~ x ρ. Value of the fit parameter is shown on the plot. 3

a Experimental setup with classical light source Laser AOM Fixation light ND BS ND Dark chamber Eye F Fiber L SPAD T 2 R R2 R3 b Change in the fraction of correct responses for the 3 confidence ratings as a function of training c Change in the probability of correct response for high confidence rating (R3) events as a function of training.7 Fraction of correct responses.8.6.4.2 R R2 R3 Probability of correct response.6.5.4 4 42 2 4 6 8 Session number.3 2 4 6 8 Session Number 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 5 5 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 Supplementary Figure 4. Experimental setup with classical light source and improvement of performance with training at low (-5) photon numbers. (a) Schematic of the experimental setup with classical light source. Light stimuli from a ~5 nm laser source, attenuated by an acousto-optic modulator (AOM), are triggered (T) by subjects and coupled into a single-mode fiber entering the dark chamber. The light is directed and focused onto the pupil (Maxwellian view) at an angle of 23ᵒ temporal to a highly attenuated red fixation light presented at the fovea. F, bandpass filter, ND, neutral density filter, L, lens, BS, 5/5 beam-splitter, SPAD, single-photon avalanche detector. (b) Fraction of correct responses for the three confidence ratings (R, R2, R3) as a function of training sessions for photons in the photon range -5 photons at the cornea. Data was pooled across subjects. Fraction of high confidence (R3) ratings increases with training. (c) The probability of correct response for combined subjects and for R3 answers shows improvement with session number. Baseline at.5 corresponds to random guessing level. Error bars denote s.e.m. 4

6 a Distribution of times between correct high confidence (R3) events b Distribution of times from beginning of sessions to correct high confidence (R3) events 6 8 5 7 Occurrence 4 3 2 Occurrence 6 5 4 3 2 6 5 5 2 25 3 35 4 Time between two consecutive correct high-confidence events (min) 2 3 4 5 6 7 Time from the beginning of sessions to correct high confidence (R3) events (min) 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 7 7 Supplementary Figure 5. Timing analysis of correct high confidence (R3) events in SPDC single-photon trials. (a) Distribution of times between the correct high confidence (R3) events in all sessions and fit to the exponential function (solid line). Data from all subjects. (b) Distribution of the times of correct high confidence events with respect to the beginning of the sessions for combined subjects and sessions (at time point ). The distribution is not statistically different from uniform on -6 min (p =.7). 72 5

a.8 Single photon priming effect with positive feedback in the preceding trial c Distribution of confidence ratings for all responses Probability of correct response b Probability of correct response.7.6.5 Positive feedback Combined fit.4 5 5 2 25 3 Delay between st and 2nd photons (s) Single photon priming effect with negative feedback in the preceding trial.8.7.6.5 Negative feedback Combined fit Probability of correct response Fraction of all responses d.8.6.4.2.7.6.5 Positive feedback Negative feedback R R2 R3 Probability of correct response in single photon 2AFC Positive feedback Negative feedback 73.4 5 5 2 25 3 Delay between st and 2nd photons (s).4 All combined R3 74 75 76 77 78 79 8 8 82 83 84 85 86 87 Supplementary Figure 6. Effect of positive and negative feedback in single-photon experiments. (a), (b) Probability of correct response averaged across subjects and ratings for single-photon trials for which subjects received positive (a) and negative (b) feedback as a function of time to the preceding single photon. Solid line is fit of the combined data (Fig. 2c). (c), (d) Distribution of ratings and probability of correct response in single-photon post-selected trials. Only those trials whose preceding trial was blank were chosen. Blue and red bars are plotted based on the feedback received in the previous blank trial. Lack of significant difference between data for positive and negative feedback suggest that higher order unspecific (cortical) effect are unlikely to mediate the observed single-photon induced priming effect. 88 89 6

9 Source Ambach, W. et al. Spectral transmission of the optical media of the human eye with respect to keratitis and cataract formation. Documenta ophthalmologica.advances in ophthalmology 88, 65-73 (994). Wavelength 45-7 nm Transmission Fraction Efficiency of producing from cornea to retina absorbed by retina output signal > 8% Overall Barlow, H. B. Retinal and central factors in human vision limited by noise., in Vertebrate photoreception. ed H.B. Barlow (Academic Press, 977). 5 nm 5-75% 5-55% 6 - % - 33% Boettner, E. A. & Wolter, J. R. Transmission of the Ocular Media. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci, 776-783 (962). 5 nm max 83.5% Donner, K. Noise and the absolute thresholds of cone and rod vision. Vision Res 32, 853-866 (992). 5 nm 5-75% 8-93% 6-7% 7-39% Hallett, P. E. Quantum efficiency of dark-adapted human vision. J Opt Soc Am A 4, 233-2335 (987). 57 nm 2-3% (*) Norren, D. V. & Vos, J. J. Spectral transmission of the human ocular media. Vision Res 4, 237-244 (974). 5 nm 75% Rieke, F. & Baylor, D. A. Single-photon detection by rod cells of the retina. Reviews of Modern Physics 7, 27-36 (998). ~ 5 nm ~ 5% 6-67% Xu, J., Pokorny, J. & Smith, V. C. Optical density of the human lens. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis 4, 953-96 (997) 43-74 nm 95.3% (only lens) 9 *This value was estimated based on psychophysics studies as theoretically possible maximal range 92 93 94 95 96 97 98 Supplementary Table. Summary of the efficiency estimates in the visual systems based on physiological measurements. The overall efficiency is the combined effect of transmission through the ocular media from cornea to the retina (column 3), efficiency of absorption (column 4) and efficiency of producing the output signal (column 5). The end column shows the product of the preceding three columns. 99 7

Camera Parameter Value Read-Out Speed MHz Vertical Shift Speed.3 µs Vertical Shift Amplitude +2 V Pre-Gain 3 Gain Exposure time µs Temperature (water-cooled) -8 C 2 3 Supplementary Table 2. Summary of EMCCD camera settings used when in (single-) photon counting mode. 4 5 8

6 N Parameter Description 95% confidence range from posterior distribution 7 tlag Lag time between an isomerization and the beginning of the increase in gain 2 Tmax Time at which the gain reaches its maximal value 3 α Parameter defining the profile of the rising part of the gain function 4 β Parameter defining the profile of the decreasing part of the gain function 5 A Maximal amplitude of the gain 6 d Average number of photon-like noise events per area per integration time 7 P Average number of photon events due to single photon incident on the eye 8 C2 Desicion criterion separating low and medium confidence ratings 9 C3 Desicion criterion separating medium and high confidence ratings - s 2.7-4 s. - 4.6-6 - 6.2.2 -.2 -.48.9 -.2 3.2-4 8 9 2 Supplementary Table 3. Parameters and their inferred values for the model of the photon-induced temporal modulation of single-photon light perception. 9

3 Supplementary Note. Performance of an ideal detector in 2AFC trials 4 5 6 7 8 9 2 Signal detection theory (SDT) provides a framework for analyzing detection in psychophysics tasks. In SDT the sensory process is assumed to have a continuous output based on a distribution of noise or noise combined with a signal. In a visual process in its simplest assumption, noise and noise-plus-signal distributions are appropriately described as Poisson. If there are no other sources of noise except for irreducible Poisson fluctuations, the probability of detecting n noise events is described by, 2 n d pd ( n) = e n! d () 22 23 where d is the mean amount of noise events per integration interval. The probability of detecting k events within the interval containing noise and signal is: 24 k S ps( k) = e k! S (2) 25 26 where S is the average number of signal plus noise events, and S=d +ηe where E is the average number of photons at the cornea, η is the quantum efficiency of the system, 27 28 SDT assumes that the detector s decision in a 2AFC task is determined by the likelihood ratio : 29 f ( x, x2 < sn > ) L ( x, x2) = (3) f ( x, x < ns > ) 2 3 3 where f x, x < sn ) and f x, x < ns ) are two distributions for the alternative ( 2 > ( 2 > cases where the signal was presented in the first or in the second interval, respectively,

32 33 and x and x 2 are respectively the number of events in the first and second intervals. Using Poisson likelihood, the log likelihood is simplified to: 34 S ln L( x, x2) = ( x x2) ln (4) d 35 36 37 38 39 We define an ideal detector as an idealistic observer, whose performance is only limited by the presence of noise and the quantum efficiency. In making a decision for a 2AFC trial, an ideal detector will maximize the likelihood and therefore pick the interval based on the difference x - x 2. Assuming the ideal observer guesses randomly when x = x 2, the probability of correct response is: 4 P ( C) = pdiff () + pdiff ( i) (5) 2 i> 4 where p diff (i) is the distribution of the difference S - d. 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 In the ideal case, the probability of correct response depends only on the two parameters d and η. Assuming an integration time of ms and using known rates of dark isomerizations 2, this equates to ~ photon per trial interval in our experiments (see also Supplementary Note 3). We have plotted the probability of correct response for the ideal detector as a function of the average number of photon events per integration time (d) for different values of quantum efficiency (η) (Supplementary Fig. 2). 49 5 5 The model of an ideal detector also allows for studying how bias in the subject s responses towards the first or second interval might influence their overall performance. In the ideal case and in the limit of an infinite amount of experimental

52 53 54 55 trials, the presence of a bias can only decrease the overall performance and cannot explain any increase. Our data indicated that on average our subjects tended to choose the second interval over the first 6% more frequently, which would result in an insignificant degradation of performance. 56 57 58 59 6 6 In reality, the finite number of post-selected trials (n=242) might have resulted, due to statistical variation, in more presentations in, e.g., the second interval. The likelihood that elevated performance results from such an effect can be evaluated using Fisher s statistical test, given the contingency matrix of our data. This analysis results in p=.5 for all responses and p=. for high confidence responses (R3). Therefore, subjects bias cannot account for the observed performance of correctly detecting single photons. 62 63 64 Supplementary Note 2. Estimating the discriminability index Δm from experimental data 65 66 67 68 69 7 7 72 Signal detection theory is widely used to extract parameters of underlying noise and signal plus noise distributions from experimental data. The distance between the means of the signal plus noise and the noise distributions in standard deviation units is called d (d-prime), and can be calculated from the data. In the visual system, detection is described by Poisson distributions, which have different variances for different means. Therefore we have chosen to calculate the index of discriminability Δm, which like d is a measure of the separation between the noise and the signal-plus-noise distributions, but can be calculated for distributions with different variances: 2

73 S d Δ m =, (6) σ N 74 where S is the mean of the signal plus noise distribution, d is the mean noise, σ N is 75 standard deviation of the noise distribution, with σ d. N = 76 77 78 The probability of correct response is related to S and d, and therefore Δm via equation (5) (Supplementary Note ). We used it to infer the Δm value from the known probability of correct response in our 2AFC trials. 79 8 8 Supplementary Note 3 Model for the photon-induced temporal modulation of single- photon light perception 82 83 84 85 86 87 Single-photon perception priming (Fig. 2c,d) is unlikely to represent an unspecific higher brain cognitive effect (see Methods), and we speculate that it rather represents a part of the retinal processing circuitry. We aimed to develop a mathematical model of the visual system that would allow us to study how the probability of giving the correct answer and, more generally, the response of the subject, depends on the parameters of the system. All parameters of the model are summarized in Supplementary Table 3. 88 89 9 9 92 We assume that each photon-like event, irrespective of stemming from light or dark noise, induces a transient modulation of the gain. In order to account for a possible lag period and for potential different profiles to the rise and fall of the gain, we introduced the following general function to describe how the gain changes in time following an event at t=: 3

93 G( t) = ( t t ( t t lag lag t < tlag ( t t ) α lag ) exp α t < t < t + T (7) lag lag max Tmax ( t t ) β lag ) exp β t > tlag + Tmax Tmax 94 95 96 where t lag is the lag time, T max is the time at which the gain reaches its maximum, and α and β determine the profiles of the rising and declining regions of the curve, respectively (Fig. 3c). 97 98 99 2 2 This time-dependent gain has an additional dependence upon the random number of noise events in the time intervals preceding the first and second experimental intervals. We assumed that all those events average to form a continuous and stationary background gain. Using these simplifications, we calculated numerically the probability of correct response and the relative change in first interval responses. 22 23 24 25 26 In order to calculate the probability of correct response or the fraction of first interval responses, we simulated 4 5 trials and averaged outcomes across them. In each trial, random numbers were drawn to determine whether a photon was present before or during the trial. This was done sequentially in time: the presence of a photon before the trial affects the gain of both intervals. 27 28 For a given gain, the average signal due to photon and photon-like noise events at is given by: 29 E = G( t) ( P + d) (8) 2 and 4

2 E = G( t) d, otherwise. (9) 22 23 24 where G (t) is the time-dependent element of the gain of the system, d is the average number of photon-like noise events during an interval, and P is the average number of light-induced photon events. 25 26 27 28 29 We assumed that the amplified signals from the two intervals are compared and that the one, probabilistically, containing the higher signal is chosen. In the case of no events being detected in either interval or of the same number of events being detected in both intervals, we assumed the subject to choose randomly between the first and second intervals. 22 22 222 223 224 225 226 We aimed to find a joint parameter set capable of simultaneously fitting the data in both Fig. 2c and Fig. 2d. In order to achieve this we used an optimization strategy based on Bayesian inference. 9% confidence intervals calculated from posterior distributions are presented in the Supplementary Table 3, with parameters 5 defining the shape of the gain function. Noise events were inferred to be in the range..6 mean photon-like noise events per integration interval per spatial area. This number is in agreement with known estimates for the number of dark isomerizations in rod cells 2. 227 228 229 23 23 232 Parameter 7 might be interpreted as the efficiency of the system, yielding the probability of the retinal detection of an event when a single photon impinges upon the cornea. Previous psychophysics measurements place the quantum efficiency of an eye between.3 and.23, while direct estimates based on losses within the eye range from. to.4 (Supplementary Table ). Interestingly, our estimate based on the psychophysics data that takes into account the effect of temporal gain enhancement is 5

233 234 235 236 more consistent with the one based on transmission measurements. It is therefore plausible that under the lowest light conditions used in the study, the efficiency of vision reaches its potential maximum set by the physical properties of the ocular medium. 237 238 239 24 24 242 In addition, we asked whether the presence of two decision criteria applied at the level of amplified signals could account for the observed higher performance for high confidence events (Fig. 2a). We introduced criteria C2 and C3, which separate the possible difference between signals equation (8) and equation (9) into 3 ranges (Fig. 3a,b). When the difference was smaller than C2 the event was counted as a rating event, between C2 and C3 as a rating 2 event and higher than C3 as a rating 3 event. 243 244 The probability of correct response and fraction of all responses were calculated as following: 245 t= P ( C) = p ( t) T ( t) dt () Ri Ri 246 F( R ) = i t= f Ri ( t) T ( t) dt () 247 248 249 25 25 Where P Ri (C) is the probability of correct response for rating R i events, and F R ) is fraction of R i events. Here p Ri (t) is the probability of giving the correct response for R i events as a function of time to the previous single-photon event, R (t) is the fraction of rating R i events as a function of time to the previous single-photon event, T(t) is the distribution of time to the previous single-photon events extracted from the data. f i ( i 6

252 253 254 255 256 257 In order to find coefficients C2 and C3 we kept the best parameter fit identified for the combined probability of correct response and relative change in interval answers temporal functions (Fig. 3a,b) and varied C2 and C3 until we found the best combined fit for the probability of correct response as a function of delay for rating 3 events (Fig. 2c inset), total probability of correct response (Fig. 3d) and the fraction of all responses assigned to each rating in the SPDC experiments (Fig. 3f). 258 Effect of feedback received on the performance of subjects 259 26 26 262 Our model suggests that temporal enhancement of the gain occurs on a time scale of several seconds following a photon detection. This makes it unlikely that this effect occurs at the level of rod cells, whose response is very fast 3. We speculated that this might be a processing mechanism in the retina (see main text). 263 264 265 266 267 In order to exclude the possibility that this temporal enhancement comes from higher brain cognitive effects, we plotted the probability of the correct response as a function of time to the previous photon and feedback received in the previous interval. Supplementary Fig. 6a and 6b show that enhancement is not correlated with the feedback received in the previous trial. 268 269 27 27 272 273 Next, we used trials in the SPDC experiments in which by random chance no photons were presented to the subject but nonetheless the feedback was received based on the interval in which the photon would have been. We found that the distribution of ratings and the probability of correct response on trials following such events do not depend on the type of feedback received by the subject (Supplementary Fig. 6c,d), suggesting that the observed effect was not due to increased attention on the part of the subject. 7

274 275 Supplementary References 276 277 278 279 28 28 Green, D. M. & Swets, J. A. Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics. reprint edn, (Peninsula Publishers, 989). 2 Baylor, D. A., Nunn, B. J. & Schnapf, J. L. The photocurrent, noise and spectral sensitivity of rods of the monkey Macaca fascicularis. J Physiol 357, 575-67 (984). 3 Field, G. D., Sampath, A. P. & Rieke, F. Retinal processing near absolute threshold: from behavior to mechanism. Annu Rev Physiol 67, 49-54 (25). 282 8